You are on page 1of 14

302

Chapter 26
Internet Use and
Psychological Well-Being
Chiungjung Huang
National Changhua University of Education, Taiwan

ABSTRACT
The direction of the correlation between Internet use and psychological well-being is debatable. The
displacement hypothesis indicates the correlation is negative, as Internet use for communication replaces
face-to face-interaction. Conversely, the augmentation hypothesis suggests that the correlation is posi-
tive because Internet use for communication complements existing social interaction. While previous
empirical findings about the relationship between Internet use and psychological well-being have been
diverse, two previous meta-analyses and the present meta-analysis about the use of social network-
ing sites and psychological well-being supported neither position, and found no relationship between
Internet use and psychological well-being. Investigation of causal predominance between Internet use
and psychological well-being, increased attention to measurement problems of social networking site
use and older adults, and consideration of effects of indicators and moderators should be addressed in
future research.

INTRODUCTION et al. (1998). Two different perspectives regarding


the effect of Internet use have been proposed by
The association between Internet use and psy- researchers. The displacement hypothesis (Lee,
chological well-being, including self-esteem, 2009; Shklovski, Kraut, & Rainie, 2004) claims
depression, loneliness, and life satisfaction, is that time spent using a given medium replaces
an intriguing topic in computer-mediated com- that spent using other mediums. Online interac-
munications. The association is described as the tion thus detrimentally replaces face-to face-
Internet paradox in the pioneering work of Kraut interaction, and Internet use negatively affects

DOI: 10.4018/978-1-4666-0315-8.ch026

Copyright © 2012, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

psychological well-being. The second position being was associated with individuals that had
is the augmentation hypothesis advanced by the adequate social support; while worse outcomes
same researchers (Lee, 2009; Shklovski, Kraut, were associated with those without such support.
& Rainie, 2004) that using certain mediums can As Kraut et al. (1998) sampled first-time users,
facilitate the use of others. For example, Internet the sample was more likely to include individu-
use for communication enhances genuine social als with low Internet self-efficacy, who in turn
interaction and thus promotes psychological well- were likely to encounter difficulties in using the
being. Although numerous researchers (Cooper, Internet that could trigger depression. LaRose
2003; Huang, 2010; Moody, 2001) have examined et al. (2001) indicated that Internet self-efficacy
this link, they obtained inconsistent findings. mediated the relationship between Internet use
The exponential increase in the number of and psychological well-being. To test this pos-
Facebook users has made the use of social network sibility, they sampled 171 undergraduate students
sites and psychological well-being an increas- and found a low correlation between Internet use
ing popular research focus. A new phenomenon and depression (r = -.02) and mediation effect of
dubbed “Facebook depression” describes indi- Internet self-efficacy was not supported. Wästlund
viduals who display depressive symptoms after et al. (2001) tested the Internet paradox in differ-
heavy use of social networking sites (O’Keeffe ent national settings using 500 Swedish students.
et al., 2011). Such phenomenon has increased Their study reached two main conclusions. First,
awareness of the risks associated with the use of younger individuals tend to use the Internet more
social networking sites. heavily and experience lower psychological well-
being than older people. Second, no connection
exists between Internet use and psychological
OVERVIEW well-being.

Whether the effect of Internet use on psycho- Internet Use and


logical well-being is beneficial or detrimental Psychological Well-Being
is debatable. To address this issue, Kraut et al.
(1998) examined the influence of Internet use Studies examining the relationship between
on psychological well-being for a sample of 169 Internet use and psychological well-being have
first-time Internet users and found that Internet frequently used cross-sectional designs. These
use negatively affected psychological well-being. cross-sectional designs have identified diverse cor-
The Internet paradox refers to the positive effects relations between Internet use and psychological
of heavy Internet use on communication. Since well-being. For example, Cooper (2003) surveyed
the publication of the Internet paradox, further a sample of 26 boys and identified a moderate and
research has been replicated. For example, Kraut positive correlation between time spent online and
et al. (2002) reported that the negative effects of depression measured by a Children’s Depression
Internet use for first-time Internet users described Inventory of.47. Moody (2001) sampled 166
in their original research dissipated during a 3-year undergraduate students and found a negative and
follow-up study. Kraut et al. (2002) also investi- weak correlation (r = -.175) between frequency
gated 406 new computer and television purchasers of Internet use and social loneliness.
via a longitudinal survey between 1998 and 1999, However, such cross-sectional studies are lim-
and reported that participants generally displayed ited because the age effect is confounded with the
positive effects of Internet use on psychological cohort effect and development trajectories were
well-being. However, better psychological well- not examined. Some longitudinal studies thus were

303
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

conducted to address these issues. As mentioned entertainment, information, and commerce pur-
previously, Kraut et al. (1988) used a 3-wave poses were.10,.07, -.02,.28, and.08, respectively.
design to examine the longitudinal relationship Given the diverse empirical findings and theo-
between Internet use and psychological well-being retical perspectives related to the displacement
for a sample of 169 first-time users. Kraut et al. and augmentation hypotheses, Huang (2010)
(1988) found that the correlation between prior meta-analyzed 39 cross-sectional studies and
loneliness and later Internet use was -.09 and that 1 longitudinal study, yielding 43 independent
between prior Internet use and subsequent loneli- samples involving 21,258 participants. The mean
ness was.15. Furthermore, the correlation between correlation between Internet use and psychologi-
prior depression and later Internet use was.07 and cal well-being was r = -.04 under random-effects
that between prior Internet use and later depression assumptions. All moderator effects, including
was.15. Prior Internet use thus appeared to be caus- Internet use indicator, psychological well-being
ally predominant over Internet use. Willoughby indicator, measure of quality of Internet use, and
(2008) used a 2-wave design with 2-year interval participant gender and age, were insignificant.
to examine longitudinal relationship Internet use Owing to the low correlation between Internet
and well-being for a sample of 1,591 Canadian use and psychological well-being, Internet use
students with a mean age of 14.82 years old at was unrelated to psychological well-being.
study inception. The correlation between prior
well-being and later Internet use was.15. However, Internet Use and Psychological
the relationship between prior Internet use and Social Interaction
later well-being was not reported, and thus the
causal predominance between Internet use and Empirical findings indicate that interpersonal
psychological well-being cannot be examined. communication is the main application of the
The relationship between Internet use and Internet (Kraut et al., 1996). Accordingly, the use
psychological well-being has been examined in of the Internet for communication has changed
various cultural and national settings. For ex- social interactions with friends and family. From
ample, Amichai-Hamburger and Ben-Artzi (2003) the perspective of the displacement hypothesis,
examined the relationship between Internet use Internet use replaces real life social interaction
and loneliness for 85 students from Israel and and thus weak ties replace close ties. On the other
found the correlations of loneliness with the use hand, close ties can be enhanced by Internet use
of the Internet for social, information and leisure for communication from the perspective of the
purposes were.15,.09, and -.14, respectively. Fur- augmentation hypothesis. To examine the effect
thermore, Leung (2002) examined the relationship of Internet use on social interaction, Katz and
between ICQ use and loneliness for a sample of Aspden (1997) compared the membership rates
576 college students from Hong Kong and found of religious, leisure, and community organiza-
that the correlation between frequency of ICQ use tions between Internet users and non-users and
and loneliness was -.09 while that between the found no statistically significant differences.
amount of time of ICQ use and loneliness was Furthermore, Internet use increased involvement
-.05. Matanda et al. (2004) surveyed a sample of in existing social ties. On the other hand, Stern
158 adults from Australia and found that the cor- (2009) supported the displacement hypothesis in
relations of loneliness with total Internet use, and a survey of a random sample of 1,315 participants
with the use of the Internet for communication, living in the Western United States that found an
association between increased use of the Internet

304
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

for communication and reduced face-to-face inter- Use of Social Networking Sites
actions. Additionally, Lee (2009) sampled 1,312 and Psychological Well-Being
adolescents and found the displacement hypothesis
was partially supported for adolescent-parent Use of social networking sites has increased rap-
interactions, while the augmentation hypothesis idly since the early 2000s. For example, Facebook
was supported for peer interactions. Furthermore, went public in 2004 and now has over 500 million
online communication benefited individuals with active users (facebook.com, 2011). Kujath (2011)
strong social ties more than those without such ties. indicated that social networking sites have two
Cross-cultural evidence also revealed mixed features that differentiate them from previous
findings regarding the relationship between In- computed-mediate communication mediums: lack
ternet use and social interactions. For example, of anonymity and existing acquaintances compris-
Mesch (2001) examined the relationship between ing the majority of online friends. As existing
Internet use and social relationships for a sample interpersonal relationships can be maintained via
of 927 adolescents from Israel, and found Internet social networking sites, their use may promote user
use did not replace social involvement but frequent psychological well-being. Because of the unique
Internet users tended to feel more socially isolated. features of social networking sites, increased
The augmentation hypothesis was supported by numbers of investigations have examined the re-
Räsänen and Kouvo (2007) who surveyed 2,000 lation between the use of social networking sites
Finnish, 2,054 British, 1,503 French, and 1,207 and psychological well-being, and have obtained
Italian participants and found that Internet use inconsistent results. For example, Mehdizadeh
was positively related to sociability in all four (2010) sampled 100 students and found a moderate
European countries, and that the magnitude of and negative correlation between time spent on
the correlation depended on sociodemographic Facebook and self-esteem measured by Rosenberg
variables. Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1986) of -.432.
As the findings of primary research were in- Conversely, Valenzuela, Park, and Kee (2009)
consistent, Shklovsik, Kiesler, and Kraut (2004) surveyed life satisfaction using Satisfaction with
conducted a meta-analysis that identified 8 cross- Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985) and Facebook
sectional and 8 longitudinal studies published use for a sample of 2,603 students and found a
between 1995 to 2003 and yielded 48 correlation positive correlation of.198. Owing to the variable
coefficients involving 35,578 participants. Shk- findings regarding the use of social networking
lovsik et al. (2004) found that the weighted mean sites and psychological well-being, meta-analysis
correlation between Internet use and social interac- was conducted to determine the overall correla-
tion was r = -.02, with a 95% confidence interval tion and identify potential moderating effects of
of -.03 to -.01. The study design (cross-sectional these relationships.
versus longitudinal) did not affect the correlation To identify relevant studies, the PsycINFO and
between Internet use and social interaction. Owing ProQuest Dissertations and Theses databases was
to the low overall correlation between Internet use searched using terms related to social networking
and social interaction, Internet use once again was sites (namely, social media, Facebook, MySpace,
unrelated to social interaction. online social network*, and social network* site*)
and psychological well-being (self-concept, self-
esteem, self-worth, depress*, loneliness, and life
satisfaction) through April, 2011. The included
studies are indicated by asterisks in the refer-
ences. Fifteen studies comprising 17 indepen-

305
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

dent samples involving 11,003 participants were teem or life satisfaction, or low depression and
included. Of these, 3 were doctoral dissertations loneliness. Each r was weighted by sample size
and 12 were journal articles. Fourteen studies used to calculate the mean correlations.
cross-sectional designs and Steinfield, Ellison, and Under fixed effect assumptions, only within-
Lampe (2008) adopted the longitudinal approach. study variability was allowed. Since the assump-
Table 1 lists the country where the research was tion of fixed-effect models was implausible, the
performed, sample size, mean age, type of well- random-effects model that assumed that both
being assessed, measure of well-being, social sampling error and random components explain
networking sites assessed, and correlation between correlation variation was used to calculate the
social networking site use and psychological mean correlation. The effect sizes ranged from -.43
well-being. Seven studies focused on Facebook, to.20. The sample size weighted mean correlation
6 focused on social networking sites in general, was -.02, with a 95% confidence interval of -.10
1 focused on CU2 (a popular social networking to.07. Consistent with findings by Huang (2010),
site in the Netherlands), and 1 focused on instant the relationship between the use of social network-
messaging and social networking sites. Nine ing sites and psychological well-being was weak.
studies were conducted in the United States, 2 Owing to the small correlation between psycho-
each in Australia and Canada, and 1 each in the logical well-being and use of social networking
Netherlands and South Korea. Four samples did sites, it was concluded that social networking
not report participant mean age, while for the sites were not related to psychological well-being.
remaining 13 samples the mean age was 21.12 Moderator analyses were conducted to explain
years old (range 14-46 years). Moreover, the av- the systematic variability in the correlation coef-
erage sample size was 464.06 participants (range ficients between social networking sites use and
35-2,603 participants). psychological well-being. Table 2 lists the number
This study used Pearson product-moment cor- of correlation coefficients, mean correlation, 95%
relation coefficient r as an effect size. For self- confidence interval and tests of between-group
esteem and life satisfaction, a positive correlation variation. The mean correlation for studies con-
coefficient indicated a conducive effect since ducted in the USA was r = -.01 while that for
heavy use of social networking sites was associ- studies conducted outside the USA was r = -.04.
ated with high self-esteem and life satisfaction. The between-study variability represented by QB
When depression and loneliness were used to indicates true heterogeneity in population correla-
measure psychological well-being, a positive tions. Since QB was statistically insignificant, the
correlation coefficient indicates a detrimental correlations between the use of social networking
effect since high use of social networking sites sites and psychological well-being did not differ
was associated with high depression and loneli- between the USA and other countries. Coding
ness. To calculate the mean correlation across multiple effect sizes for various indicators of psy-
positive (self-esteem, life satisfaction) and nega- chological well-being from the same participant
tive (loneliness and depression) indicators of sample yielded 25 effect sizes. Of these 25 data
psychological well-being, the directions of the points, 12 effect sizes focused on self esteem, 7
correlations between depression and use of social on life satisfaction, 5 on loneliness, and 1 on de-
networking sites, and between loneliness and use pression. Since QB was statistically insignificant,
of social networking sites, were reversed. Con- the psychological well-being indicator did not
sequently, a positive correlation indicates a ben- affect the correlation between the use of social
eficial effect, as heavy use of social networking networking sites and psychological well-being.
sites was associated with relatively high self-es- A negative but insignificant correlation existed

306
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

between use of social networking sites and self- strong correlation between prior Internet use and
esteem, r = -.05; between social network sites subsequent psychological well-being as compared
and life satisfaction, r =.05; and between social to the correlation between prior psychological
network sites and loneliness, r = -.07. Caution is well-being and subsequent Internet use would
necessary in interpreting the moderator effects of support this model. The second possible hypoth-
the psychological well-being indicator and study esis claims that psychological well-being drives
location as the number of correlation coefficients Internet use, and thus psychological well-being
was too small. Consequently, findings related determines Internet use and Internet use is not
to the moderator analyses should be considered considered a primary determinant of psychologi-
suggestive rather than definitive. cal well-being. The second position is supported
when the correlation between prior Internet use
and subsequent psychological well being is weaker
FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS than the correlation prior psychological well-being
and later Internet use. The third hypothesis is a
Investigation of the Causal reciprocal-effects model, which holds that prior
Predominance between Internet Internet use affects subsequent psychological
Use and Psychological Well-Being well-being, and prior psychological well-being
impacts Internet use. The reciprocal model is
Most studies examining the relationship between supported given the correlation between prior
Internet use and psychological well-being have Internet use and subsequent psychological well-
used cross-sectional designs. This methodologi- being, and between prior psychological well-being
cal limitation caused failure to establish causal and subsequent Internet use is comparable. The
predominance between Internet use and psy- fourth hypothesis is that Internet use is unrelated
chological well-being. Few investigations have to psychological well-being. This position is sup-
examined the casual ordering of Internet use and ported when the longitudinal correlation between
psychological well-being. Because of the effects Internet use and psychological well-being is low.
of low psychological well-being and prevalence Future investigations should adopt longitudinal
of Internet use for communication, understanding designs to examine the causal predominance of
causal predominance between Internet use and Internet use and psychological well-being.
psychological well-being is helpful when design-
ing intervention programs. If low psychological Attention to Measurement
well-being increases Internet use, enhancing Inconsistency of Social
psychological well-being can decrease Internet Networking Sites Use
use. Conversely, if Internet use adversely affects
psychological well-being, simply improving psy- Inconsistency in measuring the intensity of social
chological well-being and ignoring high Internet networking site use may explain the inconsistent
use will not address the problem. research findings. Some instruments asked respon-
This study proposes four possible models. dents to indicate time spent on social networking
The first hypothesis suggests that Internet use is sites via open-ended questions or Likert scales.
causally predominant over psychological well- Steinfield, Ellison, and Lampe (2008) measured
being. Internet use thus is viewed as impacting the intensity of social networking site use via
psychological well-being, while psychological both amount of time spent on social networking
well-being does not affect Internet use. If longitu- sites and number of online friends. Meanwhile,
dinal studies were used to test this model, a relative Barker (2009) measured both time spent on

307
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

social networking sites and number of visits to ness in testing whether certain psychological well-
such sites. To explain the diverse findings about being indicators are associated with Internet use.
the relationship between social networking sites Internet use may have differential correlations with
use and psychological well-being, future studies different indicators of psychological well-being.
should examine whether differential indicators It is also possible that noticeable differences ex-
of Internet use display different relations with ist in the relationship between Internet use and
psychological well-being. positive indicators of psychological well-being
(namely, self-esteem and life satisfaction) versus
Increased Attention to Older Adults that between Internet use and negative indica-
tors of psychological well-being (loneliness and
Most computer-mediated communication has fo- depression). Future research should examine the
cused on student populations and so more research effect of indicators of psychological well-being
on older adults is needed. For example, 14 of the on the relationship between such well-being and
15 studies included in the present meta-analysis Internet use. The findings may yield insights into
focused on adolescents or college students. Correa how to enhance different indicators of psychologi-
et al. (2010) was an exception and surveyed 959 cal well-being.
adults with a mean age of 46 years old. Chen and
Persson (2000) suggested that life tasks, social Consideration of Moderator Effects
beliefs and values, and cognitive capacities differ on the Relationship between Internet
between young and older adults. Hence, research Use and Psychological Well Being
findings based on younger samples may not be
able to be generalized to older adults. Since user The displacement and augmentation hypotheses
age may moderate the relationship between use describe the bivariate correlation Internet use and
of social networking sites and psychological well- psychological well-being. While the association
being, increased research attention should be paid between Internet use and psychological well-being
to older adults. Such research might illuminate is low, the magnitude of this relationship may vary
whether the relationship between use of social among individuals with different characteristics.
networking sites and psychological well-being That is, moderators may affect the relationship
varies with life stage. between Internet use and psychological well-
being. For example, Kraut et al. (2002) proposed
Investigation of the the rich-get-richer hypothesis that Internet use was
Effect of Indicators of associated with better psychological well-being for
Psychological Well-Being individuals with sufficient social support, while
a negative effect existed for individuals lacking
Psychological well-being has been represented social support. The rich-get-richer hypothesis
using numerous psychological constructs, includ- proposed that social support moderated the rela-
ing self-esteem, loneliness, depression, and life tionship between Internet use and psychological
satisfaction. This inconsistency in the represen- well-being. In contrast, the social compensation
tation of psychological well-being may create hypothesis (Lee, 2009) contended that Internet
difficulties in drawing general conclusions from use exerted a positive effect particularly for so-
the relevant studies and also explain the diversity cially anxious individuals. That is, social anxiety
of the findings. Nevertheless, multiple indicators moderates the relationship between Internet use
of psychological well-being have theoretical and and psychological well-being. Studies of potential
methodological advantages, such as their useful- moderator effects are required to identify diverse

308
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

or conflicting findings about the relationship be- Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Grif-
tween Internet use and psychological well-being. fin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75.
doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
REFERENCES
Facebook.com. (2011). Facebook statistics. Re-
Amichai-Hamburger, Y., & Ben-Artzi, E. (2003). trieved May 3, 2011, from www.facebook.com/
Loneliness and Internet use. Computers in Hu- press/info.php?statistics
man Behavior, 19(1), 71–80. doi:10.1016/S0747- Gentzler, A. L., Oberhauser, A. M., Westerman,
5632(02)00014-6 D., & Nadorff, D. K. (2009). College students’ use
Baker, L. R., & Oswald, D. L. (2010). Shyness of electronic communication with parents: Links
and online social networking services. Journal to loneliness, attachment, and relationship quality.
of Social and Personal Relationships, 27(7), Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Network-
873–889. doi:10.1177/0265407510375261 ing, 14(1-2), 71–74. doi:10.1089/cyber.2009.0409

Baker, R. K., & White, K. M. (2010). Predicting Huang, C. (2010). Internet use and psychological
adolescents’ use of social networking sites from an well-being: A meta-analysis. Cyberpsychology,
extended theory of planned behaviour perspective. Behavior, and Social Networking, 13(3), 241–249.
Computers in Human Behavior, 26(6), 1591–1597. doi:10.1089/cyber.2009.0217
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2010.06.006 Kalpidou, M., Costin, D., & Morris, J. (2010).
Barker, V. (2009). Older adolescents’ motiva- The relationship of Facebook and well-being of
tions for social network site use: The influence of undergraduate college students. Cyberpsychology,
gender, group identity, and collective self-esteem. Behavior, and Social Networking., 14(4), 183–189.
Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 12(2), 209–213. doi:10.1089/cyber.2010.0061
doi:10.1089/cpb.2008.0228 Katz, J., & Aspden, P. (1997). A nation of strang-
Chen, Y., & Persson, A. (2002). Internet use among ers? Communications of the ACM, 40(12), 81–86.
young and older adults: Relation to psychologi- doi:10.1145/265563.265575
cal well-being. Educational Gerontology, 28(9), Katz, J. E., & Rice, R. E. (2002). Syntopia:
731–744. doi:10.1080/03601270290099921 Access, civic involvement, and social interac-
Cooper, N. S. (2003). The identification of psy- tion on the Net. In Wellman, B., & Haythorn-
chological and social correlates of Internet use thwaite, C. (Eds.), The Internet in everyday
in children and teenagers. Unpublished Doctoral life (pp. 114–138). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Dissertation, California School of Professional doi:10.1002/9780470774298.ch3
Psychology. Kraut, R., Kiesler, S., Boneva, B., Cummings, J. N.,
Correa, T., Hinsley, A. W., & de Zúñiga, H. G. Helgeson, V., & Crawford, A. M. (2002). Internet
(2010). Who interacts on the Web? The intersec- paradox revisited. The Journal of Social Issues,
tion of users’ personality and social media use. 58(1), 49–74. doi:10.1111/1540-4560.00248
Computers in Human Behavior, 26(2), 247–253.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2009.09.003

309
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

Kraut, R., Patterson, M., Lundmark, V., Kiesler, Matanda, M., Jenvey, V. B., & Phillips, J. G. (2004).
S., Mukopadhyay, T., & Scherlis, W. (1998). In- Internet use in adulthood: Loneliness, computer
ternet paradox: A social technology that reduces anxiety and education. Behaviour Change, 21(2),
social involvement and psychological well-being? 103–114. doi:10.1375/bech.21.2.103.55424
The American Psychologist, 53(9), 1017–1031.
Mehdizadeh, S. (2010). Self-presentation 2.0:
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.53.9.1017
Narcissism and self-esteem on Facebook. Cy-
Kraut, R., Scherlis, W., Mukhopadhyay, T., Man- berpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking,
ning, J., & Kiesler, S. (1996). The HomeNet 13(4), 357–364. doi:10.1089/cyber.2009.0257
field trial of residential Internet services.
Mesch, G. S. (2001). Social relationships and Inter-
Communications of the ACM, 39(12), 55–63.
net use among adolescents in Israel. Social Science
doi:10.1145/240483.240493
Quarterly, 82(2), 329–339. doi:10.1111/0038-
Kujath, C. L. (2011). Facebook and MySpace: 4941.00026
Complement or substitute for face-to-face inter-
Moody, E. J. (2001). Internet use and its relationship
action? Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social
to loneliness. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 4(3),
Networking., 14(1-2), 75–78. doi:10.1089/cy-
393–402. doi:10.1089/109493101300210303
ber.2009.0311
Nie, N. H., Hillygus, D. S., & Erbring, L. (2002). In-
LaRose, R., Eastin, M. S., & Gregg, J. (2001).
ternet use, interpersonal relations, and sociability.
Reformulating the internet paradox: Social cogni-
In Wellman, B., & Haythornthwaite, C. (Eds.), The
tive explanations of Internet use and depression.
Internet in everyday life (pp. 215–243). Malden,
Journal of Online Behavior, 1(2).
MA: Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9780470774298.ch7
Lee, G., Lee, J., & Kwon, S. (2011). Use of
O’Keeffe, G. S., & Clarke-Pearson, K., & Council
social-networking sites and subjective well-being:
on Communications Media. (2011). The impact of
A study in South Korea. Cyberpsychology, Be-
social media on children, adolescents, and fami-
havior, and Social Networking, 14(3), 151–155.
lies. Pediatrics, 127(4), 800–804. doi:10.1542/
doi:10.1089/cyber.2009.0382
peds.2011-0054
Lee, S. J. (2009). Online communication and
Räsänen, P., & Kouvo, A. (2007). Linked or
adolescent social ties: Who benefits more from
divided by the web? Internet use and sociabil-
Internet use? Journal of Computer-Mediated
ity in four European countries. Information
Communication, 14(3), 509–531. doi:10.1111/
Communication and Society, 10(2), 219–241.
j.1083-6101.2009.01451.x
doi:10.1080/13691180701307461
Leung, L. (2002). Loneliness, self-disclo-
Rosenberg, M. (1986). Conceiving the self. Mala-
sure, and ICQ (“I seek you”) use. Cy-
bar, FL: Robert E. Krieger.
berpsychology & Behavior, 5(3), 241–251.
doi:10.1089/109493102760147240 Schwartz, M. (2010). The usage of Facebook as
it relates to narcissism, self-esteem and loneli-
Lou, L. L. (2009). Loneliness, friendship, and self-
ness. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Pace
esteem: First-year college students’experience of
University.
using Facebook. Unpublished Doctoral Disserta-
tion, State University of New York at Albany.

310
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

Shklovski, I., Kraut, R., & Rainie, L. (2004). Willoughby, T. (2008). A short-term longitudinal
The Internet and social participation: Contrasting study of Internet and computer game use by ado-
cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Journal lescent boys and girls: Prevalence, frequency of
of Computer-Mediated Communication, 10(1). use, and psychosocial predictors. Developmental
Psychology, 44(1), 195–204. doi:10.1037/0012-
Spraggins, A. (2009). Problematic use of online
1649.44.1.195
social networking sites for college students:
Prevalence, predictors, and association with Wilson, K., Fornasier, S., & White, K. M. (2010).
well-being. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Psychological predictors of young adults’ use of
University of Florida. social networking sites. Cyberpsychology, Be-
havior, and Social Networking, 13(2), 173–177.
Steinfield, C., Ellison, N. B., & Lampe, C. (2008).
doi:10.1089/cyber.2009.0094
Social capital, self-esteem, and use of online social
network sites: A longitudinal analysis. Journal
of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29(6),
434–445. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.07.002 ADDITIONAL READING
Stern, M. J. (2008). How locality, frequency of Amichai-Hamburger, Y., & Hayat, Z. (2011).
communication and Internet usage affect modes The impact of the internet on the social lives of
of communication within core social networks. users: A representative sample from 13 countries.
Information Communication and Society, 11(5), Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 585–589.
591–616. doi:10.1080/13691180802126778 doi:10.1016/j.chb.2010.10.009
Valenzuela, S., Park, N., & Kee, K. F. (2009). Is Caplan, S. E., & High, A. C. (2011). Online social
there social capital in a social network site? Face- interaction, psychosocial well-being, and prob-
book use and college students’ life satisfaction, lematic internet use. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley
trust, and participation. Journal of Computer- & Sons Inc.
Mediated Communication, 14(4), 875–901.
doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2009.01474.x Gordon, C. F., Juang, L. P., & Syed, M. (2007).
Internet use and well-being among college
Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., & Schouten, A. P. students: Beyond frequency of use. Journal of
(2006). Friend networking sites and their rela- College Student Development, 48(6), 674–688.
tionship to adolescents’ well-being and social doi:10.1353/csd.2007.0065
self-esteem. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 9(5),
584–590. doi:10.1089/cpb.2006.9.584 Gross, E. F., Juvonen, J., & Gable, S. L. (2002).
Internet use and well-being in adolescence.
Wästlund, E., Norlander, T., & Archer, T. The Journal of Social Issues, 58(1), 75–90.
(2001). Internet blues revisited: Replication doi:10.1111/1540-4560.00249
and extension of an internet paradox study.
Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 4(3), 385–391. Jackson, L. A., von Eye, A., Fitzgerald, H. E., Zhao,
doi:10.1089/109493101300210295 Y., & Witt, E. A. (2010). Self-concept, self-esteem,
gender, race and Information Technology use.
Computers in Human Behavior, 26(3), 323–328.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2009.11.001

311
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

Liu, X., & Larose, R. (2008). Does using the Sum, S., Mathews, M. R., Pourghasem, M., &
internet make people more satisfied with their Hughes, I. (2008). Internet technology and social
lives? The effects of the internet on college capital: How the internet affects seniors’ social
students’ school life satisfaction. Cyberpsychol- capital and well-being. Journal of Computer-
ogy & Behavior, 11(3), 310–320. doi:10.1089/ Mediated Communication, 14(1), 202–220.
cpb.2007.0040 doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2008.01437.x
Morahan-Martin, J., & Schumacher, P. (2003). van den Eijnden, R. J. J. M., Meerkerk, G., Ver-
Loneliness and social uses of the Internet. Com- mulst, A. A., Spijkerman, R., & Engels, R. C. M.
puters in Human Behavior, 19(6), 659–671. E. (2008). Online communication, compulsive
doi:10.1016/S0747-5632(03)00040-2 internet use, and psychosocial well-being among
adolescents: A longitudinal study. Developmental
Schiffrin, H., Edelman, A., Falkenstern, M., &
Psychology, 44(3), 655–665. doi:10.1037/0012-
Stewart, C. (2010). The associations among com-
1649.44.3.655
puter-mediated communication, relationships,
and well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Vergeer, M., & Pelzer, B. (2009). Consequences
Social Networking, 13(3), 299–306. doi:10.1089/ of media and internet use for offline and online
cyber.2009.0173 network capital and well-being. A causal model
approach. Journal of Computer-Mediated Com-
Selfhout, M. H. W., Branje, S. J. T., Delsing, M.,
munication, 15(1), 189–210. doi:10.1111/j.1083-
ter Bogt, T. F. M., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2009).
6101.2009.01499.x
Different types of internet use, depression, and
social anxiety: The role of perceived friendship Ybarra, M. L., Alexander, C., & Mitchell, K. J.
quality. Journal of Adolescence, 32(4), 819–833. (2005). Depressive symptomatology, youth Inter-
doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.10.011 net use, and online interactions: A national survey.
The Journal of Adolescent Health, 36(1), 9–18.
Subrahmanyam, K., & Lin, G. (2007). Adoles-
doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2003.10.012
cents on the Net: Internet use and well-being.
Adolescence, 42(168), 659–677.

312
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

APPENDIX

Table 1. Studies of the Relationships between use of social networking sites and psychological well-being

Study Country Age N Well-being Measure SNSs r


Baker & Oswald USA 19.19 207 loneliness UCLA Loneliness Scale Facebook .04
(2010)
Baker & White Australia 14.36 139 self-esteem Rosenberg Self-Esteem SNSs .17
(2010) Scale
Barker (2009) USA 18 734 self-esteem NA SNSs .085
Correa et al. (2010) USA 46 959 life satisfac- Satisfaction with Life IM & -.09
tion Scale SNSs
Gentzler et al. USA 19.46 211 loneliness UCLA Loneliness Scale SNSs .045a
(2009)
Kalpidou et al. USA 18.31 35 self-esteem Rosenberg Self-Esteem Facebook -.09
(2010) #1 Scale
Kalpidou et al. USA 20.91 35 self-esteem Rosenberg Self-Esteem Facebook -.31
(2010) #1 Scale
Lee et al. (2011) South Korea 21.3 217 life satisfac- Satisfaction with Life SNSs -.00
tion Scale
Lou (2009) USA NA 222 Loneliness, UCLA Loneliness Scale, Facebook -.15, .13b
self-esteem Rosenberg Self-Esteem
Scale
Mehdizadeh (2010) Canada 22.21 100 self-esteem Rosenberg Self-Esteem Facebook -.432
Scale
Schwartz (2010) Canada 21 213 self-esteem, Rosenberg Self-Esteem Facebook -.121, .133b
loneliness Scale, UCLA Loneliness
Scale
Spraggins (2009) USA 20 350 life sat- Satisfaction with Life SNSs -.134, .281,
isfaction, Scale, UCLA Loneliness .281, -.299b
loneliness, Scale, CES-D, Rosenberg
depression, Self-Esteem Scale
self-esteem
Steinfield et al. USA NA 288 self-esteem, Rosenberg Self-Esteem Facebook -.05, .25 a, b
(2008) #1 life satisfac- Scale, Satisfaction with
tion Life Scale
Steinfield et al. USA NA 477 self-esteem, Rosenberg Self-esteem Facebook .01, .03 a, b
(2008) #2 life satisfac- Scale, Satisfaction with
tion Life Scale
Valenzuela et al. USA NA 2603 life satisfac- Satisfaction with Life Facebook .198 a
(2009) tion Scale
Valkenburg et al. Netherlands 14.8 881 self-esteem, Harter’s Self-Perception CU2 .07, .06b
(2006) life satisfac- Profile for Adolescents,
tion Satisfaction with Life
Scale
Wilson et al. Australia 19.07 201 self-esteem Coopersmith Self-Esteem SNSs .02 a
(2010) Inventory
Note The directions of correlations between depression and use of social networking sites and that between loneliness and use of social
networking sites were not reversed in this table. NA = not available.
a Effect sizes are based on correlations obtained from the authors.
b Multiple psychological well-being indicators were used and therefore multiple effect sizes were coded.

313
Internet Use and Psychological Well-Being

Table 2. Moderator analyses

Indicator k Mean 95% CI QB


Upper Lower
Country .16
USA 11 -.01 -.12 .11
Non-USA 6 -.04 -.21 .13

Indicator of PWB 2.25


Self-esteem 12 -.05 -.15 .05
Life satisfaction 7 .05 -.09 .19
Loneliness 5 -.07 -.27 .12
Indicator of PWB = indicator of psychological well-being.
Note. The direction of correlations between depression and use of social networking sites and that between loneliness was reversed in this
table.

314
www.igi-global.com

InfoSci -Books
®

A Database for Progressive Information Science and Technology Research

Maximize Your Library’s Book Collection!


Invest in IGI Global’s InfoSci -Books database and gain access to
®

hundreds of reference books at a fraction of their individual list price.

The InfoSci -Books database offers unlimited simultaneous users the


®

ability to precisely return search results through more than 68,000 full-text
chapters from nearly 3,000 reference books in the following academic research areas:
Business & Management Information Science & Technology • Computer Science & Information Technology
Educational Science & Technology • Engineering Science & Technology • Environmental Science & Technology
Government Science & Technology • Library Information Science & Technology • Media & Communication Science & Technology
Medical, Healthcare & Life Science & Technology • Security & Forensic Science & Technology • Social Sciences & Online Behavior

Peer-Reviewed
Content:
• Cutting-edge research
• No embargoes
• Scholarly and professional
• Interdisciplinary

Award-Winning
Platform:
• Unlimited simultaneous users
• Full-text in XML and PDF
• Advanced search engine
• No DRM

Librarian-Friendly:
• Free MARC records
• Discovery services
• COUNTER4/SUSHI compliant
• Training available

To find out more or request a free trial, visit:


www.igi-global.com/eresources www.igi-global.com

You might also like