You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52:3 (2011), pp 248–255 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02336.

Perceptions of social conflicts among


incarcerated adolescents with callous-
unemotional traits: ‘You’re going to pay. It’s
going to hurt, but I don’t care.’
Dustin Pardini
University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, USA

Background: Delinquent youth with callous-unemotional (CU) traits may have a unique social-cog-
nitive processing pattern that perpetuates their violent behavior. The current study examined the
association between CU traits and the endorsement of deviant social goals during peer conflicts as
well as expectancies and values regarding victim suffering following aggression. Methods: Partici-
pants included 156 (84 males, 72 females) adjudicated juveniles residing at two gender-specific res-
idential facilities in an urban city within the southeastern United States. The association between CU
traits and participants’ ratings of their social goals in hypothetic conflict situations and outcome
expectancies/values regarding victim suffering were examined after controlling for prior violence,
intelligence, and demographic covariates. Results: CU traits were associated with an increased
endorsement of social goals associated with revenge, dominance, and forced respect in social conflict
situations. Adjudicated youth with CU traits were also less likely to endorse conflict avoidance and
friendship building as important social goals when provoked by peers. There was no association
between CU traits and expectations for victim suffering following aggression, but CU traits were sig-
nificantly associated with lower levels of concern about victim suffering. These findings were signifi-
cant after controlling for participants’ prior history of violence, intelligence, and demographic
covariates. Conclusions: Adjudicated youth with CU traits tend to emphasize power-oriented goals
when provoked by peers and have little interest in rectifying social conflicts to build potential
friendships with others. Juveniles with CU traits seem to be aware that their aggressive behavior will
cause others to suffer, but they do not care when it does. Keywords: callous-unemotional, social-
cognition, violence, social goals, outcome values. Abbreviation: CU: callous-unemotional.

The presence of callous-unemotional (CU) traits may CU traits downplay the likelihood that their aggres-
distinguish a group of delinquent youth with a par- sive behavior will result in victim suffering versus
ticularly severe form of antisocial behavior with simply not caring when it does. Studies have also not
unique etiological origins (Frick & Dickens, 2006). examined whether delinquent juveniles with CU
Some evidence suggests that youth with CU traits traits endorse power-oriented social goals and min-
may have a fairly unique social schema that serves to imize relationship building goals following minor
perpetuate their violent behavior over time (Pardini, peer provocation. It also remains unclear whether
Lochman, & Frick, 2003). In the most basic sense, there are social-cognitive processing patterns that
schemas are defined as an individual’s consistent are unique to delinquent youth with CU traits or
beliefs or ways of thinking about relationships with whether they are more generally associated with
others that organize and transform interpretations violent youth regardless of their level of CU traits.
and predictions about events in the world (Lochman
& Pardini, 2008). It is generally thought that sche-
Social goals and violent behavior
mas can influence behavior by modifying certain
social processing variables such as social goals Social goals represent the end state of the problem-
during ambiguous conflicts, expectations regarding solving process that is important to understanding
the consequences of aggression, and concerns about the motivating force behind an individual’s behaviors
the possible outcomes of aggression. Emerging evi- (Lochman, Wayland, & White, 1993). Reactions to
dence suggests that delinquent adolescents with CU relatively minor conflicts can be significantly influ-
traits may exhibit a unique set of beliefs that enced by the relative importance children place on
emphasize aggression as an effective means for various potential outcomes. For example, youth who
obtaining positive outcomes (Pardini et al., 2003). emphasize the importance of de-escalating potential
However, it is unclear whether antisocial youth with conflicts or working out misunderstandings with
the goal of friendship building should be less likely
Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared. to retaliate aggressively following relatively minor
 2010 The Author. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
 2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA 02148, USA
Perceptions of social conflicts 249

provocation from peers. In contrast, juveniles produced somewhat mixed results. While several
involved in conflicted social interactions who become studies have found that male and female psycho-
fixated on goals such as vengeance, dominance, and pathic offenders have difficulties recognizing distress
forcing others to show ‘respect’ are likely to engage cues (i.e., fear, sadness) in others (Blair et al., 2004;
perpetually in acts of violence in order to achieve Eisenbarth, Alpers, Segre, Calogero, & Angrilli,
these goals. 2008; Hastings, Tangney, & Stuewig, 2008), some
Research has supported the importance of social studies report no emotion recognition deficits among
goals in understanding aggressive behavior in adults with psychopathic traits (Dolan & Fullam,
children and adolescents. Youth with a dominance- 2004; Glass & Newman, 2006). Using hypothetical
oriented goal pattern in interpersonal conflicts tend vignettes, Dolan and Fullam (2004) found that both
to exhibit higher levels of aggression and criminal psychopathic and non-psychopathic offenders
behavior than their peers (Lochman et al., 1993). exhibit deficits in their ability to determine the im-
Aggressive boys with attention deficit/hyperactivity pact that hurtful or insulting comments may have on
disorder (ADHD) have been shown to endorse global others relative to healthy controls.
social goals involving domination, disruption, and Similar to the adult literature, conflicting evidence
trouble-making more often than nonaggressive has emerged related to the association between CU
ADHD boys and normal comparison peers (Melnick traits and anticipating the suffering of others in
& Hinshaw, 1996). Although we know of no pub- children and adolescents. One study found that
lished studies investigating the social goals of children with psychopathic traits exhibit more
children with CU traits, one study found that aggression on a computerized game unless they are
childhood conduct problems were associated with explicitly told their behavior is causing their victim
the generation of fewer prosocial and more antiso- distress (van Baardewijk, Stegge, Bushman, & Ver-
cial solutions to hypothetical social problems meiren, 2009), suggesting that they may not fully
involving sharing and friendship making only for appreciate the negative impact their behavior has on
children with low CU traits (Waschbusch, Walsh, others. Although higher levels of psychopathic fea-
Andrade, King, & Carrey, 2007). However, what tures have also been associated with a reduced
remains unclear is whether delinquent youth with ability to recognize distress cues in samples of chil-
CU traits downplay the importance of social goals dren and adolescents (Blair, 1999; Blair, Budhani,
associated with conflict avoidance and relationship Colledge, & Scott, 2005; Blair, Colledge, Murray, &
building, while overemphasizing the importance of Mitchell, 2001; Stevens, Charman, & Blair, 2001;
more power-oriented goals, when provoked by their Woodworth & Waschbusch, 2008), children with CU
peers. traits do not seem to have difficulties recognizing
how a protagonist’s actions may influence others in
hypothetical vignettes (Woodworth & Waschbusch,
Consequences of aggression: expectations and
2008). In addition, Dadds and colleagues (2009)
concerns
found that difficulties with emotional understanding
Aggressive behavior in children and adolescents may were associated with a combined measure of CU
be partially driven by an expectation that attacking traits and antisocial behavior in early childhood, but
others will result in more positive outcomes rather not in late childhood, suggesting that some of these
than negative outcomes. Studies have shown that problems may dissipate over time. However, this
aggressive children are more confident that the use study suffered from several limitations, including the
of aggression will result in positive outcomes (Perry, use of a normative sample, the failure to look at CU
Perry, & Rasmussen, 1986), and this may be par- traits and antisocial behavior separately, and the
ticularly true of juveniles with high levels of CU traits use of a parent-report measure to assess emotional
(Pardini et al., 2003) and those exhibiting proactive understanding skills in children.
aggression (Dodge, Lochman, Harnish, Bates, & Juvenile delinquents with elevated CU traits also
Pettit, 1997). However, an issue that has not been tend to be less concerned about the negative conse-
sufficiently examined is whether delinquent youth quences of aggression (i.e., outcome values) com-
with CU traits have particular difficulty anticipating pared to delinquents without CU traits (Pardini et al.,
the negative effects their aggressive behavior may 2003). Importantly, the value children and adoles-
have on others. It is possible that adolescents with cents place on the consequences of aggression can
CU traits are less likely to believe that their aggres- be disparate from their expectations regarding the
sive actions will result in victim suffering, which likelihood of the outcome occurring. For example,
serves to perpetuate continued violent behavior. two youth may equally expect that their aggressive
In the adult literature, a historically held behavior would result in victim suffering, but they
assumption is that psychopathic offenders, who may differ in terms of the concern they have about
prototypically exhibit high levels of callous-unemo- the suffering of their victim when it occurs (Boldizar,
tional traits, do not have difficulties recognizing the Perry, & Perry, 1989). In this sense, outcome values
effect their behavior has on others (Cleckley, 1976). about victim distress are consistent with conceptu-
However, explicit tests of this assertion have alizations of affective empathy, which have been
 2010 The Author
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
250 Dustin Pardini

associated with CU traits in children (van Baa- Procedure


rdewijk et al., 2008) and adolescents (Pardini et al.,
Because juveniles residing at the two detention facilities
2003). While there is some evidence that aggressive were subjected to an initial 30-day orientation program,
youth are less likely than their nonaggressive peers only adolescents who had been incarcerated for longer
to be concerned about the feelings of others in than 30 days were invited to participate in this study.
aggressive conflicts (Hall, Herzberger, & Skowronski, Potential participants were read a consent form that
1998), it remains unclear if this is more pronounced described the basic procedures of the study, and
or unique to antisocial youth with CU traits. One explained that participation was voluntary and that
would anticipate that adjudicated youth with CU answers would be kept confidential. After the consent
traits would care little about the suffering of others form was read, youth were allowed to ask questions
following aggressive acts, and this lack of concern about the study before agreeing to participate. All
would not be characteristic of violent youth after females offenders agreed to participate and only two of
the male offenders declined participation. Written
controlling for co-occurring CU traits.
informed consent was obtained from all participants
prior to data collection.
Current study To minimize problems with low reading abilities that
frequently occur with delinquent populations, all items
The primary focus of this study is to examine the were read to participants while they responded on their
social-cognitive processing patterns associated with own questionnaires. Similar procedures for adminis-
CU traits in adjudicated juveniles. First, the notion tering social-cognitive measures have been successfully
that adjudicated youth with elevated CU traits will used in a court-ordered treatment program for chronic
endorse social goals associated with dominance and violent offenders who had low intellectual abilities
revenge and minimize social goals associated with (Dodge et al., 1997) and outpatient psychiatric clinics
conflict avoidance and friendship building when (Matthys, Cuperus, & Van Engeland, 1999). After
administration of the questionnaires, a detailed review
provoked by their peers will be examined. We will
of each participant’s file was used to gather demo-
also examine whether there is a tendency for adju- graphic characteristics, criminal history information,
dicated juveniles with CU traits to underestimate the and psychoeducational testing results.
probability that their aggressive behavior will cause
victim suffering, as well as whether juveniles with
CU traits exhibit little concern when their aggressive Measures
behavior results in victim suffering. Because social- Demographic and background variables. Informa-
cognitive processing problems have been associated tion on the participant’s age, race, and gender was
with violent behavior in children and adolescents in gathered from a demographic form included in the
several previous studies (Dodge et al., 1997; Hall detention facility client files. For analysis purposes,
et al., 1998; Lochman & Dodge, 1994; Lochman race was coded as Caucasian (1) versus all other racial
et al., 1993; Melnick & Hinshaw, 1996; Perry et al., groups (0).
1986), analyses will also examine whether CU traits
are uniquely associated with a specific pattern of Intellectual abilities. Upon admission to the deten-
social information processing even after controlling tion facility each juvenile was administered either the
for a history of violent behavior. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – 3rd Edition (WAIS-
III; Wechsler, 1997) or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale
for Children – 3rd Edition (WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991).
To control for the potential confounding effects of IQ,
Method each participant’s Full Scale IQ score was recorded.

Participants Antisocial Processes Screening Device (APSD; Frick


This sample consisted of 156 (84 males, 72 females) & Hare, 2001). This 20-item rating scale was
adjudicated juveniles with complete data on all mea- designed to measure psychopathic features in children
sures used in the current study residing at two gender- and adolescents. The self-report version of the APSD
specific residential facilities in an urban city within the has been used to identify subgroups of serious and
southeastern United States. The majority of the partic- persistent offenders in several previous studies with
ipants were African-American (n = 107, 69%), with the adolescents (for review see Frick & Dickens, 2006) and
remainder being Caucasian (n = 46, 30%), Native has shown evidence of significant temporal stability
American (n = 1, .6%), Hispanic (n = 1, .6%), and ‘Other’ from childhood to adolecence (Munoz & Frick, 2007).
unspecified (n = 1, .6%). Most juveniles were in their The callous-unemotional (CU) dimension was used in
mid teens (M = 15.83, SD = 1.30, range 11.34–18.71) the current study, which consists of 6 items related to a
and had been incarcerated for several months (M = lack of remorse and guilt, shallow and constricted
5.78, SD = 6.09). According to court records, partici- affect, a lack of empathy, apathy toward school, and
pants tended to have several prior criminal charges (M = superficial charm (Frick, O’Brien, Wootton, & McBur-
7.87, SD = 5.01), with the first charge occurring in early nett, 1994). Participants rate each item on a three-point
adolescence (M = 12.79, SD = 1.93). Approximately one- scale (0 = ‘not true at all’ to 2 = ‘definitely true’), with
third had been incarcerated at least one other time in some items being reverse scored. As with prior studies
the past (n = 47, 30%). (Frick & Dickens, 2006; Munoz & Frick, 2007), the
 2010 The Author
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Perceptions of social conflicts 251

internal consistency of the self-reported CU scale was would occur on a four-point Likert scale (1 = ‘very sure
relatively modest (a = .52). the outcome would not occur’ to 4 = ‘very sure the
outcome would occur’). For each vignette, participants
Prior violence. Two sources of information were used were asked to rate the likelihood that their aggressive
to index participants’ history of violent behavior. First, behavior would cause suffering in the victim. Items on
participants were asked to report whether they had ever the scale were averaged, with higher scores indicating
engaged in nine different violent acts included as part of increased expectations that the outcome would occur.
a modified version of the Self-Report of Delinquency The internal consistency for the scale was moderate (a =
(SRD; Loeber, Farrington, Stouthamer-Loeber, & Van .72). Studies using similar measures were able to dis-
Kammen, 1998). Items included acts such as gang criminate antisocial youth from controls (Perry et al.,
fighting, attacking someone to seriously hurt or kill 1990; Hall et al., 1998).
them, robbery, and attacking school staff. The internal
consistency of the scale was moderate (a = .70). The Outcomes Values Questionnaire (OVQ; Pardini
second source of violence history information was et al., 2003). This version of the OVQ was adapted
extracted from the official criminal record information from a measure developed by Perry and colleagues
kept in each participant’s client file. Specifically, the (1986) and includes eight brief vignettes designed to
number of prior violent charges the youth had received assess the values that children place on the outcomes of
was collected, including simple assault, aggravated aggression against a same-sex peer. Although the sto-
assault, robbery, murder, rape, and kidnapping. An ries in these vignettes were different than those used in
index of prior violence was calculated for each partici- the OEQ, the vignettes also described the use of
pant by summing the total number of prior violent aggression to obtain objects and retaliate against aver-
charges and the number of different violent acts they sive peer actions. After each vignette, participants rate
endorsed on SRD. how much they would care if various outcomes
occurred using a four-point Likert scale (1 = ‘not care at
Social Goal Measure (SGM; Lochman et al., all’ to 4 = ‘really care a lot’). Just like the OEQ, partici-
1993). This adaptation of the SGM consisted of four pants rated how much they would care if their behavior
hypothetical vignettes designed to assess children’s caused suffering in the peer. Items making up the scale
social goals in benign conflict situations. Participants were averaged, with higher scores indicating increased
were presented with a depiction of a relatively minor importance being placed on the outcome. The internal
provocation from a same-sex peer (e.g., kid laughs after consistency for the scale was high (a = .92). Similar
you trip accidentally) and are then asked to indicate the measures have discriminated between aggressive and
importance of various social goals on a four-point scale nonaggressive youth (Hall et al., 1998).
(1 = ‘not important’ to 4 = ‘very important’). The five goals
assessed were avoiding conflict (i.e., ‘avoid problems
with him/her’), dominance (‘let the guy/girl know who is Results
in charge or who’s boss’), revenge (i.e., ‘get back at him/
her’), forced respect (i.e., ‘make him/her show you some Descriptive statistics and correlations for study
respect’), and reconciliation (‘Work things out with him/ variables are presented in Table 1. As expected, a
her so you could possibly be friends’). Participants’ rat- moderate correlation was found between CU traits
ings were summed across the four stories to produce a and prior violence. Both CU traits and prior violence
total score for each possible social goal, with higher were significantly correlated with all social goal
scores indicating greater importance being placed on
outcomes, including a greater endorsement of goals
that particular goal. The internal consistency of the
related to dominance, revenge, forced respect, and
scales ranged from acceptable to good (as = .61–.80).
After rating the five social goals, participants were overall conflict escalation, as well as lower endorse-
asked to indicate which would be their primary goal for ment of goals related to conflict avoidance and rela-
each vignette. A variable representing an overall ten- tionship building. While neither CU traits nor prior
dency to focus on escalating conflict was created by violence was significantly associated with expecta-
summing the number of times youth reported a deviant tions that aggressive acts would cause victim suf-
primary goal (i.e., revenge, dominance, forced respect) fering, they both were significantly correlated with a
and subtracting the number of times they reported decreased concern about the victim suffering as the
prosocial goals (i.e., avoiding conflict, reconciliation). result of aggression.
Regression analysis was used to examine the
Outcomes Expectations Questionnaire (OEQ; Par- association between CU traits and prior violence and
dini et al., 2003). This version of the OEQ was each of the social goal variables after controlling for
adapted from a measure developed by Perry and overlap between the constructs as well as potential
colleagues (1989) and consists of eight brief vignettes
confounds (i.e., gender, race, age, Full Scale IQ).
designed to measure juveniles’ expectations that
Similar to correlation analysis, both CU traits and
aggressive behavior against a same-sex peer would
result in various outcomes. In the vignettes, partici- prior violence significantly predicted social goals
pants imagined using aggressive behavior to obtain related to conflict avoidance, dominance, revenge,
objects and retaliate against aversive actions from a forced respect, and overall conflict escalation (see
hostile peer. After hearing each vignette, participants Table 2). However, only CU traits significantly
are asked to rate the likelihood that various outcomes predicted the social goal of relationship building,

 2010 The Author


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
252 Dustin Pardini

17.88
7.50
14 Table 2 Callous-unemotional traits and prior violence pre-
dicting social goals and outcome expectations/values

Predictors

14.49
4.17
13

CU traits Prior violence

)12
Outcomes B SE b B SE b

).30***
Social goals
12

).01

2.34
1.28
Avoid conflict ).24 .12 ).16* ).41 .10 ).31***
Dominance .41 .11 .28*** .29 .10 .23**
Revenge .35 .11 .25** .27 .10 .20**
).66***

.36***
Forced respect .26 .12 .17* .24 .11 .18*
11

).01

7.83
3.54
Relationship building ).41 .13 ).26** ).13 .12 ).09
Primary goal: conflict .29 .09 .24** .18 .08 .18*
escalation
).47***
.60***

).33***
Outcome expectation
).12

10.92
3.28
10

Victim suffering .14 .16 .07 .08 .14 .05


Outcome values
Victim suffering )1.81 .24 ).53*** ).38 .21 ).13
.57***
).51***
.56***

).29***
).01

8.88
3.09
9

Note: All effects depicted are after controlling for gender,


minority status, and Full Scale IQ. CU = callous-unemotional
traits; B = unstandardized coefficient; b = standardized coef-
.73***
.66***
).46***
.52***

).31***

ficient.
).12

9.40
3.13
8

with higher levels of CU traits predicting lower re-


).30***
).33***
).36***
.42***
).47***

.25*

lationship building scores.


).10

9.32
3.29
7

Analysis predicting outcome expectancies and


outcome values related to victim suffering following
).31***
.29***

.25***

).32***
.27**

aggression are presented in Table 2. While CU traits


).16*
.24*
.06

5.99
2.51
6

were not significantly related to expectations that


aggressive behavior would result in victim suffering,
.27***
.33***

.35***
.31***

).58***

they were significantly related to less concern about


).24**

.25**
).26**

.08

5.59
2.20
5

victim suffering following aggression. On the other


hand, prior violence did not significantly contribute
to the prediction of expectations or values regarding
).26**

78.94
14.15
.11
).08
).14

).08
).05
).03
).13

.14
.03

victim suffering in the regression analysis after


4

controlling for co-occurring CU traits and potential


confounds.
.46***

).21*
).16*

.16*
.03
).05

.05
.02
).07
.01

.08
3

46
29%

Gender differences
Table 1 Correlations and descriptive statistics for study variables

Because there are relatively few studies of social-


.22**
.21**

).20*

.17*

cognitive processes in adjudicated girls, interactions


.01
).11

.07
.04
.02
).12
.00

).10
2

between gender and both CU traits and prior


72
46%

violence in predicting all social-cognitive variables


were tested. Only one significant interaction was
.36***
.23**

observed, namely an interaction between female


15.83
.14
.15
).14
).09
.05
).01
.01
).15
).11

.05
.15

1.30
1

gender and prior violence predicting the social goal of


forced respect (B = .47, SE = .22, p < .05). Probing of
this interaction revealed that there was a significant
12. Primary goal: conflict escalation

relation between prior violence and an increased


13. Expectation: victim suffering

*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.


11. Goal: relationship building

endorsement of the social goal of forced respect for


females (B = .52, SE = 17, p < .01), but this associ-
7. Goal: conflict avoidance

14. Value: victim suffering

ation was non-significant for males (B = .06, SE =


10. Goal: forced respect

.14, p = .70). No other significant interactions were


8. Goal: dominance

found (all ps ‡ .10).


9. Goal: revenge
6. Prior violence
4. Full Scale IQ
3. Caucasian

5. CU traits
2. Female

CU traits by prior violence interactions


1. Age

SD/N
M/%

To examine whether the associations between CU


traits and the social-cognitive variables examined
 2010 The Author
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Perceptions of social conflicts 253

were more or less pronounced in juveniles with a standing the consequences their behavior has on
history of violent behavior, the analyses were re-run others (Dadds et al., 2009) and recognizing distress
to include an interaction between CU traits and prior cues (Blair, 1999), the current results provided no
violence. None of the interaction terms were statis- evidence of such a deficit in terms of anticipating
tically significant in these models (all ps > .42). This victim suffering. When delinquents were asked to
indicates that the associations between CU traits rate how likely it was that their aggressive behavior
and the social-cognitive variables did not differ for would produce victim suffering across vignettes,
individuals with high versus low levels of prior vio- those with higher CU traits did not exhibit a de-
lence. creased expectation that this outcome would occur.
While this runs counter to evidence that young chil-
dren with CU traits may have difficulties under-
standing emotions in others, once children enter
Discussion
adolescence, this deficit may dissipate (Dadds et al.,
The current study was designed to examine the 2009). Among the juvenile delinquents, there was
association between CU traits and adjudicated also no evidence that violent offenders exhibited a
juveniles’ social perceptions of peer conflicts after reduced expectation that aggressive behavior would
controlling for co-occurring violence. Findings indi- cause victim suffering. In contrast to findings
cated that juveniles with elevated CU traits are more regarding expectations, juvenile delinquents with CU
likely to endorse social goals associated with revenge traits exhibited a substantial decrease in concern
and dominance following minor provocation from about the suffering of their victims following acts of
peers, even after controlling for prior violent behav- aggression. As expected, this lack of concern was
ior. In addition, a relative lack of relationship- unrelated to a history of violent offending after con-
building social goals following peer conflicts seemed trolling for co-occurring CU traits. In sum, one of the
to be unique to juveniles exhibiting CU traits, rather core deficits in adjudicated juveniles with CU traits is
than common to all violent delinquents. The findings that they care very little about the suffering of others,
also clearly illustrate that among adjudicated youth, not that they have difficulty anticipating the extent to
those with CU traits do not seem to have particular which their aggression will cause others to suffer.
difficulties judging whether their aggressive behavior
will cause victim suffering. Instead, they simply do
Limitations
not care when it does.
Juveniles with CU traits generally seemed to focus These findings need to be interpreted cautiously due
on social goals aimed at escalating conflict with to several limitations. First, the cross-sectional nat-
peers in order to assert a position of power. However, ure of the data makes it impossible to fully test
this social schema was also characteristic of juve- models regarding the temporal ordering of CU traits,
niles with a prior history of violence even after con- violent behavior, and the social-cognitive variables
trolling for levels of CU traits. This is consistent with measured. Also, most of the variables used in this
studies indicating that aggressive and violent youth study were assessed through self-report, which may
tend to endorse a dominance-oriented goal pattern have artificially inflated variable associations due to
more often than their peers (Lochman et al., 1993; shared method variance. In contrast, some of the
Melnick & Hinshaw, 1996). The current study subscales used in this study had relatively low
expands on these findings by demonstrating that internal consistencies, so the significance tests
juveniles who are high on both CU traits and violent reported should be viewed as conservative estimates.
behavior have a particularly deviant goal-set in While the self-report scale of CU traits used in the
conflict situations that may serve to perpetuate their current study has historically exhibited lower than
violent behavior over time. Given that many of the desirable levels of internal consistency, it has been
social goals endorsed involve attempts at revenge shown to predict serious antisocial behavior, pro-
and dominance, these juveniles may be prone to gram non-compliance, and recidivism in previous
engage in premeditated forms of retaliatory violence studies (Frick & Dickens, 2006). Another limitation
following even slight provocation from peers. There is that this study was conducted solely with incar-
was also a tendency for juveniles with CU traits to be cerated adolescents, so the results cannot be gen-
particularly unlikely to endorse relationship building eralized to youth exhibiting antisocial behavior in the
as a social goal in minor conflict situations, but this community. This sampling scheme also prevented us
was not characteristic of violent offenders after con- from examining how delinquents and non-delin-
trolling for co-occurring CU traits. Therefore, youth quents may differ on the social-cognitive variables
with CU traits tend to have little desire to develop assessed in the study. For example, it is possible
meaningful relationships with others, especially that delinquent youth are less likely to expect that
those peers who provoke them in relatively minor aggressive behavior would cause victim suffering
ways. when compared to non-delinquent youth. Lastly,
While there has been some speculation that delin- the current study did not assess relational aggres-
quents with CU traits may have difficulties under- sion, which is postulated to be more relevant to
 2010 The Author
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
254 Dustin Pardini

understanding antisocial behavior in girls and may lihood not be effective for juveniles with CU traits.
be associated with different social information pro- Children with CU traits may be fully aware that their
cessing deficits than overt violence (Crick, Grotpeter, victims will suffer when they are attacked, but they
& Bigbee, 2002). While the current study found do not care when it does. While getting these youth to
that violent and callous-unemotional behaviors in care about the suffering of others is a critical element
boys and girls are associated with similar types of to their rehabilitation, treatments designed to instill
maladaptive social information processing, future feelings of concern for others may only be effective if
studies must examine whether this also applies to implemented early in development when CU features
more relational forms of aggression. are initially manifested.

Potential clinical implications


Acknowledgements
The presence of CU traits seems to designate a
The first author was supported by a grant from
subgroup of delinquent youth who have a number of
National Institute of Mental Health (K01MH078039),
maladaptive social-cognitive processing character-
National Institute on Drug Abuse (F31DA14452),
istics. Although firm recommendations for dealing
and University of Alabama Graduate Student award.
with juvenile offenders with CU traits cannot be
Special thanks to Drs. William Chaplin, Leroy Rich-
made based on the current findings, these juveniles
ardson, Mark Burge, John Lochman and Paul Frick
may be particularly difficult to treat given their
and the study participants.
fixation on revenge, dominance, and forced respect
following relatively minor conflict situations.
Because youth with CU traits are less likely to be
Correspondence to
motivated to resolve conflicts as a way to develop and
maintain relationships with others, therapeutic Correspondence regarding this manuscript should
interventions may have to emphasize more self- be addressed to Dustin Pardini, University of Pitts-
serving reasons for not attacking others. It also burgh Medical Center, Sterling Plaza, Suite 408, 201
seems that interventions designed to teach juvenile North Craig Street, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA; Tel:
offenders to more fully understand the negative 412-383-5041; Fax: 412-383-5068; Email: dap38@
effect their aggression has on others will in all like- pitt.edu

Key points

• CU traits may be useful for identifying adjudicated youth with a particular social-cognitive processing
pattern underlying their antisocial behavior.
• Adjudicated adolescents with CU traits tend to endorse social goals associated with dominance and
revenge in peer conflict situations, while downplaying conflict avoidance and friendship building as social
goals.
• Adjudicated juveniles with CU traits did not appear to have difficulties judging whether their aggressive
behavior would result in victim suffering; they simply did not care when it did.
• CU traits were associated with deviant social goals in conflict situations and reduced concern about the
suffering of others, even after controlling for participants’ prior history of violent behavior.

Boldizar, J.P., Perry, D.G., & Perry, L.C. (1989). Outcome


References values and aggression. Child Development, 60, 571–
Blair, R.J. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the 579.
child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Cleckley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity (5th ed.). St. Louis,
Individual Differences, 27, 135–145. MO: Mosby.
Blair, R.J., Budhani, S., Colledge, E., & Scott, S. (2005). Crick, N.R., Grotpeter, J.K., & Bigbee, M.A. (2002). Rela-
Deafness to fear in boys with psychopathic tendencies. tionally and physically aggressive children’s intent attri-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 327–336. butions and feelings of distress for relational and
Blair, R.J., Colledge, E., Murray, L., & Mitchell, D.G. instrumental peer provocations. Child Development, 73,
(2001). A selective impairment in the processing of sad 1134–1142.
and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic Dadds, M.R., Hawes, D.J., Frost, A.D., Vassallo, S., Bunn,
tendencies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, P., Hunter, K., & Merz, S. (2009). Learning to ‘talk the
491–498. talk’: The relationship of psychopathic traits to deficits in
Blair, R.J., Mitchell, D.G., Peschardt, K.S., Colledge, E., empathy across childhood. Journal of Child Psychology
Leonard, R.A., Shine, J.H., Murray, L.K., & Perrett, D.I. and Psychiatry, 50, 599–606.
(2004). Reduced sensitivity to others’ fearful expressions Dodge, K.A., Lochman, J.E., Harnish, J.D., Bates, J.E., &
in psychopathic individuals. Personality and Individual Pettit, G.S. (1997). Reactive and proactive aggression in
Differences, 37, 1111–1122. school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically

 2010 The Author


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Perceptions of social conflicts 255

assaultive youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, and comparison boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-
37–51. chology, 24, 169–185.
Dolan, M., & Fullam, R. (2004). Theory of mind and Munoz, L.C., & Frick, P.J. (2007). The reliability, stability,
mentalizing ability in antisocial personality disorders and predictive utility of the self-report version of the
with and without psychopathy. Psychological Medicine, Antisocial Process Screening Device. Scandinavian Jour-
34, 1093–1102. nal of Psychology, 48, 299–312.
Eisenbarth, H., Alpers, G.W., Segre, D., Calogero, A., & Pardini, D.A., Lochman, J.E., & Frick, P.J. (2003). Cal-
Angrilli, A. (2008). Categorization and evaluation of lous/unemotional traits and social-cognitive processes
emotional faces in psychopathic women. Psychiatry in adjudicated youths. Journal of the American Academy
Research, 159, 189–195. of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 364–371.
Frick, P.J., & Dickens, C. (2006). Current perspectives on Perry, D.G., Perry, L.C., & Rasmussen, P. (1986). Cognitive
conduct disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 8, 59–72. social learning mediators of aggression. Child Develop-
Frick, P.J., & Hare, R.D. (2001). Antisocial Process Screen- ment, 57, 700–711.
ing Device technical manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi- Perry, D.G., Perry, L.C., & Weiss, R.J. (1989). Sex differ-
Health Systems. ences in the consequences that children anticipate for
Frick, P.J., O’Brien, B.S., Wootton, J.M., & McBurnett, K. aggression. Developmental Psychology, 25, 312–319.
(1994). Psychopathy and conduct problems in children. Perry, D.G., Williard, J.C., & Perry, L.C. (1990). Peers’
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 700–707. perceptions of the consequences that victimized chil-
Glass, S.J., & Newman, J.P. (2006). Recognition of facial dren provide aggressors. Child Development, 61, 1310–
affect in psychopathic offenders. Journal of Abnormal 1325.
Psychology, 115, 815–820. Stevens, D., Charman, T., & Blair, R.J. (2001). Recognition
Hall, J.A., Herzberger, S.D., & Skowronski, K.J. (1998). of emotion in facial expressions and vocal tones in
Outcome expectancies and outcome values as predictors children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of
of children’s aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 24, 439– Genetic Psychology, 162, 201–211.
454. van Baardewijk, Y., Stegge, H., Andershed, H., Thomaes,
Hastings, M.E., Tangney, J.P., & Stuewig, J. (2008). S., Scholte, E., & Vermeiren, R. (2008). Measuring
Psychopathy and identification of facial expressions of psychopathic traits in children through self-report. The
emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, development of the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory–
1474–1483. Child Version. International Journal of Law and Psychi-
Lochman, J.E., & Dodge, K.A. (1994). Social-cognitive atry, 31, 199–209.
processes of severely violent, moderately aggressive, van Baardewijk, Y., Stegge, H., Bushman, B.J., & Vermei-
and nonaggressive boys. Journal of Consulting and ren, R. (2009). Psychopathic traits, victim distress and
Clinical Psychology, 62, 366–374. aggression in children. Journal of Child Psychology and
Lochman, J.E., & Pardini, D.A. (2008). Cognitive-behav- Psychiatry, 50, 718–725.
ioral therapies. In M. Rutter, D. Bishop, D. Pine, S. Scott, Waschbusch, D.A., Walsh, T.M., Andrade, B.F., King, S.,
J. Stevenson, E. Taylor, & A. Thapar (Eds.), Rutter’s child & Carrey, N.J. (2007). Social problem solving, conduct
and adolescent psychiatry (5th edn, pp. 1026–1045). problems, and callous-unemotional traits in children.
Oxford: Blackwell. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 37, 293–
Lochman, J.E., Wayland, K.K., & White, K.J. (1993). Social 305.
goals: Relationship to adolescent adjustment and to Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Chil-
social-problem solving. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- dren – Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
chology, 21, 135–151. Corporation.
Loeber, R., Farrington, D.P., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale –
Van Kammen, W.B. (1998). Antisocial behavior and Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corpora-
mental health problems: Explanatory factors in childhood tion.
and adolescence. New York: Routledge. Woodworth, M., & Waschbusch, D. (2008). Emotional
Matthys, W., Cuperus, J.M., & Van Engeland, H. (1999). processing in children with conduct problems and cal-
Deficient social problem-solving in boys with ODD/CD, lous/unemotional traits. Child Care Health and Devel-
with ADHD, and with both disorders. Journal of the opment, 34, 234–244.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
38, 311–321. Manuscript accepted 18 August 2010
Melnick, S.M., & Hinshaw, S.P. (1996). What they want
and what they get: The social goals of boys with ADHD

 2010 The Author


Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2010 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Copyright of Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like