You are on page 1of 3

NEUROSCIENCE: The Brain's Dark Energy

Marcus E. Raichle, et al.


Science 314, 1249 (2006);
DOI: 10.1126/science. 1134405

The following resources related to this article are available online at


www.sciencemag.org (this information is current as of November 24, 2006 ):

Updated information and services, including high-resolution figures, can be found in the online
version of this article at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/314/5803/1249

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on November 24, 2006


This article appears in the following subject collections:
Neuroscience
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/neuroscience

Information about obtaining reprints of this article or about obtaining permission to reproduce
this article in whole or in part can be found at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/help/about/permissions.dtl

Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by the
American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. Copyright
c 2005 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title SCIENCE is a
registered trademark of AAAS.
PERSPECTIVES

The study of domains and domain walls larity on a domain wall analogous to a Bloch 2. F. Kidwingira, J. D. Strand, D. J. Van Harlingen, Y. Maeno,
Science 314, 1267 (2006); published online 26 October
has a long tradition in magnetism, and these line in magnetism. The observation of such 2006 (10.1126/science.1133239).
experiments open this field to the study of a vortex with a fractional magnetic flux is 3. J. Xia et al., Phys. Rev. Lett. 97, 167002 (2006).
chiral superconductors. Volovik and Gor’kov a challenge to this emerging field, but it 4. T. M. Rice, M. Sigrist, J. Phys. Condens. Matter 7, L643
(8) were the first to study the theory of these will not be easy in view of the short charac- (1995).
5. A. P. MacKenzie, Y. Maeno, Rev. Mod. Phys. 75, 657
kinds of superconducting domain walls. teristic length scales that Kidwingira et (2005).
They found that the domains should contain al. deduce from their experiments. Con- 6. K. D. Nelson, Z. Q. Mao, Y. Maeno, Y. Liu, Science 306,
counterflowing supercurrents along the wall firmation of these exotic predictions will be 1151 (2004).
7. G. M. Luke et al., Nature 394, 558 (1998).
that generate a perpendicular magnetic a clear test of our understanding of these 8. G. E. Volovik, L. P. Gor’kov, Sov. Phys. JETP 61, 843
dipole (see the figure). All this remains to be intriguing superconductors. (1985).
verified experimentally, as does another 9. M. Sigrist et al., Phys. Rev. Lett. 63, 1727 (1989).
effect predicted by Sigrist et al. (9): A novel References
magnetic vortex should accompany a singu- 1. Y. Maeno et al., Nature 372, 532 (1994). 10.1126/science.1134921

NEUROSCIENCE

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on November 24, 2006


Much of the brain’s enormous energy
consumption is unaccounted for by its
The Brain’s Dark Energy responses to external stimuli. What is this
energy used for, and how do we study it?
Marcus E. Raichle

S
ince the 19th century, and possibly used, it is estimated that 60 to 80% of the tions intrinsically within the cerebral cortex.
longer, two perspectives on brain func- energy budget of the brain supports communi- Take the visual cortex, whose primary func-
tions have existed (1). One view posits cation among neurons and their supporting tion is to respond to external input to the retina.
that the brain is primarily reflexive, driven by cells (2). The additional energy burden associ- Less than 10% of all synapses carry incoming
the momentary demands of the environment; ated with momentary demands of the environ- information from the external world (3)—a
the other, that the brain’s operations are ment may be as little as 0.5 to 1.0% of the total surprisingly small number. From a brain
mainly intrinsic, involving the maintenance energy budget (2). This cost-based analysis energy perspective, however, the cortex may
of information for interpreting, responding implies that intrinsic activity may be far more simply be more involved in intrinsic activities.
to, and even predicting environmental de- significant than evoked activity in terms of What is this intrinsic activity? One possi-
mands. While neither view is dominant, the overall brain function. bility is that it simply represents unconstrained,
former has motivated most neuroscience Consideration of brain energy may thus spontaneous cognition—our daydreams
research. But technological advances, partic- provide new insights into questions that have or, more technically, stimulus-independent
ularly in neuroimaging, have provoked a long puzzled neuroscientists. For example, thoughts. But it is highly unlikely to account
reassessment of these two perspectives. researchers have sought to explain the relative for more than that elicited by responding to
Human functional neuroimaging, first with disproportion of connections (i.e., synapses) controlled stimuli, which accounts for a very
positron emission tomography (PET) and now among neurons that appear to perform func- small fraction of total brain activity.
largely with functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI), allows the brain’s responses to
controlled stimuli to be studied by measuring
changes in brain circulation and metabolism
(energy consumption). Surprisingly, these
studies have revealed that the additional energy
required for such brain responses is extremely
small compared to the ongoing amount of
energy that the brain normally and continu-
ously expends (2). The brain apparently uses
most of its energy for functions unaccounted
for—dark energy, in astronomical terms. What
do we know about this dark energy?
The adult human brain represents about
2% of the body weight, yet accounts for about
20% of the body’s total energy consumption,
–10.0 0.0 10.0
10 times that predicted by its weight alone.
What fraction of this energy is directly related
At rest, but active. fMRI images of a normal human brain at rest. The images reveal the highly organized
to brain function? Depending on the approach nature of intrinsic brain activity, represented by correlated spontaneous fluctuations in the fMRI signal.
Correlations are depicted by an arbitrary color scale. Positive correlations reside in areas known to increase
activity during responses to controlled stimuli; negative correlations reside in areas that decrease activity
The author is in the Washington University School
under the same conditions. (Left) Lateral and medial views of the left hemisphere; (center) dorsal view;
of Medicine, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA. E-mail:
(right) lateral and medial views of the right hemisphere. [Reprinted from (12)]
marc@npg.wustl.edu

www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 314 24 NOVEMBER 2006 1249


Published by AAAS
PERSPECTIVES

Another possibility is that the brain’s enor- 10). In this regard, descriptions of slow fluc- References
mous intrinsic functional activity facilitates tuations (nominally <0.1 Hz) in neuronal 1. R. Llinas, I of the Vortex: From Neurons to Self (MIT Press,
Cambridge, MA, 2001).
responses to stimuli. Neurons continuously membrane polarization—so-called up and 2. M. E. Raichle, M. Mintun, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 29, 449
receive both excitatory and inhibitory inputs. down states—are intriguing (4, 10). Not only (2006).
The “balance” of these stimuli determines the does their temporal frequency correspond to 3. A. Peters, B. R. Payne, J. Budd, Cereb. Cortex 4, 215
(1994).
responsiveness (or gain) of neurons to corre- that of the spontaneous fluctuations in the 4. B. Haider, A. Duque, A. R. Hasenstaub, D. A. McCormick,
lated inputs and, in so doing, potentially fMRI BOLD signal, but their functional con- J. Neurosci. 26, 4535 (2006).
sculpts communication pathways in the brain sequences may be relevant to an understand- 5. B. A. Olshausen, in The Visual Neurosciences, L. M. Chalupa,
J. S. Werner, Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2003),
(4). Balance also manifests at a large systems ing of the variability in task-evoked brain pp. 1603–1615.
level. For example, neurologists know that activity as well as behavioral variability in 6. D. H. Ingvar, Hum. Neurobiol. 4, 127 (1985).
strokes that damage cortical centers that con- human performance. 7. D. Gilbert, Stumbling on Happiness (Knopf, New York,
2006).
trol eye movements lead to deviation of the William James presciently suggested in 8. J. Hawkins, S. Blakeslee, On Intelligence (Holt, New York,
eyes toward the side of the lesion, implying 1890 (11) that “Enough has now been said to 2004).
the preexisting presence of “balance.” It may prove the general law of perception, which is 9. D. A. Leopold, Y. Murayama, N. K. Logothetis, Cereb.
Cortex 13, 422 (2003).
be that in the normal brain, a balance of this, that whilst part of what we perceive comes 10. C. C. H. Petersen, T. T. G. Hahn, M. Mehta, A. Grinvald, B.
opposing forces enhances the precision of a through our senses from the object before us, Sakmann, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 13638

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on November 24, 2006


wide range of processes. Thus, “balance” another part (and it may be the larger part) (2003).
might be viewed as a necessary enabling, but always comes (in Lazarus’s phrase) out of our 11. W. James, Principles of Psychology (Henry Holt &
Company, New York, 1890), vol. 2, p. 103.
costly, element of brain function. own head.” The brain’s energy consumption 12. M. D. Fox et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 9673
A more expanded view is that intrinsic tells us that the brain is never at rest. The chal- (2005).
activity instantiates the maintenance of infor- lenge of neuroscience is to understand the func-
mation for interpreting, responding to, and tions associated with this energy consumption. 10.1126/science. 1134405
even predicting environmental demands. In
this regard, a useful conceptual framework
from theoretical neuroscience posits that the ATMOSPHERE
brain operates as a Bayesian inference engine,
designed to generate predictions about the
future (5). Beginning with a set of “advance” How Fast Are the Ice Sheets
predictions at birth (genes), the brain is
then sculpted by worldly experience to repre-
sent intrinsically a “best guess” (“priors” in
Melting?
Bayesian parlance) about the environment Anny Cazenave
and, in the case of humans at least, to make
predictions about the future (6). It has long Remote-sensing data suggest that ice sheets currently contribute little to sea-level rise. However,
been thought that the ability to reflect on the dynamical instabilities in response to climate warming may cause faster ice-mass loss.
past or contemplate the future has facilitated
the development of unique human attributes

I
f the ice sheets covering Greenland and tion in sea-level rise or a natural fluctuation on
such as imagination and creativity (7, 8). Antarctica were to melt completely, they a decadal time scale.
fMRI provides one important experimen- would raise sea level by about 65 m. But Present-day sea-level rise has several
tal approach to understanding the nature of even a small loss of ice mass from the ice causes. During the past decade, ocean warm-
the brain’s intrinsic functional activity with- sheets would have a great impact on sea level, ing has contributed roughly half of the ob-
out direct recourse to controlled stimuli and particularly on low-lying islands and coastal served rate of sea-level rise (5), leaving the
observable behaviors. A prominent fea- regions. New satellite other half for ocean-mass increase caused by
ture of fMRI is that the unaveraged signal observations, includ- water exchange with continents, glaciers, and
is quite noisy, prompting researchers to Enhanced online at ing those reported ice sheets (6). The contribution of mountain
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/
average their data to reduce this “noise” content/full/314/5803/1250
by Luthcke et al. glaciers and small ice caps to sea-level rise in
and increase the signals they seek. In on page 1286 of this the past decade is estimated to be ~0.8
doing this, it turns out that a considerable issue (1), now allow mm/year (7). These figures constrain the con-
fraction of the variance in the blood oxygen estimates of the mass balances of the ice tribution from ice sheets to less than 1
level–dependent (BOLD) signal of fMRI in sheets and their evolution through time. mm/year in the past decade.
the frequency range below 0.1 Hz, which For the past 3000 years, global sea level Since the early 1990s, remote-sensing data
reflects fluctuating neural activity, is lost. has remained stable, but since the end of the based on airborne laser and satellite radar
This activity exhibits striking patterns of 19th century, tide gauges have detected global altimetry, as well as the space-borne Synthetic
coherence within known networks of specific sea-level rises [~1.8 mm/year on average over Aperture Radar Interferometry (InSAR) tech-
neurons in the human brain in the absence of the past 50 years (2, 3)]. Satellite altimetry nique, have provided the first observations of
observable behaviors (see the figure). data document a rate of ~3 mm/year since ice sheet mass balance (8–13). These observa-
Future research should address the cellu- 1993 (4). However, it remains unclear whether tions indicate accelerated ice-mass loss in
lar events underlying spontaneous fMRI the recent rate increase reflects an accelera- recent years in the coastal regions of southern
BOLD signal fluctuations. Studies likely will Greenland. In contrast, slight mass gain is
cover a broad range of approaches to the The author is at the Observatoire Midi-Pyrenees, 31400 reported in central high-elevation regions.
study of spontaneous activity of neurons (9, Toulouse, France. E-mail: anny.cazenave@cnes.fr Over Antarctica, remote sensing indicates

1250 24 NOVEMBER 2006 VOL 314 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org


Published by AAAS

You might also like