You are on page 1of 10

Mindfulness

DOI 10.1007/s12671-015-0427-2

ORIGINAL PAPER

The Relationship Between Mindfulness


and Forgiveness of Infidelity
Keri N. Johns 1 & Elizabeth S. Allen 2 & Kristina Coop Gordon 3

# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Abstract Forgiveness has been associated with multiple ben- associated with forgiveness or nonforgiveness as hypothe-
efits for individuals as well as for relationships. Mindfulness sized or were accounted for by the control variables. Future
may facilitate an individual’s forgiveness of interpersonal be- researchers could consider whether integrating mindfulness
trayal by enhancing emotional recovery and perspective tak- into intervention could be of benefit to individuals who have
ing and reducing overidentification with anger. The current experienced partner infidelity.
study evaluated whether higher levels of self-reported mind-
fulness were associated with forgiveness or nonforgiveness of Keywords Forgiveness . Mindfulness . Relationships .
past partner infidelity. Ninety-four participants (49 % male, Infidelity
51 % Caucasian, 30 % Hispanic, and 16 % African
American) with a history of a partner affair anonymously
completed an online survey of their own levels of current Introduction
mindfulness and forgiveness or nonforgiveness regarding an
affair. Correlations between facets of mindfulness and In general, Americans expect and place high value on fidelity
forgiveness/nonforgiveness were evaluated. To examine sig- in their intimate relationships (e.g., Johnson et al. 2002; Treas
nificant associations, separate regressions were run, first con- and Giesen 2000). However, infidelity occurs in many serious
trolling for affair variables (e.g., perceived severity of the in- intimate relationships and marriages (e.g., Davis et al. 2005;
fidelity, remorse of the partner) and secondly controlling for Mark et al. 2011; Whisman and Snyder 2007). Discovering
general empathy, perspective taking, and anger. In controlled that a partner has been unfaithful can be traumatic, with in-
analyses, lower levels of the mindfulness facets of acting with tense feelings of rage, shame, depression, and abandonment
awareness and being nonjudgmental of inner experience were (e.g., Glass 2002; Gordon et al. 2008). Forgiveness often is
related to higher levels of current nonforgiveness of the part- considered essential in the process of recovery from an inti-
ner. Additionally, the mindfulness skill of being nonreactive mate betrayal, such as infidelity (e.g., Gordon and Baucom
was positively related to higher levels of current forgiveness. 2003; Paleari et al. 2005). Although the definition of forgive-
However, other aspects of mindfulness either were not ness is often debated, forgiveness has been conceptualized as
a reduction of chronic negative affect toward the transgressing
partner, a more balanced view of the partner and relationship,
and relinquishing the desire for revenge (e.g., Fincham et al.
2006; Gordon et al. 2009; Worthington 2005). Forgiveness
* Keri N. Johns
knjohns@gmail.com
can occur whether or not the intimate relationship is
maintained.
More recently, forgiveness researchers have found it useful
1
Department of Psychology, University of South Alabama, to distinguish different dimensions of forgiveness, both
Mobile, AL 36688, USA
positive forgiveness (i.e., the presence of forgiveness) and
2
University of Colorado Denver, Denver, CO, USA negative forgiveness (i.e., the presence of nonforgiveness or
3
University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, USA unforgiveness) dimensions (e.g., Fincham et al. 2006; Gordon
Mindfulness

et al. 2009; Worthington 2005). The positive dimension of positive interpersonal interactions (Dekeyser et al. 2008),
forgiveness is conceptualized by a balanced view of the part- greater abilities in identifying and communicating emotions
ner and event without chronic intense anger and resentment (Wachs and Cordova 2007), higher relationship satisfaction,
toward the partner. In contrast, the negative dimension of for- and less relationship conflict (Barnes et al. 2007; Jones et al.
giveness is conceptualized as intense negative affect toward 2011), all of which also might have implications for fostering
the partner, a desire to punish the partner, withdrawal from the forgiveness. Overall, mindfulness is associated with specific
partner, and emotional turmoil and dysregulation. intrapersonal and interpersonal processes (e.g., less anger)
Forgiveness, defined in this way, is generally considered to which may facilitate increased forgiveness. In fact, a positive
be healthy, as continually harboring strong and pervasive neg- relationship between mindfulness and trait forgiveness—peo-
ative emotions—such as anger and desire for retaliation—are ple who are generally forgiving across circumstances—has
harmful to the individual and the relationship (e.g., Gordon been established in a few empirical studies (e.g., Oman et al.
and Baucom 1998; Paleari et al. 2005; Worthington et al. 2008; Webb et al. 2012). Unpublished research by Klevnick
2007). For the purposes of this study, we refer to these dimen- (2008) showed significant effects of a Mindfulness-Based
sions of positive and negative forgiveness as Forgiveness and Stress Reduction (MBSR) intervention on self- and other-
Nonforgiveness, respectively. forgiveness as compared to a waitlist control group.
Several factors, including partner behavior, the nature of However, partner-specific forgiveness was not assessed.
the betrayal, and individual temperament variables, may im- With regard to the potential role of mindfulness in the for-
pact an individual’s willingness to forgive. For example, an giveness of partner infidelity in particular, the utilization of
individual’s forgiveness can be facilitated by partner apology mindfulness skills—awareness and acceptance of aversive
or remorse (Carmody and Gordon 2011; Gunderson and thoughts and emotions and lower experiential avoidance—
Ferrari 2008), by perception of the transgression as relatively may lead an individual to achieve increased levels of forgive-
less severe (Gordon and Baucom 1999; Gunderson and ness and decrease his or her feelings of nonforgiveness.
Ferrari 2008), and by increased personal empathy and per- Mindfulness is believed to increase emotional recovery from
spective taking (e.g., Paleari et al. 2005; McCullough et al. negative events by helping individuals be both less avoidant
1997). However, one potentially important construct that has of negative thoughts and feelings as well as less identified
not yet been examined when looking at forgiveness of infidel- with these thoughts and feelings (Davidson 2010; Erisman
ity is mindfulness. Mindfulness is an acute awareness of one’s and Roemer 2010). Therefore, a mindful individual might
experience in the present moment with the stance of accep- experience the emotions and mood states associated with a
tance and nonjudgment (Kabat-Zinn 1990). Essentially, a negative event, such as infidelity, in a more observant,
mindful person is able to acknowledge and attend to his or equanimous, and objective perspective, as well as in a more
her current emotional or physical experience, and also remain self-compassionate and less avoidant manner. Along these
unattached and accept the experience nonjudgmentally with lines, MBSR programs designed to increase mindfulness have
the knowledge that it is transient. Trait mindfulness—or the been shown to promote self-compassion, as well as perspec-
general disposition toward being mindful in daily life—has tive taking (Birnie et al. 2010). Being more compassionate
been operationalized as having five facets discovered via ex- toward oneself when experiencing an adverse intimate rela-
ploratory factor analysis of existent mindfulness measures tionship event, such as divorce, is related to lower psycholog-
resulting in the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire ical distress associated with that event (Sbarra et al. 2012).
(FFMQ; Baer et al. 2006). The FFMQ includes (a) observa- Being nonjudgmental of one’s internal experience following
tion of sensations, thoughts, feelings, and perceptions the impact of an interpersonal betrayal might allow individ-
(observing), (b) skill in describing this inner experience with uals to feel more accepted and compassionate toward them-
words (describing), (c) the ability to act with awareness and selves and their experiences, and consequently increase their
concentration rather than performing daily activities thought- ability to cope more effectively and not become entrenched in
lessly or being on autopilot (acting with awareness), (d) being their emotional turmoil. Mindfulness is also conceptualized as
nonjudgmental of one’s experience (nonjudging), and (e) be- independent of the behavior of others, which may help in the
ing nonreactive to one’s experience (nonreacting). forgiveness process. Perhaps mindfulness, which can be cul-
An overview of existing research on mindfulness and in- tivated regardless of partner contrition, facilitates forgiveness
terpersonal relationships suggests that mindfulness skills may regardless of other circumstances.
help facilitate forgiveness in individuals that have experienced The notion of mindfulness facilitating intimate relationship
an interpersonal betrayal. For example, increased mindfulness forgiveness has been articulated well by Menahem and Love
has been associated with regulating anger and increasing em- (2013). These authors theorized that meditation, which is one
pathic concern and perspective taking (Block-Lerner et al. of the most common practices to develop deeper levels of
2007; Dekeyser et al. 2008; Wachs and Cordova 2007). mindfulness, in conjunction with cognitive techniques, engen-
Moreover, increased mindfulness has been related to more ders mindful awareness, empathy, and compassion (for the
Mindfulness

self and others), and thus reframes a transgression, which in up with Study Response (which means they have indicated
turn reduces negative affect and promotes forgiveness. willingness to be approached for survey research through the
Moreover, a few authors have proposed the use of Buddhist company) were emailed screening/invitation questions from
philosophy, mindfulness (Warren et al. 2008), and loving- study response asking if they were over 18, had a partner who
kindness meditation (Cunningham and Cardoso 2012) as in- had an affair while in a serious relationship with them, and
terventions for relationship recovery from infidelity. In these were willing to participate in the anonymous survey on this
articles, the proposed pathways between mindfulness and for- topic. The term Baffair^ was chosen to better elicit behavior
giveness include increased empathy, compassion, and nonat- which would have been considered a betrayal, in contrast with
tachment to strong negative emotions. Though theoretical more neutral descriptions of sex with other persons, which
links have been posited between mindfulness and forgiveness may or may not be considered a betrayal. A sample propor-
of infidelity, no known studies have empirically explored the tionate to the US population (i.e., equal selection of men and
relationship between mindfulness and this offense-specific women, as well as representative percentages of race and eth-
forgiveness toward an intimate partner. nicity) of 130 eligible individuals was then selected by study
For the current study, we hypothesized that having greater response and directed via email invitation from study response
levels of each of the mindfulness facets, as delineated by Baer to the online survey managed by the current researchers. Once
et al. (2006), would be associated with lower levels of at the survey, participants were provided with an informed
nonforgiveness toward a specific partner who has been un- consent with comprehension questions; 101 individuals
faithful. Also, higher levels of each of the mindfulness facets consented and progressed to the survey. The survey included
were expected to be related to higher levels of forgiveness. A quality control questions to detect random or inattentive
benefit to operationalizing mindfulness with this five facet responding; participants failing these questions (N=7) were
model (Baer et al. 2006) was the ability to examine these eliminated, resulting in a sample size of 94 participants.
hypotheses for each facet of mindfulness. We examined these Participation was compensated with a $15 Amazon gift card
relationships controlling for potentially relevant features of the distributed by study response to protect anonymity. All data
partner infidelity such as perceived severity of the betrayal and was collected at a single time point.
time since the affair, and temperament factors of general an-
ger, empathy, and perspective taking. Although no gender Measures
differences were expected, analyses were conducted to exam-
ine whether gender moderated associations between mindful- For all scales in this study, items were reverse scoring items as
ness facets and forgiveness dimensions. needed first such that a higher score always denotes higher
levels of the construct. Table 1 displays internal consistency,
means, and standard deviations for scales.
Method
Features of Partner Infidelity We asked participants ques-
Participants tions about their relationship with the partner that had the
affair. In the cases of multiple partners who have had an af-
Approximately 49 % of respondents were male. Participants fair(s), participants chose Bthe one that hurt the most.^
ranged in age from 22 to 69, with a mean age of 42 years old Participants rated the subjective severity of the partner’s infi-
(SD=12.56 years). Fifty-one percent of participants were delity (i.e., Bhow serious you feel it was^) from 1=minor
Caucasian, 30 % were Hispanic, 16 % were African- transgression to 10=extremely serious violation or betrayal
American, 4 % were Native American, and 3 % were Asian. (M=8.17, SD=1.89). Participants were also asked about the
The mean length of the relationship at the time of the partner’s length of the partner’s affair(s), when they discovered the af-
infidelity was 5.77 years (SD=5.17 years). At the time of fair(s), when the infidelity ended, the perceived amount of
study participation, over half (57 %) of participants were no remorse of the partner, and the partner’s effort to repair the
longer in a relationship with the partner that committed the relationship.
infidelity.
Mindfulness The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire
Procedure (FFMQ; Baer et al. 2006) is designed to assess trait mindful-
ness. Item response options ranged from 1=never or very
A community sample was recruited nationally through the rarely true to 5=very often or always true. The facets of mind-
Study Response Project (www.studyresponse.net), which is fulness include: (a) observing, (b) describing, (c) acting with
a service that assists with behavioral, social, and awareness, (d) nonjudging, and (e) nonreacting. The observ-
organizational online survey research. Participants ing subscale is composed of eight items measuring an ability
completed the study anonymously. Individuals who signed to observe or notice one’s thoughts, feelings, and experiences.
Mindfulness

Table 1 Descriptive statistics

Example item Cronbach’s Male M (SD) Female M (SD) Overall M (SD)

FFMQ Observing BI pay attention to how my emotions affect my .88 28.19 (6.22) 28.29 (6.35) 28.24 (5.25)
thoughts and behavior.^
FFMQ Describing BEven when I’m feeling terribly upset, I can find .86 28.04 (5.56) 29.85 (6.87) 28.97 (6.30)
a way to put it into words.^
FFMQ Acting with Awareness BWhen I do things, my mind wanders off and .94 25.82 (7.27) 26.15 (7.46) 25.99 (7.33)
I’m easily distracted.^ (reversed)
FFMQ Nonjudging BI think some of my emotions are bad or inappropriate .92 24.89 (7.05) 25.19 (8.08) 25.04 (7.55)
and I shouldn’t feel them.^ (reversed)
FFMQ Nonreacting BI watch my feelings without getting lost in them.^ .80 23.54a (4.19) 21.63b (5.22) 22.56 (4.82)
FI Forgiveness BI am able to let go of my anger about my .85 24.39 (4.55) 23.48 (5.75) 23.93 (5.19)
partner’s infidelity^
FI Nonforgiveness BI feel overwhelmed by confusing emotions about .77 22.39 (6.99) 21.90 (6.71) 22.14 (6.81)
my partner’s infidelity^
IRI Empathic Concern BI often have tender, concerned feelings for people .88 23.96 (5.16) 27.35 (5.44) 25.69 (5.55)
less fortunate than me.^
IRI Perspective Taking BWhen I'm upset at someone, I usually try to ‘put .81 24.59 (4.54) 25.17 (5.32) 24.88 (4.93)
myself in his/her shoes’ for a while.^
TAS BI am a hotheaded person.^ .94 44.00 (12.47) 41.58 (12.68) 42.77 (12.56)

Means and SDs are of the sum of items on the scales. Different superscripts indicate significant mean differences (p<.01)
FFMQ five facet mindfulness questionnaire, FI forgiveness inventory, IRI interpersonal reactivity index, TAS trait anger scale

The describing subscale is composed of eight items measuring one’s partner. Forgiveness assesses the degree to which the
an ability to describe one’s thoughts, feelings, and experi- individual feels control over emotions regarding the infidelity,
ences. The acting with awareness subscale is composed of is able to let go of anger regarding the infidelity and is ready to
eight items measuring an ability to act with awareness and Bput it behind^ him or her, and is able to see both the negatives
focus, rather than on Bautopilot^ or although distracted. The and positives about the relationship and partner. Given the
nonjudging subscale is composed of eight items measuring an underlying conceptualization of forgiveness as a process,
ability to withhold judgment of one’s thoughts, feelings, and Gordon et al. (2009) calls nonforgiveness Bstage one^ and
experiences. The nonreacting subscale is composed of seven forgiveness Bstage three^ (stage two is an intermediate stage
items measuring an ability to withhold an immediate reaction and is not used in this study due to the unclear representation
to one’s thoughts, feelings, and experiences. Since the seminal of the scale as either forgiveness or nonforgiveness); given that
psychometric study described in the introduction (Baer et al. the current study is cross sectional, we do not use the stage
2006), several studies have supported the FFMQ’s construct language and instead consider these to simply be assessments
validity in different populations and replicated its structure of current levels of forgiveness and nonforgiveness.
with confirmatory factor analyses (e.g., Baer et al. 2008; Confirmatory factor analysis supported the existence of three
Bohlmeijer et al. 2011; Christopher et al. 2012), but there factors, and the scales demonstrate good reliability across two
are also concerns regarding minimal differential item func- samples (Gordon and Baucom 2003; Gordon et al. 2009). The
tioning (Baer et al. 2011; Van Dam et al. 2009). scales have also shown strong convergent validity, correlating
as expected with other measures of forgiveness, marital ad-
Forgiveness and Nonforgiveness Forgiveness and justment, and co-parenting (Gordon and Baucom 2003;
nonforgiveness were assessed with the Forgiveness Gordon et al. 2009); moreover, means on these scales changed
Inventory (FI; Gordon and Baucom 2003). The FI is overtly across time in the expected directions as couples moved
based on the conceptualizations of trauma and forgiveness through an intervention designed to increase forgiveness fol-
relevant to infidelity described in the introduction. Although lowing infidelity (Gordon et al. 2004). In prior research,
the forgiveness is conceptualized as a process, the scales used nonforgiveness (stage one) and forgiveness (stage three) have
in this study generally assess current levels of certain cogni- displayed an inverse relationship (r = −.20; Gordon and
tive and affective experiences, rather than amount of change Baucom 2003).
over time in these constructs. The items on the FI range from
1=almost never to 5=almost always. Nonforgiveness assesses Empathy and Perspective Taking Two subscales from the
emotional dysregulation regarding the partner’s infidelity, Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis 1980) were used:
numbness/withdrawal from the partner, and a desire to punish (a) empathic concern, measuring one’s emotional sensitivity
Mindfulness

and ability to feel other’s misfortune, and (b) perspective tak- nonforgiveness. Though gender was not expected to affect
ing, measuring one’s ability to understand different perspec- the relationship between mindfulness and forgiveness/
tives or sides of an issue. Item responses are from 1=does not nonforgiveness, moderation analyses (Hayes and Matthes
describe me well to 5=describes me very well. 2009) were conducted with the MODPROBE SPSS macro
to examine whether any relationship between mindfulness
Anger The Trait Anger Scale (TAS; Spielberger 1988) was and forgiveness/nonforgiveness could be moderated by gen-
used to measure general anger tendencies. The TAS is a 15- der; moderation analyses were run between all mindfulness
item scale with response options ranging from 1=never or facets and forgiveness dimensions.
very rarely true to 5=very often or always true.

Results
Statistical Analyses
Initial analyses regarding features of partner infidelity re-
Prior to an examination of the hypotheses and research ques- vealed that respondents believed that the partner’s affair(s)
tions, descriptive statistics were computed for features of part- lasted, on average, between 6 months to less than a year.
ner infidelity, and distributions, reliabilities, means, and stan- Participants also reported that on average, they first discov-
dard deviations were evaluated for the scales (see Table 1). ered their partner’s infidelity between 2 to 5 years prior to
Table 2 presents correlation results for the variables of interest taking survey and that it ended 1 to 2 years prior. Seventeen
in order to establish the basic predicted relationships. For sig- individuals said that the partner’s affair was ongoing; of these
nificant correlations between FFMQ facets and forgiveness/ individuals, 11 reported that they were no longer together with
nonforgiveness scales, hierarchical regression analyses were that partner, one stated that they were still together, and five
then conducted to test the association controlling for affair declined to answer. Finally, participants rated their perceived
variables: severity of the affair, length of the affair, time since amount of effort the partner put forth to repair the relationship
the discovery, time since affair ended, how sorry the partner (on a scale from 1=no effort to 5=a lot of effort, M=2.97,
was, and how much the partner tried to repair the relationship SD=1.46) and to show that they were sorry (M=2.93, SD=
(all of which were related to a forgiveness dimension). A 1.37). About half (49 %) of the participants reported that nei-
second hierarchical regression examined the same significant ther themselves nor their partner had therapy because of the
relationships between the mindfulness facets and forgiveness affair. Thirty-seven percent of participants reported having an
dimensions controlling for temperament variables: empathy, affair of their own (half of these participants had an affair
perspective taking, and anger. We ran these two regression before their partner’s affair and the other half of participants
analyses (one with affair variables and another one with tem- had an affair after their partner’s affair). Additionally, there
perament variables) separately for conceptual clarity to see were no significant differences in levels of nonforgiveness
how these two types of variables separately affected the rela- and forgiveness between individuals who discovered their
tionships between min dfulness and forgivene ss/ partner’s infidelity over a year prior to the survey compared

Table 2 Correlations

1. Nonforgiveness 2. forgiveness 3. IRI empathic concern 4. IRI perspective taking 5. TAS

1. Nonforgiveness –
2. Forgiveness −.08 –
3. IRI empathic concern −.21* −.30** –
4. IRI perspective taking −.11 .20† .64*** –
5. TAS .53*** .17 −.47*** −.44*** –
6. FFMQ observing .21* (.08/08) .17 .11 .36** .15
7. FFMQ describing −.26* (−.14/−.19*) .00 .25* .29** −.22*
8. FFMQ acting with awareness −.63*** (−.52***/−.50***) .00 .31** .27** −.66***
9. FFMQ nonjudging −.60*** (−.52***/−.45***) .02 .28** .28** −.63***
10. FFMQ nonreacting .07 .40** (.27**/.35**) −.04 .27** .00

The first entry in each cell is a bivariate correlation. Values in parentheses denote standardized regression coefficients (β) controlling for (a) variables
related to the affair (b) temperament variables of empathic concern, perspective taking, and anger
IRI interpersonal reactivity index, TAS trait anger scale, FFMQ five facet mindfulness questionnaire
*p<.05; **p<.01; ***p<.001; † p<.10
Mindfulness

to individuals who discovered the infidelity less than a year revealed a trend for acting with awareness and nonjudging
prior to the survey (nonforgiveness t(88)=.99, p=.32; forgive- with nonforgiveness (acting with awareness β=.29, p=.05;
ness t(88)=.38, p=.70). nonjudging β=.25, p<.10). Whereas both males and fe-
Bivariate correlation analyses indicated that neither IRI males had a significant negative relationship between act-
empathic concern nor TAS was significantly related to for- ing with awareness and nonjudging with nonforgiveness,
giveness (see Table 2). IRI perspective taking trended toward the relationship was stronger for men (acting with aware-
a positive association with forgiveness, but was unrelated to ness β = −.73, p < .001; nonjudging β = −.69, p < .001).
nonforgiveness. As expected, the TAS (trait anger) was in- Gender was a significant moderator for the relationship
versely related and IRI empathic concern was positively relat- between the observing facet and nonforgiveness (β=−.47,
ed to the FFMQ describing, acting with awareness, and p<.05). Specifically for women, the observing facet and
nonjudging subscales. However, these measures were unrelat- nonforgiveness were not significantly related (β = .02,
ed to observing and nonreacting. IRI perspective taking was p=.90). In contrast, for men, the observing facet was sig-
positively correlated with all five of the mindfulness sub- nificantly related to greater nonforgiveness (β = .49,
scales. Nonforgiveness and forgiveness were statistically un- p<.01). However, when controlling for the infidelity or
related. Describing, acting with awareness, and nonjudging temperament variables, there were no significant gender
converged with one another (r’s ranged from .42 to .81), while moderation effects across all mindfulness facets.
the observing and nonreacting subscales had inconsistent re- Similarly, in separate analyses of the bivariate relationship
lationships (r’s ranged from−.21 to .43) with the other scales. between observing and nonforgiveness for men, control-
Table 2 also presents the basic relationships between ling for the infidelity and/or temperament variables eliminat-
forgiveness dimensions with the mindfulness facets. ed any relationship between observing and nonforgiveness.
Nonforgiveness was negatively related to the describing, act-
ing with awareness, and nonjudging facets of mindfulness,
such that higher levels of these aspects of mindfulness related Discussion
to lower levels of nonforgiveness. Nonforgiveness was posi-
tively related to the observing facet of mindfulness and This sample, recruited based on whether they endorsed that a
showed no relation to the nonreacting scale. Forgiveness partner had an affair, confirmed that the affair was generally
was unrelated to the observing, describing, acting with aware- viewed as a betrayal and also showed variability regarding
ness, and nonjudging facets of mindfulness. The only aspect levels of nonforgiveness and forgiveness regarding the event.
of mindfulness that was significantly positively related to for- Although the results regarding the degree to which facets of
giveness was nonreacting. self-reported mindfulness predicted forgiveness or
Within Table 2, the first parenthetical values beneath the nonforgiveness were complex and did not consistently sup-
correlations reflect the standardized regression coefficient (β) port the hypotheses, the relationships between some facets of
between the mindfulness facet and forgiveness dimension mindfulness and dimensions of forgiveness showed support in
controlling for these features of the affair. Whereas these con- the expected directions, particularly as hypothesized for
trols did attenuate the relationships between forgiveness di- nonforgiveness. Specifically, higher levels of the mindfulness
mensions and facets of mindfulness, significant relationships facets of describing, acting with awareness, and nonjudging
remained for acting with awareness and nonjudging with related to lower levels of nonforgiveness, whereas higher
nonforgiveness, and for the relation of nonreacting with for- levels of nonreacting predicted greater forgiveness. These re-
giveness. Describing and observing were no longer significant lationships held even when controlling for empathy, perspec-
predictors of nonforgiveness with these controls. tive taking, and anger, and were not significantly moderated
Within Table 2, the second parenthetical values beneath the by gender. These relationships also generally held when con-
correlations reflect the standardized regression coefficient (β) trolling for affair variables such as the severity of the affair and
between the mindfulness facet and the forgiveness dimension the perceived regret of the partner, with the exception
controlling for these temperament variables. These controls that describing was no longer a significant predictor of
operated in a virtually identical manner as the affair variable nonforgiveness with these controls in place. Consistent with
controls, such that significant relationships held for acting the general hypothesis of higher levels of mindfulness relating
with awareness, nonjudging, and nonreacting, whereas the to lower levels of nonforgiveness (characterized by negative
observing facet of mindfulness was no longer a significant emotion and instability), the higher the scores in the ability to
predictor of nonforgiveness once these controls were in place. describe one’s internal experiences, to act with awareness and
However, with these controls, describing remained a signifi- focus, and withhold judgment about one’s internal experi-
cant predictor of nonforgiveness. ences, the less the level of nonforgiveness. Acting with aware-
Mean gender differences were only seen in the ness and nonjudging are the two facets of the FFMQ that are
nonreacting facet (Table 1). Gender moderation analyses most inversely related to the constructs of experiential
Mindfulness

avoidance and thought suppression (Baer et al. 2006). They negative emotion. Additionally, the nonreacting facet has been
are both also positively related to self-compassion (Baer et al. shown to be most related to self-compassion out of the FFMQ
2006), which has been shown to aid recovery from the inter- subscales (Baer et al. 2006), which again has been noted as
personal trauma of divorce (Sbarra et al. 2012). Self- important in recovery from divorce (Sbarra et al. 2012). Self-
compassion involves being nonjudgmental toward the self compassion requires this nonreactivity to painful and negative
and painful experiences; it is inversely related to self-criticism, emotions and experiences; if an individual is avoidant or
rumination, and thought suppression (Neff et al. 2007). entrenched (both forms of reaction) in this negative emotion-
Therefore, having a present awareness and being nonjudg- ality, there is little room to become compassionate toward the
mental of negative experiences may lend to self-compassion human experience of painful feelings (Neff et al. 2007). The
and in turn less rumination and entrenchment with the resent- relationship between these constructs warrants exploration in
ment and emotional lability. Individuals higher in these skills future studies.
may be willing to accept the turmoil and discomfort they are It is notable that, whereas aspects of both mindfulness and
feeling without overidentification with these states and feel forgiveness were often related to empathy, perspective taking,
compassion for themselves going through this experience. and anger, controlling for these did not eliminate most of the
Initial gender moderation analyses indicated that these mind- significant associations between mindfulness and levels of
fulness skills may have a particularly strong link with forgiveness. Although these results indicate that mindfulness
nonforgiveness for males; however, this gender moderation is often related to greater empathic concern and perspective
was just a trend that was eliminated when control variables taking and lower general anger (and thus relevant to the for-
were included and thus was not a robust finding. giveness process; e.g., Paleari et al. 2005), aspects of mind-
Other mindfulness-forgiveness associations were not as fulness were generally related to dimensions of forgiveness in
predicted. Specifically, the observing subscale was positively a unique way apart from these aspects of trait emotionality.
related to nonforgiveness. It may be that the experiences in- Thus, some aspects of being mindful may increase one’s ten-
herent in nonforgiveness—high affect and cognitive process- dency to forgive (or not forgive) through other pathways than
ing—lead individuals to engage in higher levels of observing simply being able to regulate one’s emotions or have empathy
behaviors (e.g., attending to immediate sensory stimuli), per- and perspective.
haps in an effort to distract from thoughts and feelings in order
to calm oneself. In fact, in a sample that was not chosen for
mindfulness skill or practice, it may be that Bobserving^ is Limitations and Future Directions
more akin to ruminating (see below). However, this relation-
ship held for men only, not women, and when controlling for These findings should be interpreted with caution given the
variables such as time since the affair and level of anger, this issues inherent in the cross-sectional, self-report methods
relationship was eliminated. Given the inconsistent and used. Specifically, it is important to note that this sample
unpredicted relationships between observing and was recruited generally for the experience of partner infidelity,
nonforgiveness, further exploration in future studies is and participants’ levels of mindfulness skill or practices, such
warranted. as various forms of meditation, are unknown. There are con-
Also contrary to predictions, most aspects of mindfulness cerns within the mindfulness literature on the ability to mea-
were simply not related to higher levels of forgiveness. The sure mindfulness via self-report. One concern is that the con-
only exception was a positive association of the nonreacting struct of mindfulness as measured by the FFMQ is as a stable
scale with forgiveness, which held even after controlling for trait, in contrast with Buddhist conceptualizations of mindful-
affair variables and temperament variables such as perspective ness as a process or practice (e.g., Grossman and Van Dam
taking. Thus, the ability to Bstep back^ from inner experience 2011). Moreover, those trained in mindfulness may have a
and not immediately react may facilitate a sense of being in better meta-cognitive awareness of their Btrue^ level of daily
control of the emotions about the partner and affair, as well as mindfulness and answer a self-report mindfulness question-
seeing multiple perspectives of the affair and the partner. naire differently than someone with no mindfulness or medi-
When an individual is nonreactive to distressing emotions or tation training. For example, someone with meditation and
thoughts, they are able to remain separated from the experi- mindfulness experience may have different semantic under-
ence. He or she is not avoidant of the experience but is able to standing of Bpaying attention^ or Bnoticing^, and thus, the
see the emotion or thought for the experience that it is without FFMQ’s items may be measuring something other than mind-
attaching to it. The individual understands that it is transient fulness in meditation naive samples (Grossman 2008).
and distinct from the self (Kabat-Zinn 1990). Thus, this skill Consistent with this concern, there is evidence that the
may allow an individual to have a more balanced perspective FFMQ, particularly the observe facet, may function differen-
even if they experience the negative emotionality associated tially in meditators, meditation naive, and distressed individ-
with betrayal because they do not overidentify with the uals. Observing may in fact be maladaptive in distressed or
Mindfulness

meditation naive samples, perhaps being more akin to rumi- increased self-compassion could be a possible result of mind-
nating on internal distress and conflict (Baer et al. 2008; fulness practice which may in turn increase the capacity to
Bohlmeijer et al. 2011). Thus, in future studies, understanding forgive in the face of betrayal; self-compassion as well as a
the participants’ experiences with meditation and mindfulness general change in perspective of the self, or an acceptance of a
training may help in deciphering self-reported levels of transient sense of self (Hölzel et al. 2011), could also be po-
mindfulness. tentially interesting mechanisms to examine. While exact
Also, because this study is cross-sectional in nature, we are mechanisms may yet be unknown, these results add to the
limited in our assumptions from this study of how mindful- literature that cultivating certain mindfulness skills can sup-
ness may facilitate the process of forgiveness, or even if there port resilience in the face of difficult events, including infidel-
was a Bprocess^ of forgiveness. We can only determine the ity. Given these findings, future studies should continue to
basic association between mindfulness and forgiveness/ explore this relationship further and the effects of mindfulness
nonforgiveness at the current time, controlling for the time on the process of forgiveness utilizing multiple time point,
since discovery. Interestingly, there were no significant differ- dyadic level, and observational data. An intervention study
ences in forgiveness versus nonforgiveness between individ- in which true effects of mindfulness practice and underlying
uals that discovered their partner’s infidelity over a year prior mechanisms could be explicitly measured would be the ideal
to the survey versus less than a year. However, we still cannot next step.
track the forgiveness process and how it might be affected by
mindfulness. Cross-sectional correlations also have interpre-
Acknowledgments Funding for this study was partially provided by the
tive limitations in terms of directionality. For example, per- Dean’s Fund for Excellence award from the University of Colorado
haps it is only when individuals are higher in forgiveness that Denver.
they are then more likely to be able to experience their
thoughts and feelings without reacting to them. The fact that
the study was cross-sectional also opens up possible retrospec- References
tive bias on the part of participants for issues they were asked
to recall, such as the severity of the affair. Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L.
There may also be unknown selection factors related to this (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of
particular sample. Although internet surveys in general appear m i n d f u l n e s s . A s s e s s m e n t , 1 3 , 2 7 – 4 5 . d o i : 1 0 . 11 7 7 /
1073191105283504.
to yield consistent results to other survey methods (Gosling
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer,
et al. 2004), there may be generalizability issues relevant to S., Walsh, E., Duggan, D., & Williams, J. M. G. (2008). Construct
the particular sample recruited for this study, a group willing validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and
to share personal experiences with researchers. Moreover, this nonmeditating samples. Assessment, 15(3), 329–342. doi:10.1177/
1073191107313003.
sample lacked adequate power and representativeness to eval-
Baer, R. A., Samuel, D. B., & Lykins, E. L. (2011). Differential item
uate factors such as race/ethnicity or personal affair history as functioning on the five facet mindfulness questionnaire is minimal
potential differentiating or moderating factors. in demographically matched meditators and nonmeditators.
As noted by others (Birnie et al. 2010; Davidson 2010; Assessment, 18(1), 3–10. doi:10.1177/1073191110392498.
Sbarra et al. 2012), further research is needed to elucidate Barnes, S., Brown, K. W., Krusemark, E., Campbell, W. K., & Rogge, R.
D. (2007). The role of mindfulness in romantic relationship satisfac-
the precise mechanisms of the links between constructs such
tion and responses to relationship stress. Journal of Marital and
as mindfulness and recovery from difficult events such as Family Therapy, 33(4), 482–500. doi:10.1111/j.1752-06062007.
infidelity. Apart from an unpublished dissertation study 00033.x.
(Klevnick 2008), this is the first study to attempt to explore Birnie, K., Speca, M., & Carlson, L. E. (2010). Exploring self-
this construct’s association with forgiveness of infidelity. The compassion and empathy in the context of mindfulness-based stress
reduction (MBSR). Stress and Health, 26, 359–371. doi:10.1002/
associations shown in the current study and the success of smi.1305.
mindfulness-based intervention methods for individuals and Block-Lerner, J., Adair, C., Plumb, J. C., Rhatigan, D. L., & Orsillo, S. M.
couples (e.g., Carson et al. 2004; Peterson et al. 2009) do (2007). The case for mindfulness-based approaches in the cultiva-
together suggest that cultivating some aspects of mindfulness tion of empathy: does nonjudgmental, present-moment awareness
increase capacity for perspective-taking and empathic concern?
practice and principles may be a useful therapeutic adjunct in
Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 33(4), 501–516. doi:10.
the recovery process after partner infidelity. More recently, the 1111/j.1752-0606.2007.00034.x.
mindfulness literature has begun to focus on possible mecha- Bohlmeijer, E., ten Klooster, P. M., Fledderus, M., Veehof, M., & Baer, R.
nisms of mindfulness practices that may impact this proposed (2011). Psychometric properties of the five facet mindfulness ques-
relationship (e.g., Desbordes et al. 2014; Hölzel et al. 2011). tionnaire in depressed adults and development of a short form.
Assessment, 18(3), 308–320. doi:10.1002/jclp.20741.
For example, the role of equanimity, or an evenness of temper Carmody, P., & Gordon, K. C. (2011). Offender variables: unique predic-
and general composure of emotion, should also be specifically tors of benevolence, avoidance, and revenge? Personality and
examined (Desbordes et al. 2014). As we stated earlier, Individual Differences, 50, 1012–1017.
Mindfulness

Carson, J. W., Carson, K. M., Gil, K. M., & Baucom, D. H. (2004). Grossman, P. (2008). On measuring mindfulness in psychosomatic and
Mindfulness-based relationship enhancement. Behavior Therapy, psychological research. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 64(4),
35, 471–494. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(04)80028-5. 405–408. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.02.001.
Christopher, M. S., Neuser, N. J., Michael, P. G., & Baitmangalkar, A. Grossman, P., & Van Dam, N. T. (2011). Mindfulness, by any other
(2012). Exploring the psychometric properties of the five facet name…: trials and tribulations of sati in Western psychology and
mindfulness questionnaire. Mindfulness, 3(2), 124–131. doi:10. science. Contemporary Buddhism, 12(1), 219–239. doi:10.1080/
1007/s12671-011-0086-x. 14639947.2011.564841.
Cunningham, L., & Cardoso, Y. (2012). Loving kindness meditation and Gunderson, P. R., & Ferrari, J. R. (2008). Forgiveness of sexual cheating
couples therapy: healing after an infidelity. Ideas and Research You in romantic relationships: effects of discovery method, frequency of
Can Use: VISTAS, 2012, 1. offense, and presence of apology. North American Journal of
Davidson, R. J. (2010). Empirical explorations of mindfulness: concep- Psychology, 10(1), 1–14.
tual and methodological conundrums. Emotion, 10(1), 8–11. doi:10. Hayes, A. F., & Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedures for probing
037/a0018480. interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS
Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differ- implementations. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 924–936.
ences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Hölzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., &
Psychology, 10, 85–103. Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness meditation work? proposing
Davis, J. A., Smith, T. W., & Marsden, P. V. (2005). General social mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective.
surveys, 1972–2004: cumulative codebook. Chicago: National Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 537–559. doi:10.
Opinion Research Center. 1177/1745691611419671.
Dekeyser, M., Raes, F., Leijssen, S., Leysen, S., & Dewulf, D. (2008). Johnson, C. A., Stanley, S. M., Glenn, N. D., Amato, P. R., Nock, S. L.,
Mindfulness skills and interpersonal behaviour. Personality and Markman, H. J., & Dion, M. R. (2002). Marriage in Oklahoma:
Individual Differences, 44, 1235–1245. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007. 2001 baseline statewide survey on marriage and divorce. Bureau
11.018. for Social research, Oklahoma State University.
Desbordes, G., Gard, T., Hoge, E. A., Hölzel, B. K., Kerr, C., Lazar, S. Jones, K. C., Welton, S. R., Oliver, T. C., & Thoburn, J. W. (2011).
W., … & Vago, D. R. (2014). Moving beyond mindfulness: defining Mindfulness, spousal attachment, and marital satisfaction: a mediat-
equanimity as an outcome measure in meditation and contemplative ed model. The Family Journal, 19(4), 357–361. doi:10.1177/
research. Mindfulness, 1–17. doi: 10.1007/s12671-013-0269-8. 106648071141734.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: using the wisdom of your
Erisman, S. M., & Roemer, L. (2010). A preliminary investigation of the
body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Random
effects of experimentally induced mindfulness on emotional
House.
responding to film clips. Emotion, 10(1), 72–82. doi:10.1037/
Klevnick, L. (2008). An exploration of the relationship between mindful-
a0017162.
ness and forgiveness (Doctoral thesis, University of Toronto,
Fincham, F. D., Hall, J., & Beach, S. R. (2006). Forgiveness in marriage:
Toronto, Canada). Retrieved from http://www.collectionscanada.
current status and future directions. Family Relations, 55(4), 415–
gc.ca/obj/thesescanada/vol2/002/NR39772.PDF.
427. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2005.calif.x-i1.
Mark, K. P., Janssen, E., & Milhausen, R. R. (2011). Infidelity in hetero-
Glass, S. P. (2002). Couple therapy after the trauma of infidelity. sexual couples: demographic, interpersonal, and personality-related
In A. S. Gurman & N. S. Jacobson (Eds.), Clinical handbook predictors of extradyadic sex. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40(5),
of couple therapy (3rd ed., pp. 488–507). New York: 971–982. doi:10.1007/s10508-011-9771-z.
Guilford. McCullough, M. E., Worthington, E. L., & Rachal, K. C. (1997).
Gordon, K. C., & Baucom, D. H. (1998). Understanding betrayals in Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships. Journal of
marriage: a synthesized model of forgiveness. Family Process, Personality and Social Psychology, 73(2), 321–336. doi:10.1037/
37(4), 425–449. doi:10.1111/j.1545-5300.1998.00425.x. 0022-3514.73.2.321.
Gordon, K. C., & Baucom, D. H. (1999). A multitheoretical intervention Menahem, S., & Love, M. (2013). Forgiveness in psychotherapy: the key
for promoting recovery from extramarital affairs. Clinical to healing. Journal of Clinical Psychology: In Session, 69(8), 829–
Psychology: Science and Practice, 6, 382–399. doi:10.1093/clipsy. 835. doi:10.1002/jclp.22018.
6.4.382. Neff, K. D., Kirkpatrick, K. L., & Rude, S. S. (2007). Self-compassion
Gordon, K. C., & Baucom, D. H. (2003). Forgiveness and marriage: and adaptive psychological functioning. Journal of Research in
preliminary support for a measure based on a model of recovery Personality, 41, 139–154. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.03.004.
from a marital betrayal. The American Journal of Family Therapy, Oman, D., Shapiro, S. L., Thoresen, C. E., Plante, T. G., & Flinders, T.
31(3), 179–199. doi:10.1080/01926180301115. (2008). Meditation lowers stress and supports forgiveness among
Gordon, K. C., Baucom, D. H., & Snyder, D. K. (2004). An integrative college students: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of
intervention for promoting recovery from extramarital affairs. American College Health, 56(5), 569–578. doi:10.3200/JACH.56.
Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 30(2), 213–231. doi:10. 5.569-578.
1111/j.1752-0606.2004.tb01235.x. Paleari, F. G., Regalia, C., & Fincham, F. (2005). Marital quality, forgive-
Gordon, K. C., Baucom, D. H., & Snyder, D. K. (2008). Optimal strate- ness, empathy, and rumination: a longitudinal analysis. Personality
gies in couple therapy: treating couples dealing with the trauma of and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(3), 368–378. doi:10.1177/
infidelity. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 38, 151–160. 0146167204271597.
doi:10.1007/s10879-008-9085-1. Peterson, B. D., Eifert, G. H., Feingold, T., & Davidson, S. (2009). Using
Gordon, K. C., Hughes, F. M., Tomcik, N. D., Dixon, L. J., & Litzinger, acceptance and commitment therapy to treat distressed couples: a
S. C. (2009). Widening spheres of impact: the role of forgiveness in case study with two couples. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16,
marital and family functioning. Journal of Family Psychology, 430–442. doi:10.1016/j.cbpra.2008.12.009.
23(1), 1–13. doi:10.1037/a0014354. Sbarra, D. A., Smith, H. L., & Mehl, M. R. (2012). When leaving your ex,
Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastaya, S., & John, O. P. (2004). Should we love yourself: observational ratings of self-compassion predict the
trust web-based studies? a comparative analysis of six preconcep- course of emotional recovery following marital separation.
tions about internet questionnaires. American Psychologist, 59(2), Psychological Science, 23(3), 261–269. doi:10.1177/
93–104. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.93. 0956797611429466.
Mindfulness

Spielberger, C. D. (1988). Manual for the state-trait anger expression Webb, J. R., Phillips, T. D., Bumgarner, D., Conway-Williams, E. (2012).
inventory (STAXI). Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources. Forgiveness, mindfulness, and health. Mindfulness, 1–11. doi:10.
Treas, J., & Giesen, D. (2000). Sexual infidelity among married and 1007/s12671-012-0119-0.
cohabitating Americans. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, Whisman, M. A., & Snyder, D. K. (2007). Sexual infidelity in a
48–60. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00048.x. national survey of American women: differences in preva-
Van Dam, N. T., Earleywine, M., & Danoff-Burg, S. (2009). Differential lence and correlates as a function of method of assessment.
item function across meditators and non-meditators on the five facet Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 147–154. doi:10.1037/
mindfulness questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 0893-3200.21.2.147.
47(5), 516–521. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.005. Worthington, E. L. (2005). Handbook of forgiveness. New York:
Wachs, K., & Cordova, J. V. (2007). Mindful relating: exploring mind- BrunnerR outledge.
fulness and emotion repertoires in intimate relationships. Journal of Worthington, E. L., Witvliet, C. V. O., Pietrini, P., & Miller, A. J.
Marital and Family Therapy, 33(4), 464–481. doi:10.1111/j.1752- (2007). Forgiveness, health, and well-being: a review of ev-
0606.2007.0032.x. idence for emotional versus decisional forgiveness, disposi-
Warren, J. A., Morgan, M. M., Williams, S. L., & Mansfield, T. L. (2008). tional forgivingness, and reduced unforgiveness. Journal of
The poisoned tree: infidelity as opportunity for transformation. The Behavioral Medicine, 30, 291–302. doi:10.1007/s10865-007-
Family Journal, 16(4), 351–358. doi:10.1177/1066480708323084. 9105-8.

You might also like