You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association

2007, Vol. 116, No. 3, 543–553 0021-843X/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.543

The Acute Effects of Nicotine on Positive and Negative Affect in


Adolescent Smokers
Jon D. Kassel, Daniel P. Evatt, Justin E. Greenstein, Margaret C. Wardle, Marisa C. Yates, and
Jennifer C. Veilleux
University of Illinois at Chicago

Although adolescent cigarette smoking remains a critical public health concern, little is known about the
reinforcing mechanisms governing smoking in this vulnerable population. To assess predictions derived
from both positive and negative reinforcement models of drug use, the authors measured the acute effects
of nicotine, as administered via tobacco cigarettes, on both positive and negative affect in a group of 15-
to 18-year-old smokers. A matched group of nonsmokers served as a comparison group. Findings
revealed that whereas adolescents who smoked a cigarette experienced reductions in both positive and
negative affect, the observed reductions in negative affect were moderated by nicotine content of the
cigarette (high yield vs. denicotinized), level of nicotine dependence, level of baseline craving, and
smoking expectancies pertinent to negative affect regulation. Nonsmokers experienced no change in
affect over the 10-min assessment period, and no interaction effects were observed for positive affect.
Overall, the findings conform to a negative reinforcement model of nicotine effects and strongly suggest
that, even among young light smokers, nicotine dependence and resultant withdrawal symptomatology
may serve as motivating factors governing smoking behavior.

Keywords: adolescent smoking, nicotine, affect, withdrawal, dependence

Adolescent cigarette smoking remains a significant public about the motivational processes subserving adolescent smoking.
health concern. As many as 80% of tobacco users begin smoking Given the notable health risks posed by the use of tobacco,
before they are 18. Whereas adolescent smoking in the United research on the determinants of adolescent smoking is urgently
States appears to have peaked in the mid-1990s (Johnston, needed, as it can inform prevention, treatment, and policy at
O’Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2005b), recent data suggest multiple levels.
that the rate of decline in adolescents’ use of cigarettes has been Although there has been some problem in clearly determining
decelerating over the past several years (Johnston, O’Malley, the underlying mechanisms that explain why tobacco smoking is
Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2005a). In fact, among eighth graders, reinforcing among adolescents, there is strong reason to believe
smoking rates have plateaued, potentially a bad sign for future that most adult smokers smoke, at least in part, as a way of
smoking trends among young adults. In short, if the trend in early regulating affect. Indeed, some of the most influential models of
initiation of cigarette smoking continues, approximately 5 million addictive behavior have emphasized the importance of aversive
children who are living today will die prematurely because they affective states precipitating drug use, coupled with the notion that
began to smoke cigarettes during adolescence. As significant as drug use assuages such emotional distress (e.g., Siegel, 1983;
this public health problem is, however, surprisingly little is known Wikler, 1973; see Kassel et al., 2006, for a discussion of negative
affect [NA] and addiction). Such sentiments have also been well
articulated within the stress– coping (Wills & Shiffman, 1985) and
self-medication models (Khantzian, 1997) of substance use. Con-
Jon D. Kassel, Daniel P. Evatt, Justin E. Greenstein, Margaret C.
Wardle, Marisa C. Yates, and Jennifer C. Veilleux, Department of Psy-
sistent with these conceptualizations, a large literature on smoking
chology, University of Illinois at Chicago. motives (Shiffman, 1993) and smoking expectancies (Brandon,
Portions of this article were presented at the 11th annual meeting of the Juliano, & Copeland, 1999) reliably demonstrates that the majority
Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco, Prague, Czechoslovakia, of adult smokers attribute their smoking to its reputed ability to
March 2005, and the 13th annual meeting of the Society for Research on calm them when stressed and, more generally, reduce NA (e.g.,
Nicotine and Tobacco, Austin, Texas, February 2007. Ikard, Green, & Horn, 1969; McKennell, 1970; Spielberger, 1986;
This research was supported by the Ove Ferno Award for Innovative see Kassel, Stroud, & Paronis, 2003). Thus, smoking as an anxi-
Research on Nicotine and Tobacco by the Society for Research on Nicotine olytic tool emerges as the most widely endorsed motive among
and Tobacco and by Pharmacia and Upjohn. Preparation of this article was adult smokers, particularly among those who are nicotine depen-
also made possible, in part, by National Cancer Institute Grant
dent (Shiffman, 1993).
1PO1CA98262 and by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcohol-
ism Grant 5RO1AA12240-04.
However, less is known about the motives governing adolescent
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jon D. cigarette smoking. Although it is commonly thought that early
Kassel, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Chicago, 1007 smoking is governed more by peer and social factors (e.g., Fergu-
West Harrison Street, Behavioral Sciences Building, Room 1009, Chicago, son et al., 1992; Wang, Fitzhugh, Eddy, Fu, & Turner, 1997) than
IL 60607. E-mail: jkassel@uic.edu by attempts at mood regulation, there are relatively few data to
543
544 KASSEL ET AL.

support these assumptions. In fact, several studies have suggested experiencing at least one of five specified subjective effects of
that motives linked to affect regulation may take on significance smoking. Feeling calmer was the most frequently endorsed effect,
earlier than previously believed (Dozois, Farrow, & Miser, 1995; and daily smokers were more likely to report this than nondaily
Johnson et al., 2005; McNeill, Jarvis, & West, 1987; see also smokers (64% vs. 38%, respectively). Moreover, the likelihood of
Chassin, Presson, Rose, & Sherman, 2007, for an interesting take experiencing at least one withdrawal symptom when trying to quit
on adolescents’ views of nicotine addiction). In particular, these was greater among those who reported feeling calmer when smok-
studies have suggested that a significant proportion of adolescents ing (84% vs. 40%, respectively). Other subjective effects of smok-
attribute their smoking to relaxation and anxiolytic motives. How- ing received relatively little endorsement, with 34% experiencing
ever, we are unaware of any studies of adolescent smokers that dizziness and lightheadedness, 15% describing feeling sick, 1%
have assessed the acute effects of smoking on emotional response feeling “high,” and 8% experiencing greater alertness.
under controlled laboratory conditions. Such an approach is critical The importance attributed to relaxation motives is echoed in
to shedding light on the processes that subserve smoking behavior findings by Dozois et al. (1995) and Nichter, Nichter, Vuckovic,
among adolescents. As such, the primary goal of the present study Quintero, and Ritenbaugh (1997), who similarly found that smok-
is to assess the acute effects of nicotine, as administered via ing to relax and stress reduction were commonly reported motives
tobacco cigarettes, on both positive affect (PA) and NA in a among adolescent smoking initiates. Recent studies by Johnson et
sample of young smokers. al. (2003, 2005) also point to the significant endorsement of
emotional reinforcement motives, as a large proportion of adoles-
Do Adolescents Smoke to Regulate Affect? cent smokers attributed their smoking to its ability to facilitate
affect regulation. Correspondingly, Stevens, Colwell, Smith, Rob-
The pathways to becoming a smoker are clearly complex and inson, and McMillan (2005) found that the motive most frequently
governed by a host of genetic, social, pharmacological, and psy- endorsed by their sample of 721 adolescent smokers was smoking
chological determinants (Jamner et al., 2003; Kassel, Weinstein, in response to NA.1 Moreover, in support of previous findings
Skitch, Veilleux, & Mermelstein, 2005). In fact, social factors, (Miller, Hemenway, & Rimm, 2000), this study revealed that
such as peer affiliations and peer socialization (e.g., Oetting & adolescents whose smoking was predominantly cued by NA were
Donnermeyer, 1998), have emerged as perhaps the most potent significantly more likely to have future smoking intentions as well
and reliable predictors of smoking initiation and experimentation. as evidence significantly less self-efficacy to quit smoking, relative
Yet there is also reason to believe that among the many adoles- to adolescents who reported other reasons for smoking.
cents who try cigarettes, those who progress to regular smoking Taken together, these studies strongly suggest that many—
status, as well as nicotine dependence, are more likely to report perhaps even most—adolescents smoke both in response to and in
heightened levels of stress and NA (Chassin, Presson, Sherman, an effort to alleviate NA. However, although such findings may be
Corty, & Olshavsky, 1984; Stein, Newcomb, & Bentler, 1996) as compelling, the conclusions that can ultimately be drawn from
well as experience numerous manifestations of psychopathology them are limited because of the highly subjective and retrospective
associated with affective distress, including depression (e.g., nature of the data. Controlled laboratory studies would be useful to
Brown, Lewinsohn, Seeley, & Wagner, 1996; Escobedo, Reddy, &
determine whether smoking exerts genuine anxiolytic and NA-
Giovino, 1998; Koval, Pederson, Mills, McGrady, & Carvajal,
reducing effects (although even controlled laboratory studies that
2000) and anxiety (e.g., Patton et al., 1996; Sonntag, Wittchen,
use self-report as the primary index of affect ultimately rely on
Hofler, Kessler, & Stein, 2000). Correspondingly, it becomes
subjectivity as well). Although we acknowledge the ethical con-
critical to distinguish the processes that govern smoking initiation
straints associated with administration of nicotine to minors (Eis-
and experimentation from those that underlie progression to nic-
senberg & Balster, 2000; Moolchan & Mermelstein, 2002), such
otine dependence (Colby, Tiffany, Shiffman, & Niaura, 2000;
information is nonetheless crucial to further our knowledge of the
Kassel, 2000; Shadel, Shiffman, Niaura, Nichter, & Abrams,
effects of nicotine in new smokers.
2000).
Given the theoretical importance of appetitive, positive rein-
On the basis of findings from both cross-sectional and longitu-
forcement processes in governing drug use (e.g., Stewart, de Wit,
dinal studies, many researchers have inferred that because adoles-
& Eikelboom, 1984; Wise, 1998), it is also surprising that exam-
cents who experience significant levels of affective distress are
ination of the effects of smoking on PA has received such short
more likely to smoke, they must do so because smoking necessar-
shrift in both the adolescent and the adult smoking literatures.
ily alleviates NA. However, as we have argued elsewhere (Kassel
Indeed, understanding the extent to which drugs influence reward
& Hankin, 2006; Kassel et al, 2003), one must be careful not to
processes as well as negative reinforcement mechanisms is critical
instantiate causal (within-subject) mechanisms on the basis of
results derived from cross-sectional (between-subjects) analyses. to delineating the motivational landscape underlying substance
Although there is a dearth of empirical work devoted to answering abuse and dependence. Moreover, as there is now ample reason to
the question of whether smoking genuinely influences emotional believe that PA and NA are independent constructs (not simply
response in adolescents, several studies have offered some insight opposite sides of a bipolar continuum; e.g., Watson & Tellegen,
into the motives associated with adolescent smoking. Although
they are not a direct indicator of nicotine dependence, the subjec- 1
It is important to note that whereas delineating the immediate precip-
tive effects experienced by smokers likely reflect the role of itants, or cues, of smoking behavior is critical with respect to both theory
nicotine (and other constituents of tobacco smoke) in governing development and intervention efforts, such knowledge ultimately has no
smoking behavior. For example, McNeill et al. (1987) found that bearing on the question of whether smoking genuinely reduces NA (see
82% of a sample of 170 female adolescent smokers reported Kassel et al., 2003, for a discussion of this issue).
ADOLESCENT SMOKING 545

1985; Watson, Wiese, Vaidya, & Tellegen, 1999), it becomes to include a comparison group of nonsmokers. Participants who
important for drug researchers to examine the impact of drugs on qualified as smokers were then randomly assigned to one of two
both PA and NA, as it is conceivable that drugs may exert experimental groups: a high-yield (HY) cigarette condition or a
independent effects on each. denicotinized (DN) cigarette condition.
In sum, it is clear that many adolescent smokers attribute their
smoking to its alleged ability to reduce NA. However, we are Cigarettes
unaware of any previous laboratory investigations of nicotine’s
influence on affect in adolescent smokers (although see Corrigall, To ensure standardization in nicotine and tar yields within the
Zack, Eissenberg, Belsito, & Scher, 2001, who assessed the effects respective nicotine conditions, we used research cigarettes (Ultra-
of smoking on other subjective parameters). Because expectancies tech/Lifetech, Inc., Lafayette Hill, PA) containing .06 mg nicotine/
of NA reduction can actually influence nicotine’s effects on emo- 17.9 mg tar (DN) or 1.14 mg nicotine/15.9 mg tar (HY) nicotine
tional response to a stressor (Juliano & Brandon, 2002), we also levels (cf. Juliano & Brandon, 2002). As previously reported by
measured smoking expectancies relevant to affect regulation. On Pickworth, Fant, Nelson, Rohrer, and Henningfield (1999), the DN
the basis of the assumption that as individuals approach or have cigarettes deliver no appreciable amount of nicotine. All partici-
reached a state of nicotine dependence, they are motivated to pants were led to believe that they would be receiving and smoking
smoke, in great part, to stave off or escape aversive symptoms of a regular nicotine-yield cigarette.
nicotine withdrawal (e.g., NA; see Baker, Piper, McCarthy,
Majeskie, & Fiore, 2004), we also assessed nicotine dependence. Measures
Correspondingly, we measured craving at the study’s outset,
guided by the belief that craving represents a critical component of Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark,
the withdrawal syndrome (Baker et al., 2004; Kassel & Shiffman, & Tellegen, 1988). The PANAS is a widely used and psycho-
1992) and, as such, complements the measure of nicotine depen- metrically valid self-report measure that assesses state (how one
dence. Finally, we incorporated a measure of PA because so little feels “right now”) PA (10 items, e.g., “excited,” “alert,” “enthu-
is known about how nicotine may influence PA, or reward pro- siastic”) and NA (10 items, e.g., “upset,” “nervous,” “irritated”).
cesses, in adolescents. Thus, we examined the acute effects of The scales have been shown to be highly internally consistent,
nicotine, as administered via tobacco cigarettes, on PA and NA in largely uncorrelated, and stable at appropriate levels over a
a group of light-smoking adolescent smokers. Measures of smok- 2-month time period. In the present study, the obtained reliability
ing expectancies, nicotine dependence, and craving were also (alpha) coefficients ranged from (across the two measurement
included as possible moderators of any observed nicotine effects. periods, respectively) .74 to .75 (NA) and from .85 to .90 (PA).
Finally, we included a comparison group of nonsmokers who did Smoking Expectancies Questionnaire (Wahl, Turner, Mermel-
not receive nicotine. stein, & Flay, 2005). The Smoking Expectancies Questionnaire
is a 13-item questionnaire that taps four domains of smoking
expectancies specifically applicable to adolescent smokers. For the
Method
present analyses, we used only the Negative Affect Management
Participants (NAM; 4 items; ␣ ⫽ .72) and the Boredom Relief (BR; 3 items;
␣ ⫽ .78) subscales, as these measures were most directly related to
This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at our interest in nicotine’s effects on affect. The Smoking Expect-
the University of Illinois at Chicago. Given that most of the study ancies Questionnaire has demonstrated sound psychometric prop-
participants were under 18 years of age, such individuals also erties and predictive validity (Wahl et al., 2005).
provided signed parental consent forms, in addition to their own Nicotine dependence. Nicotine dependence was assessed with
signed assent. Because of the potentially sensitive nature of the a modified version of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire
study (i.e., administration of nicotine via cigarettes to minors), we (MFTQ; Fagerstrom, 1978), specifically designed to measure de-
took great care in ensuring that all participants and their parents pendence in adolescent smokers (Prokhorov et al., 2000). Previous
understood the objectives of the study and the requests being made findings indicate that this measure is valid and applicable to
of the participants. No participant was mandated to smoke as a part adolescent smokers (Prokhorov et al., 2000; ␣ ⫽ .71).
of study participation. Rather, adolescents were given the oppor- Cigarette craving. Craving was measured with the six-item
tunity to smoke “as much or as little” of the cigarette as they so Craving subscale of the Shiffman Jarvik Withdrawal Question-
desired. naire (Shiffman & Jarvik, 1976). This self-report questionnaire has
Forty-five adolescent smokers and 27 nonsmokers were re- been widely used and has excellent psychometric properties. In the
cruited from the Chicago greater metropolitan area as part of their present study, we obtained an alpha coefficient of .82.
participation in a larger study. Participants received either mone- Other smoking behavior. All smokers were asked how re-
tary compensation ($45) or, if they were university students (n ⫽ cently they last smoked a cigarette (to the nearest quarter hour) and
35), course credit for their participation in this one-visit study. how many cigarettes they had smoked on the study day prior to
Participants were between 15 and 18 years of age; to qualify as coming to the lab.
smokers, they were required to have smoked for at least 4 weeks Cigarette ratings. At the study’s end, all participants com-
and to smoke a minimum of one cigarette a week but no more than pleted a brief questionnaire querying them about the “harshness,”
five cigarettes a day on average. Nonsmokers must never have “strength,” and “pleasantness” of the research cigarette they
smoked at all. Thus, our intention was to assess the acute effects of smoked (Kassel & Unrod, 2000). Response options ranged from 1
nicotine in a relatively young, novice group of smokers as well as (not at all) to 4 (very much so).
546 KASSEL ET AL.

Procedure community and college student participants in terms of age (␩p2 ⫽


.01), sex (␩p2 ⫽ .01), how many days a week they smoked (␩p2 ⫽
Upon their arrival at the laboratory, all participants completed .03), number of cigarettes smoked per day (␩p2 ⫽ .06), or emo-
either informed consent or assent and baseline self-report ques- tional responsivity with respect to change in NA (␩p2 ⫽ .03) and
tionnaires, after which the details of the procedure were reiterated PA (␩p2 ⫽ .04). Thus, the study sample included young, relatively
verbally. Participants who were under 18 years old also provided light smokers and comparably matched—in terms of demograph-
signed parental consent forms. Participants who qualified as smok- ics and baseline affect ratings—nonsmokers.
ers were then given the opportunity to smoke a research cigarette
provided by the experimenter and were blind to the nicotine
content of the cigarette. Smokers took, on average, 10 min to CO Manipulation Check
smoke their cigarette. To maintain parallelism across sessions for
To ascertain whether smokers and nonsmokers could be differen-
both smokers and nonsmokers, we asked all nonsmokers to simply
tiated on the basis of their expired air CO scores, we conducted a
sit for 10 min during this same time period. Both smokers and
one-way ANOVA on baseline CO levels across the three experimen-
nonsmokers were given access to several emotionally neutral
tal groups. Results indicated a significant effect, F(2, 69) ⫽ 12.48,
magazines.
p ⬍ .001 (␩p2 ⫽ .26). Post hoc testing revealed that whereas partic-
After being instructed to smoke, in an ad libitum manner, as
ipants in both nicotine conditions exhibited comparable baseline COs
much or as little of the cigarette as they wanted, participants lit the
(HY, M ⫽ 8.5, SD ⫽ 1.52; DN, M ⫽ 10.33, SD ⫽ 1.80), smokers
cigarette and then inserted it into a mouthpiece used to assess
evidenced significantly higher COs relative to nonsmokers (M ⫽
smoking topography via the Clinical Research Support System
1.70, SD ⫽ 1.16), t(69) ⫽ ⫺4.52, p ⬍ .001. We then assessed
(Plowshare Technologies, Baltimore, MD; see Kassel et al., 2007,
whether participants in the two nicotine conditions experienced com-
for a detailed analysis of the smoking topography data).2 Just prior
parable boosts in CO as a result of smoking a cigarette. Results of a
to (Time 1) and immediately after smoking the cigarette (Time 2),
2 (HY vs. DN) ⫻ 2 (time: pre- vs. postcigarette CO) repeated
participants completed the PANAS. All participants provided
measures ANOVA revealed a main effect for time, F(1, 44) ⫽
baseline expired air breath samples (with the smokers providing a
135.40, p ⬍ .001 (␩p2 ⫽ .76; indicating that all smokers had, indeed,
second sample immediately after smoking the research cigarette)
inhaled smoke from the cigarette), and a nearly significant trend for
that were assessed for alveolar carbon monoxide (CO) levels with
the Time ⫻ Nicotine condition, F(1, 44) ⫽ 3.66, p ⫽ .06 (␩p2 ⫽ .08),
a Vitalograph EC 50 CO monitor. At the study’s end, participants
suggesting that those who smoked the DN cigarette manifested
were debriefed, compensated, and, if they were smokers, given
slightly greater CO boosts (M ⫽ 9.50, SD ⫽ 4.72) than did those who
referrals to smoking cessation programs.
smoked the HY cigarette (M ⫽ 6.82, SD ⫽ 3.76). Because those
participants who smoked the DN cigarette took more puffs per ciga-
Results rette than those who smoked the HY cigarette (see Kassel et al.,
Sample Characteristics 2007), the marginal group differences in CO observed in the present
study may be attributable, at least in part, to group differences in puffs
Participants who met criteria as smokers were randomized into per cigarette. However, inclusion of puff number as a covariate in this
either the DN (n ⫽ 23) or the HY (n ⫽ 22) experimental group, same analysis yielded little effect on outcome, F(1, 43) ⫽ 3.10, p ⬍
with 27 nonsmokers serving as a comparison group. Participant .09 (␩p2 ⫽ .07).
characteristics can be found in Table 1. The sample was ethnically
diverse, consisting of 36% Caucasians, 27% Asian/Pacific Island-
ers, 16% Hispanics, 12% African Americans, and 9% others. Correlational Analyses
One-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) and chi-square tests
We next proceeded to conduct correlational analyses among the
revealed that the three experimental groups were comparable in
primary study variables (see Table 2). For the purposes of these
terms of age, sex, proportion of community participants to student
analyses only, we created a proportion-of-change score for both
participants, and baseline PA and NA (as assessed with the
PA and NA, respectively, by subtracting Time 1 affect scores from
PANAS). Correspondingly, among the smokers only, no differ-
ences emerged between participants in the two nicotine conditions
on years smoked, days smoked per week, cigarettes smoked per 2
As described in Kassel et al. (2007), we assessed several smoking
day, level of nicotine dependence, or perceived harshness and topography indices, including number of puffs per cigarette, puff volume,
strength of the experimental cigarettes, although smokers did rate puff duration, interpuff interval, and maximum flow rate (velocity) per
the HY cigarettes as slightly more pleasant than the DN cigarettes puff. Significant group differences (between the DN and HY cigarettes)
( p ⬍ .05; cf. Juliano & Brandon, 2002). Moreover, as previously emerged for total puff number, total interpuff interval, and total flow rate.
reported (Kassel et al., 2007), those who smoked the DN cigarette However, the latter two indexes reflect the more important differences
observed in total puff number. Hence, only puff number is incorporated
took more puffs per cigarette relative to those in the HY nicotine
into the present article. Also, 10 additional participants are included in the
condition ( p ⬍ .05, ␩p2 ⫽ .11).3 Both nicotine groups evidenced
analyses described herein.
similar scores on the NAM and BR smoking expectancy subscales. 3
We report partial eta-squared effect size estimates to document the
Taken together, then, our sample was made up of smokers who magnitude of theoretically and/or clinically meaningful effects—␩p2 ⫽
smoked an average of 20 cigarettes a week and had been smoking SSeffect/(SSeffect ⫹ SSerror); they yield estimates comparable to a squared
for just over 2 years. Their scores on the MFTQ were also partial correlation from multiple regression models. Cohen (1992) has
indicative of very low levels of nicotine dependence (M ⫽ 2.15, defined squared partial correlations of .02, .15, and .35 as small, medium,
SD ⫽ 0.33). Finally, analyses revealed no differences among the and large effects, respectively.
ADOLESCENT SMOKING 547

Table 1
Smoking and Demographic Characteristics of the Sample

HY nicotine group DN nicotine group


Nonsmokers (n ⫽ 27) (n ⫽ 22) (n ⫽ 23)

Demographic M SD n M SD n M SD n F(2, 70) t(42)

Age (years) 17.81 0.57 17.50 0.96 17.63 0.97 0.81


Sex (no. female) 10 13 11
Community students 12 13 12
College students 15 9 11
Baseline PA (PANAS) 28.33 7.99 27.00 6.91 27.30 6.91 0.23
Baseline NA (PANAS) 13.81 3.28 14.36 3.67 14.75 5.17 0.72
Nicotine dependence 2.22 0.38 2.12 0.25 1.02
Craving 25.25 6.05 25.81 6.13 0.31
Days a week smoked 5.78 1.48 5.48 1.75 0.61
Cigarettes a day smoked 3.33 1.11 3.75 1.89 ⫺0.88
Years smoked 2.61 1.19 2.04 1.37 1.51
Last cigarette smoked (hours ago) 6.95 8.91 6.18 8.29 0.29
No. cigarettes smoked on study day 2.00 1.70 1.87 1.87 0.25
Cigarette harshness 2.40 1.03 2.77 0.98 1.28
Cigarette strength 2.57 1.12 2.41 0.73 ⫺0.55
Cigarette pleasantness 3.65 0.65 3.13 0.71 ⫺2.55*
SEQ Negative Affect Management 2.99 0.58 2.78 0.75 1.04
SEQ Boredom Relief 2.74 0.77 2.60 0.91 0.58
Puff no. 11.00 8.65 17.71 11.05 ⫺2.21*

Note. Except as noted, none of the between-groups comparisons were statistically significant. HY ⫽ high yield; DN ⫽ denicotinized; PA ⫽ positive
affect; PANAS ⫽ Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; NA ⫽ negative affect; SEQ ⫽ Smoking Expectancies Questionnaire.
*
p ⬍ .05.

Time 2 scores and dividing by the Time 1 score (see Blumenthal, meaningful ways with other dependence-related constructs. For
Elden, & Flaten, 2004). Several interesting findings emerged. instance, the positive association between the MFTQ and NAM
First, craving was significantly correlated with nicotine depen- may not be particularly surprising, as there is ample reason to
dence (MFTQ), NA management expectancies (NAM), boredom believe that smoking to relieve NA is the most predominant
relief expectancies (BR), CO boost, and change over time in NA. attribution made by smokers (Kassel et al., 2003) and can actually
Moreover, the MFTQ demonstrated significant correlations with serve as a marker of nicotine dependence in itself (Shiffman, 1993;
recency of smoking (the more dependent the participant was, the Wahl et al., 2005). What is of great interest, though, is the
more recently he or she had smoked), number of cigarettes smoked emergence of these associations in such light smokers.
on the study day, and NA management expectancies. Taken to- Interestingly, and consistent with the findings above, we ob-
gether, these findings are compelling and suggest that, even among served that recency of smoking was positively correlated with
these young and light smokers, signs and symptoms of nicotine baseline NA, such that the longer it had been since a participant
dependence (albeit at low levels) were evident and correlated in smoked, the greater was his or her baseline NA. Increases in CO

Table 2
Correlation Matrix of Variables

Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1. Craving —
2. MFTQ .60*** —
3. Cigs smoked today .25 .49*** —
4. Recency of smoking (0.25 hr) ⫺.23 ⫺.40** ⫺.71*** —
5. Baseline PA .25 .15 .18 ⫺.17 —
6. Baseline NA .21 ⫺.02 ⫺.20 .34* .01 —
7. PA change .11 .10 .12 ⫺.14 .14 ⫺.04 —
8. NA change ⫺.38* ⫺.26 .10 ⫺.05 ⫺.06 ⫺.49*** .18 —
9. NAM .37* .32* .16 ⫺.05 .05 .02 .05 ⫺.15 —
10. BR .53*** .24 .24 ⫺.05 .10 ⫺.01 .10 .05 .28 —
11. Puff no. ⫺.02 ⫺.06 ⫺.04 .01 .11 .20 ⫺.21 ⫺.04 .07 ⫺.02 —
12. CO boost .40** .27 .33* ⫺.40** .06 ⫺.08 .00 .05 .04 .23 .10 —

Note. MFTQ ⫽ modified Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnare; cigs ⫽ cigarette; PA ⫽ positive affect; NA ⫽ negative affect; NAM ⫽ Negative Affect
Management Expectancies; BR ⫽ Boredom Relief Expectancies; CO ⫽ expired air carbon monoxide.
*
p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01. *** p ⬍ .001.
548 KASSEL ET AL.

attributable to smoking in the lab also proved important, as such (␩p2 ⫽ .59). Thus, regardless of the nicotine content of the ciga-
boosts were related to (a) number of cigarettes smoked on the rette, all smokers experienced significant reductions in PA.
study day (the more cigarettes the participants smoked—perhaps Identical analyses were run on change in NA and resulted in a
indicative of dependence—the greater was the CO increase) and, significant main effect for time, F(1, 69) ⫽ 8.68, p ⫽ .004 (␩p2 ⫽
in a somewhat paradoxical fashion, (b) recency of smoking, such .21), as well as a marginally significant Group ⫻ Time interaction,
that the more recently one had smoked—again, perhaps reflecting F(2, 69) ⫽ 2.92, p ⫽ .06 (␩p2 ⫽ .08; see Figure 2). Thus, averaged
degree of dependence—the greater was his or her CO boost. across all three experimental groups, NA significantly fell from
Finally, it is important to note that baseline NA was related to Time 1 to Time 2. However, post hoc tests of the observed
subsequent change in NA. As was observed with baseline craving, interaction effect revealed that among nonsmokers, there was no
higher levels of initial NA were associated with larger decreases in change in NA, F(1, 26) ⫽ 0.07, p ⫽ .80 (␩p2 ⫽ .00). Whereas
NA over time, suggesting that the greater the baseline NA was, the participants in the HY nicotine condition experienced a significant
more motivated the person was to smoke, resulting in a larger decrease in NA, F(1, 21) ⫽ 8.60, p ⫽ .008 (␩p2 ⫽ .29), those in
precigarette–postcigarette decline in NA. the DN condition evidenced a nearly significant drop in NA, F(1,
All in all, these findings provide reassuring validity data regard- 22) ⫽ 3.90, p ⫽ .06 (␩p2 ⫽ .08).
ing the MFTQ as a measure of nicotine dependence in young Given the observed strong associations between baseline crav-
smokers. Scores on this measure were significantly correlated with ing and smoking expectancies, CO boost, nicotine dependence,
recency of smoking, expectation of negative reinforcement, and and change in NA, we reran these same analyses on change in NA
craving scores and predicted which smokers would experience the among smokers only, including craving as an independent vari-
greatest relief of NA in response to smoking an HY cigarette. able.5 Results revealed that, whereas no significant Group ⫻ Time
Hence, these correlational analyses appear to organize important interaction was evident, F(1, 41) ⫽ 0.00, p ⫽ .99 (␩p2 ⫽ .00), a
data relevant to smoking motivation in these young, light smokers. significant Craving ⫻ Time interaction did emerge, F(1, 41) ⫽
8.57, p ⫽ .006 (␩p2 ⫽ .17), as well as a significant three-way
Analyses Assessing Change in PA and NA (Craving ⫻ Time ⫻ Group) interaction, F(2, 41) ⫽ 4.29, p ⫽ .02
(␩p2 ⫽ .17). Follow-up tests showed that among those who
To determine whether the three experimental groups varied in smoked the HY cigarette, a Craving ⫻ Time interaction was
their change in affect over time, we conducted separate 3 (group: evident, F(1, 19) ⫽ 7.49, p ⬍ .02 (␩p2 ⫽ .28), indicating that
nonsmokers, HY smokers, and DN smokers) ⫻ 2 (time: PANAS 1 greater NA reduction was observed among those who reported
vs. PANAS 2) mixed model ANOVAs on PA and NA, respec- high craving. Among the DN smokers, no such interaction effect
tively.4 Results from the PA ANOVA revealed a significant main was observed, F(1, 21) ⫽ 1.81, p ⬎ .10 (␩p2 ⫽ .11).
effect for time, F(1, 69) ⫽ 33.58, p ⬍ .001 (␩p2 ⫽ .33), as well as In summary, for nonsmokers, their affect (both PA and NA)
a significant Group ⫻ Time interaction, F(2, 69) ⫽ 8.95, p ⬍ .001 tended to be quite stable over the 10-min time period. Smokers,
(␩p2 ⫽ .21). These effects are depicted in Figure 1. Averaged however, experienced significant reductions in PA, regardless of
across all experimental groups, PA decreased significantly from the nicotine content of their cigarette. Smokers in the HY condition
Time 1 to Time 2. Post hoc tests of the observed interaction effect also experienced significant decreases in NA, whereas those who
revealed that the nonsmokers demonstrated no significant change smoked the DN cigarette evidenced a nearly significant drop in
in PA across time, F(1, 26) ⫽ 0.01, p ⫽ .96 (␩p2 ⫽ .00). Smokers NA. However, these observed effects with respect to change in NA
who smoked the HY cigarette, however, experienced a significant were clearly influenced by levels of baseline craving, particularly
decrease in PA, F(1, 21) ⫽ 14.84, p ⫽ .001 (␩p2 ⫽ .41), as did among those who smoked the HY cigarette.
those in the DN nicotine condition, F(1, 22) ⫽ 31.09, p ⬍ .001

The Relation of Nicotine Dependence and Smoking


Expectancies With Change in PA and NA
To further explore the observed findings with respect to
nicotine’s effects on both PA and NA, we proceeded to conduct
several moderation analyses. We dichotomized (via median
splits) the nicotine dependence and smoking expectancy scores
to determine whether the relationship between change in affect
and nicotine yield differed as a function of these potential
moderating variables. Thus, we ran a series of mixed model
ANOVAs in which PA and NA, respectively, at Times 1 and 2
served as the within-subject, repeated measures variables and

4
In addition to the analyses reported subsequently, possible sex differ-
ences were examined through main effects and interactions with the
Figure 1. Change in positive affect (PA) over time as a function of independent variables. None was found.
5
experimental condition. Error bars are standard errors of the mean. To include a continuous independent variable such as craving in the
PANAS ⫽ Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; HY ⫽ high-nicotine- general linear model repeated measures ANOVA procedure in SPSS 14.0,
yield cigarette; DN ⫽ denicotinized cigarette; NS ⫽ nonsmokers. we entered craving as a covariate.
ADOLESCENT SMOKING 549

Regarding the influence of NAM on change in NA, a significant


NAM ⫻ Group (nicotine condition) ⫻ Time interaction was
similarly evident, F(1, 41) ⫽ 10.34, p ⫽ .003 (␩p2 ⫽ .20).
Inspection of Figure 4 reveals that, comparable to what was
observed with the MFTQ analyses, those who scored high on the
NAM (i.e., held strong beliefs that smoking relieves NA) and who
smoked the HY cigarette derived the largest reduction in NA, F(1,
11) ⫽ 17.29, p ⫽ .002 (␩p2 ⫽ .61). Those with high NAM beliefs
who smoked the DN cigarette derived no significant reduction in
NA, F(1, 11) ⫽ 0.16, p ⬎ .70 (␩p2 ⫽ .01). Hence, among those in
the high NAM group, significant differences were observed be-
tween the HY and DN cigarette conditions, F(1, 22) ⫽ 9.95, p ⫽
.005 (␩p2 ⫽ .31). Examination of those participants in the low
NAM group revealed a marginally significant interaction effect,
Figure 2. Change in negative affect (NA) over time as a function of
experimental condition. Error bars are standard errors of the mean. F(1, 19) ⫽ 3.18, p ⬍ .10 (␩p2 ⫽ .14), such that low NAM smokers
PANAS ⫽ Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; HY ⫽ high-nicotine- derived a slightly greater reduction in NA when smoking a DN
yield cigarette; DN ⫽ denicotinized cigarette; NS ⫽ nonsmokers. cigarette, F(1, 10) ⫽ 4.37, p ⬍ .07 (␩p2 ⫽ .30), relative to the HY
cigarette, F(1, 9) ⫽ 0.02, p ⬎ .80 (␩p2 ⫽ .00).

the MFTQ, NAM, and BR served as the respective predictor Discussion


variables (with nicotine condition—i.e., group— entered into all
Despite the fact that adolescent smoking continues to pose a
models). Results for the three PA analyses revealed no signif-
serious public health concern, relatively little is known about the
icant main or interaction effects (all ␩p2s ⬍ .07).
motivational processes governing smoking initiation and subse-
With respect to change in NA, BR was not a significant predic-
quent development of nicotine dependence. Moreover, although a
tor of change, nor did it interact with nicotine condition to predict
burgeoning literature suggests that many adolescents attribute their
change (all ␩p2s ⬍ .02). However, analyses of both the MFTQ and
smoking to its alleged ability to facilitate affect regulation (e.g.,
the NAM yielded significant findings. Regarding the MFTQ, a
Johnson et al., 2003), we are unaware of any previous controlled
significant MFTQ ⫻ Group ⫻ Time interaction emerged, F(1,
laboratory studies that have assessed the acute effects of smoking
41) ⫽ 4.12, p ⬍ .05 (␩p2 ⫽ .09). As can be seen in Figure 3, those
(or nicotine) on emotional response in this vulnerable population.
who scored high on the MFTQ and who smoked the HY cigarette
As such, the current study represents the first to examine the
derived the largest reduction in NA, F(1, 11) ⫽ 13.55, p ⫽ .04
effects of nicotine on both PA and NA in adolescents.
(␩p2 ⫽ .55). By contrast, those with high MFTQ scores who
Our findings provide persuasive evidence that smoking a ciga-
smoked the DN cigarette experienced no significant change in NA,
rette reduces NA in adolescents. It is important to note, however,
F(1, 10) ⫽ 1.12, p ⬎ .30 (␩p2 ⫽ .10). Thus, among those in the
that the extent of that observed reduction appears to be dependent
high MFTQ group, significant differences were detected between
on several key factors, including the nicotine content of the ciga-
the HY and DN cigarette conditions, F(1, 21) ⫽ 4.79, p ⫽ .04
rette, level of dependence, level of baseline craving, and smoking
(␩p2 ⫽ .19). Among those in the low MFTQ group, no differences
expectancies. In light of the fact that demonstration of NA reduc-
in NA across time were observed between the two cigarette con-
tion attributable to smoking and/or nicotine has certainly proven
ditions (␩p2 ⫽ .05).
difficult in older, more dependent smokers (see, e.g., Conklin &

Figure 3. Change in negative affect (NA) examined as a function of Figure 4. Change in negative affect (NA) examined as a function of
nicotine yield (HY ⫽ high-nicotine-yield cigarette; DN ⫽ denicotinized nicotine yield (HY ⫽ high-nicotine-yield cigarette; DN ⫽ denicotinized
cigarette) and dichotomized modified Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire cigarette) and dichotomized NA management (NAM) scores. Error bars are
(MFTQ) scores. Error bars are standard errors of the mean. PANAS ⫽ standard errors of the mean. PANAS ⫽ Positive and Negative Affect
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. Schedule.
550 KASSEL ET AL.

Perkins, 2005; Kassel et al., 2003)—findings that ultimately cast cigarettes very much and that this experience influenced their
doubt on negative reinforcement models of drug motivation—the experience of PA (but less so NA). At odds with such an idea,
present results, obtained in young, relatively light smokers, are however, are findings from correlational analyses revealing no
most compelling. Moreover, our findings strongly suggest that the association between the cigarette ratings (harshness, strength, and
observed NA reduction may be due, in great part, to alleviation of pleasantness) and PA (all rs ⬍ .15).
withdrawal symptomatology. Indeed, implication of withdrawal is Finally, although the PA–NA model views PA and NA as
supported by the association of NA reduction with nicotine depen- separate constructs, it still subsumes a variety of aversive affective
dence and baseline craving. Hence, such observations are impor- states (e.g., anger, contempt, and nervousness) under the rubric of
tant in that they provide evidence that even neophyte, light smok- NA and a host of positive affective states (e.g., excited, enthusi-
ers may experience significant levels of nicotine dependence and astic, and inspired) under the rubric of PA (Watson et al., 1988).
may be more motivated to smoke for withdrawal relief than might The basic emotions viewpoint posits a discrete number of evolu-
once have been supposed (see Kassel, 2000). Finally, it is impor- tionarily derived and biologically based emotional states (e.g.,
tant to note that the extant literature implicating nicotine depen- happiness, sadness, anger, disgust, and fear), in lieu of two over-
dence in young smokers rests almost entirely on questionnaire arching affective dimensions (e.g., Ekman, 1984; Johnson-Laird &
studies, not real-time reactions to smoking under relatively con- Oatley, 1992). Indeed, there is growing evidence in support of
trolled conditions. basic-emotion-specific brain activity (Murphy, Nimmo-Smith, &
Lawrence, 2003; Phan, Wager, Taylor, & Liberzon, 2004). This
On the Specificity of Affect raises the intriguing possibility that diverse drugs of abuse, includ-
ing nicotine, may differentially affect these distinct emotion sub-
The findings are, in many respects, both interesting and per- strates. Therefore, one implication for future drug research is that
plexing. First, we observed that all smokers, regardless of whether it examine the effects of drugs, such as nicotine, on basic emotions
they smoked an HY or a DN nicotine cigarette, experienced rather than limiting its scope to the broader constructs of NA and
reductions in both PA and NA. It is important to note, however, PA (see also Barrett, 2006; Russell, 2003).
that nonsmokers experienced virtually no change in PA and NA
across the same 10-min time period. Thus, the notion that affect The Moderating Role of Nicotine Dependence and
(be it positive or negative) simply drifts downward over time can Smoking Expectancies
probably be ruled out. The findings nonetheless raise the question
of how a drug could reduce both PA and NA simultaneously. As Our findings revealed that regardless of the nicotine content of
noted earlier, some comfort can be derived from influential con- the cigarette smoked, all participants experienced reductions in
ceptualizations of affect asserting that PA and NA are independent both PA and NA. It is true that with respect to NA, those who
constructs, not simply opposite ends of a bipolar continuum smoked the DN cigarette experienced only a nearly significant
(Watson & Tellegen, 1985; see also Perkins, Jetton, & Keenan, decline (␩p2 ⫽ .08) in NA. Nonetheless, we do not want to hold
2003, for an interesting discussion of these issues within the realm too rigid an interpretation of this finding. One implication of these
of nicotine research). Indeed, the observed correlation between results is that the act of smoking a cigarette alone may account for
baseline PA and NA in our study was .01. When viewed in the its effects on emotional response. This may be particularly true for
context of such theoretical and applied levels of analyses, the adolescent smokers, many of whom have perhaps not yet pro-
pattern of findings observed in the present study is perhaps less gressed to nicotine dependence (although see DiFranza et al.,
disconcerting than at first glance. 2000). Correspondingly, the DN cigarette might have acted as an
In sum, regardless of the nicotine content, smoking a cigarette effective placebo (cf. Robinson, Houtsmuller, Moolchan, & Pick-
appeared to reduce both NA and PA in adolescent smokers. The worth, 2000), such that the very belief that one was smoking a
fact that smoking assuaged NA is, indeed, consistent with the normal cigarette yielded changes in affect comparable to those
majority of self-report studies assessing smoking motives among derived from the HY cigarette. (Remember that participants’ rat-
adolescent smokers (e.g., Dozois et al., 1995; Johnson et al., 2005). ings of cigarette strength and harshness did not differ across
As noted earlier, however, there are scant data with respect to nicotine conditions).
determining the influence of smoking and nicotine on PA in adult In addition, as noted earlier, the expectancies one holds for drug
smokers, let alone adolescent smokers. It is conceivable that, in effects can, in themselves, influence the actual derived effects
concert with reports indicating that early experiences with smok- (e.g., Hull & Bond, 1986; Juliano & Brandon, 2002). Therefore,
ing are often perceived as aversive (Eissenberg & Balster, 2000), we were interested in examining whether expectancies related to
certain aspects of smoking may genuinely diminish feelings of PA. nicotine’s effects on affect regulation—in this instance, boredom
It is also worth noting, though, that PA, as measured by the relief and NA management—might yield independent or interac-
PANAS, contains only high-activation (arousing), positively va- tive effects on PA and/or NA. We were similarly interested in
lenced items (Watson et al., 1988). Therefore, it is plausible that assessing whether nicotine dependence also acted as a moderator.
the reduction in PA observed in the present study among those Our thinking was that as individuals approach or have reached a
who smoked either HY or DN cigarettes may, in fact, reflect an state of nicotine dependence, they may be motivated to smoke to
increase in low-arousal aspects of PA (e.g., relaxation or calm). stave off or escape aversive symptoms of nicotine withdrawal
Such an interpretation would certainly be consistent, again, with a (e.g., negative affect; see Baker et al., 2004).
wealth of self-report data suggesting that smokers find smoking Our results showed that none of the assessed independent vari-
relaxing (Dozois et al., 1995; McNeil et al., 1987). Yet another ables influenced PA either directly or in interaction with nicotine
possibility is that the smokers simply did not like the research yield. However, several interesting findings emerged with respect
ADOLESCENT SMOKING 551

to change in NA. Those who both held strong beliefs that smoking Methodological Considerations
facilitates NA management and smoked the HY cigarette experi-
enced the greatest reductions in NA. By contrast, if smokers held Several limitations of the study need to be acknowledged. First,
relatively low expectations that smoking assuages NA and smoked we used a between-subjects design, such that each participant was
exposed to only one of the two nicotine conditions. A more
the HY cigarette, they experienced no meaningful change in NA.
statistically powerful approach would use a repeated measures,
A similar patterning of findings was found for the MFTQ measure.
within-subject design, which ultimately allows participants to act
Those who were more nicotine dependent and who smoked the HY
as their own control. Second, participants were asked to smoke a
cigarette experienced the largest reductions in NA. Last, baseline
research cigarette provided to them by the experimenter in lieu of
craving similarly emerged as a predictor of subsequent reductions
smoking their own typical brand.6 In concert with the artificiality
in NA among those who smoked the HY cigarette.
of the laboratory environment as well as smoking the cigarette
Again, given that, as far as we are aware, this is the first study
through a mouthpiece, this approach might have weakened the
to examine the acute effects of nicotine on self-reported affect in
ecological (external) validity of our findings. Third, those in the
adolescent smokers, these findings certainly need to be replicated.
HY condition rated their cigarettes as more pleasant relative to
Nonetheless, the observation that nicotine dependence, craving,
those who smoked the DN cigarettes. Whereas this finding is
and expectancies of NA management all exerted a significant
certainly not without precedent (Juliano & Brandon, 2002), it does
influence on reducing NA only among those who smoked the HY reflect an inherent limitation in the use of placebo cigarettes.
nicotine cigarette is most interesting. One interpretation is that, on It is also important to note that, because the adolescents in this
the basis of prior experience and learning—limited though it might study were such light smokers, we did not control for time since
have been in this sample of young, light smokers—these adoles- last cigarette. Thus, whereas one would typically attempt to stan-
cents have come to pair the experience of smoking nicotine ciga- dardize potential withdrawal-related motivation to smoke among
rettes with NA reduction (which, in turn, might have helped to heavier, adult smokers, we did not feel it necessary to do so with
shape their NA management expectancies). As such, they bene- this particular sample. Moreover, we feared that were we to ask
fited from this reduction in NA only when they smoked the subjects to smoke on their arrival at the laboratory and then smoke
cigarette containing a pharmacologically active dose of nicotine again, for instance, 50 min later (cf. Kassel & Shiffman, 1997), we
and held high expectancies for NA reduction. would risk inducing aversive reactions to smoking. However, we
Finally, the findings from this study may, perhaps, be most did assess length of deprivation and found it to correlate in mean-
parsimoniously interpreted within a withdrawal-relief framework ingful ways with nicotine dependence (r ⫽ ⫺.40) and baseline NA
(Baker et al., 2004; Kassel et al., 2006). Hence, it is conceivable (r ⫽ .34).
that the smokers were in a state of mild withdrawal when allowed Finally, inclusion of a no-smoke condition for the smokers
to smoke—an idea not inconsistent with their self-reports of crav- would ultimately make comparisons between the nonsmokers and
ing (mean score of 25.5, with a range of 13–37)—and that what smokers more meaningful. As it is, we can only conclude that
was observed (at least with respect to NA) was indicative of whereas the nonsmokers experienced no change in either PA or
withdrawal relief resulting from smoking a cigarette. Indeed, de- NA, the smokers did, regardless of the nicotine content of the
pendence— even low levels, as evidenced in this sample of very cigarette. Thus, although our results may be interpreted as sugges-
light smokers—may be related to the tendency to experience more tive of smoking effects, they do not constitute conclusive evidence.
severe withdrawal, as manifested by craving (Baker et al., 2004). Of course, it is always possible that some other, unmeasured
As such, the presence of craving might be associated with greater variable may differentiate smokers from nonsmokers and account,
NA reduction among such smokers. Consistent with such an in- at least in part, for the observed differences in affective outcome.
terpretation are the findings that craving was significantly corre- Yet some reassurance can be taken from the findings that no
lated with subsequent change in NA (r ⫽ ⫺.38) and that when it differences emerged between the smokers and nonsmokers on
was included as an independent variable, craving predicted down- baseline measures of affect, despite the fact that, at least among
ward shifts in NA. Moreover, baseline NA, although only mod- adults, smokers typically evidence more affective distress in gen-
estly associated with craving, was also significantly correlated eral relative to nonsmokers (Lasser et al., 2000).
with subsequent reduction in NA (r ⫽ ⫺.49).
In addition, it is important to note that the causal pathways Summary
underlying any predictors or moderators of the observed reduction
in NA warrant further empirical scrutiny. For example, we ob- In the first study to assess the acute effects of nicotine on affect
served that NA regulation expectancies moderated the reduction in in adolescent smokers, it was found that smoking a cigarette—
NA but did so only in response to smoking an HY, versus a DN, regardless of the nicotine content—led to reductions in both PA
cigarette. The question arises as to why such expectancies did not and NA. The most marked reductions in NA, however, were
observed among those who smoked the HY cigarette. Nonsmok-
also exert their effects through a placebo (DN) cigarette. Perhaps
ers, who served as a comparison control group, evidenced no
the answer lies in the manifest correlations among NA manage-
change in affect. It is important to note that NA management
ment expectancies, craving, and nicotine dependence. That is, it is
expectancies as well as nicotine dependence emerged as modera-
possible that some of the association of these expectancies with
NA reduction was really due to their shared variance with depen-
dence and consequent withdrawal influences. Further research 6
In fact, anecdotal reports from many of the participants indicated they
with larger numbers of young smokers is clearly needed to eluci- did not have a usual cigarette brand, as they often simply borrowed their
date such motivational pathways. cigarettes from others.
552 KASSEL ET AL.

tors of these effects. Moreover, baseline craving levels were also between depressive symptoms and cigarette smoking in U.S. adoles-
associated with subsequent declines in NA directly attributable to cents. Addiction, 93, 433– 440.
nicotine. Although we acknowledge that conducting laboratory Fagerstrom, K. O. (1978). Measuring degree of physical dependence to
research with adolescent smokers presents daunting pragmatic, tobacco smoking with reference to individualization of treatment. Ad-
ethical, and even legal challenges, such research is nonetheless dictive Behaviors, 3, 235–241.
Ferguson, K. J., Burke, J. A., Becker, S. L., Reimers, T. M., Daughety,
critical to shedding light on the underlying reinforcing mecha-
V. S., & Pomrehn, P. R. (1992). The recruitment of new smokers by
nisms governing adolescent cigarette smoking. This study repre- adolescents. Health Communication, 4, 171–181.
sents a first step in what we hope will prove to be a long line of Hull, J. G., & Bond, C. F. (1986). Social and behavioral consequences of
research aimed at better understanding the affective determinants alcohol consumption and expectancy: A meta-analysis. Psychological
of smoking in this vulnerable population. In sum, a far more Bulletin, 99, 347–360.
systematic and theory-based approach to delineation of smoking– Ikard, F. F., Green, P. E., & Horn, D. (1969). A scale to differentiate
affect relationships is needed (see Kassel et al., 2006). Such an between types of smoking as related to the management of affect.
approach not only will help clarify these complex associations but International Journal of the Addictions, 4, 649 – 659.
will also inform the prevention and intervention efforts that are so Jamner, L. D., Whalen, C. K., Loughlin, S. E., Mermelstein, R. J., Audrain-
sorely required to stem the tide of adolescent smoking. McGovern, J., Krishnan-Sarin, S., et al. (2003). Tobacco use across the
formative years: A road map to developmental vulnerabilities. Nicotine
& Tobacco Research, 5, S71–S87.
References Johnson, J. L., Bottorff, J. L., Moffat, B., Ratner, P. A., Shoveller, J. A., &
Lovato, C. Y. (2003). Tobacco dependence: Adolescents’ perspectives
Baker, T. B., Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., Majeskie, M. R., & Fiore,
on the need to smoke. Social Science & Medicine, 56, 1481–1492.
M. C. (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective process-
Johnson, J. L., Ratner, P. A., Tucker, R. S., Bottorff, J. L., Zumbo, B.,
ing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111, 33–51.
Prkachin, K. M., et al. (2005). Development of a multidimensional
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the
measure of tobacco dependence in adolescence. Addictive Behaviors, 30,
experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10,
501–515.
20 – 46.
Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Oatley, K. (1992). Basic emotions, rationality, and
Blumenthal, T. D., Elden, A., & Flaten, M. A. (2004). A comparison of
folk theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 201–223.
several methods used to quantify prepulse inhibition of eyeblink re-
Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E.
sponding. Psychophysiology, 41, 326 –332.
(2005a). Decline in teen smoking appears to be nearing its end. Re-
Brandon, T. H., Juliano, L. M., & Copeland, A. L. (1999). Expectancies for
trieved December 19, 2005, from http://www.monitoringthefuture.org/
tobacco smoking. In I. Kirsch (Ed.), How expectancies shape experience
pressreleases/05cigpr.pdf
(pp. 263–299). New York: American Psychological Association.
Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E.
Brown, R. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., & Wagner, E. F. (1996).
(2005b). Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use,
Cigarette smoking, major depression, and other psychiatric disorders
1975–2004: Vol. 1. Secondary school students (NIH Publication No.
among adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1602–1610. 05–5727). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse.
Chassin, L., Presson, C. C., Rose, J., & Sherman, S. J. (2007). What is Juliano, L. M., & Brandon, T. H. (2002). Effects of nicotine dose, instruc-
addiction? Age-related differences in the meaning of addiction. Drug tional set, and outcome expectancies on the subjective effects of smok-
and Alcohol Dependence, 87, 30 –38. ing in the presence of a stressor. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111,
Chassin, L., Presson, C. C., Sherman, S. J., Corty, E., & Olshavsky, R. 88 –97.
(1984). Predicting the onset of cigarette smoking in adolescents: A Kassel, J. D. (2000). Are adolescent smokers addicted to nicotine? The
longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 14, 224 –243. suitability of the nicotine dependence construct as applied to adoles-
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. cents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse, 9, 27– 49.
Colby, S. M., Tiffany, S. T., Shiffman, S., & Niaura, R. S. (2000). Are Kassel, J. D., Greenstein, J. E., Evatt, D. P., Wardle, M. C., Yates, M. C.,
adolescent smokers dependent on nicotine? A review of the evidence. Veilleux, J. C., et al. (2007). Smoking topography in response to deni-
Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 59, S83–S95. cotinized and high-yield nicotine cigarettes in adolescent smokers. Jour-
Conklin, C. A., & Perkins, K. A. (2005). Subjective and reinforcing effects nal of Adolescent Health, 40, 54 – 60.
of smoking during negative mood induction. Journal of Abnormal Psy- Kassel, J. D., & Hankin, B. L. (2006). Smoking and depression. In A.
chology, 114, 153–164. Steptoe (Ed.), Depression and physical illness (pp. 321–347). Cam-
Corrigall, W. A., Zack, M., Eissenberg, T., Belsito, L., & Scher, R. (2001). bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Acute subjective and physiological responses to smoking in adolescents. Kassel, J. D., & Shiffman, S. (1992). What can hunger teach us about drug
Addiction, 96, 1409 –1417. craving? A comparative analysis of the two constructs. Advances in
DiFranza, J. R., Rigotti, N., McNeill, A. D., Ockene, J. K., Savageau, J. A., Behaviour Research and Therapy, 14, 141–167.
& St. Cyr, D. (2000). Initial symptoms of nicotine dependence in Kassel, J. D., & Shiffman, S. (1997). Attentional mediation of cigarette
adolescents. Tobacco Control, 9, 313–319. smoking’s effect on anxiety. Health Psychology, 16, 359 –368.
Dozois, D. N., Farrow, J. A., & Miser, A. (1995). Smoking patterns and Kassel, J. D., Stroud, L. R., & Paronis, C. A. (2003). Smoking, stress, and
cessation motivations during adolescence. International Journal of the negative affect: Correlation, causation, and context across stages of
Addictions, 30, 1485–1498. smoking. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 270 –304.
Eissenberg, T., & Balster, R. L. (2000). Initial tobacco use episodes in Kassel, J. D., & Unrod, M. (2000). Smoking, anxiety, and attention:
children and adolescents: Current knowledge, future directions. Drug Support for the role of nicotine in attentionally mediated anxiolysis.
and Alcohol Dependence, 59(Suppl. 1), S41–S60. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 161–166.
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In K. Scherer & Kassel, J. D., Veilleux, J. C., Wardle, M. C., Yates, M. C., Greenstein,
P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 319 –343). Hillsdale, NJ: J. E., Evatt, D. P., et al. (2006). Negative affect and addiction. In M.
Erlbaum. al’Absi (Ed.), Stress and addiction: Biological and psychological mech-
Escobedo, L. G., Reddy, M., & Giovino, G. A. (1998). The relationship anisms (pp. 171–190). New York: Elsevier.
ADOLESCENT SMOKING 553

Kassel, J. D., Weinstein, S., Skitch, S. A., Veilleux, J., & Mermelstein, R. (2000). Current models of nicotine dependence: What is known and
(2005). The development of substance abuse in adolescence. In B. L. what is needed to advance understanding of tobacco etiology among
Hankin & J. R. Abela (Eds.), Development of psychopathology: A youth. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 59, S9 –S22.
vulnerability-stress perspective (pp. 355–384). Thousand Oaks, CA: Shiffman, S. (1993). Assessing smoking patterns and motives. Journal of
Sage. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 732–742.
Khantzian, E. J. (1997). The self-medication hypothesis of substance use Shiffman, S. M., & Jarvik, M. E. (1976). Smoking withdrawal symptoms
disorders: A reconsideration and recent applications. Harvard Review of in two weeks of abstinence. Psychopharmacology, 50, 35–39.
Psychiatry, 4, 231–244. Siegel, S. (1983). Classical conditioning, drug tolerance, and drug depen-
Koval, J. J., Pederson, L. L., Mills, C. A., McGrady, G. A., & Carvajal, dence. In R. G. Smart, F. B. Glaser, Y. Israel, H. Kalant, & R. E. Popham
S. C. (2000). Models of the relationship of stress, depression, and other (Eds.), Research advances in alcohol and drug problems (Vol. 7, pp.
psychosocial factors to smoking behavior: A comparison of a cohort of 207–246). New York: Plenum.
students in Grades 6 and 8. Preventive Medicine, 30, 463– 477. Sonntag, H., Wittchen, H. U., Hofler, M., Kessler, R. C., & Stein, M. B.
Lasser, K., Boyd, J. W., Woolhandler, S., Himmelstein, D. U., McCormick, (2000). Are social fears and DSM–IV social anxiety disorder associated
D., & Bor, D. H. (2000). Smoking and mental illness: A population- with smoking and nicotine dependence in adolescents and young adults?
based prevalence study. Journal of the American Medical Association, European Psychiatry, 15, 67–74.
284, 2606 –2610. Spielberger, C. D. (1986). Psychological determinants of smoking behav-
McKennell, A. C. (1970). Smoking motivation factors. British Journal of ior. In R. D. Tollison (Ed.), Smoking and society: Toward a more
Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 8 –22. balanced assessment (pp. 89 –134). Lexington, MA: Heath.
McNeill, A. D., Jarvis, M. J., & West, R. J. (1987). Subjective effects of Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1996). Initiation and
cigarette smoking in adolescents. Psychopharmacology, 92, 115–117. maintenance of tobacco smoking: Changing personality correlates in
Miller, M., Hemenway, D., & Rimm, E. (2000). Cigarettes and suicide: A adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Applied Social Psychol-
prospective study of 50,000 men. American Journal of Public Health, ogy, 26, 160 –187.
90, 768 –773. Stevens, S. L., Colwell, B., Smith, D. W., Robinson, J., & McMillan, C.
Moolchan, E. T., & Mermelstein, R. (2002). Research on tobacco use (2005). An exploration of self-reported negative affect by adolescents as
among teenagers: Ethical challenges. Journal of Adolescent Health, 30, a reason for smoking: Implications for tobacco prevention and interven-
409 – 417. tion programs. Preventive Medicine, 41, 589 –596.
Murphy, F. C., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Lawrence, A. D. (2003). Functional Stewart, J., de Wit, H., & Eikelboom, R. (1984). Role of unconditioned and
neuroanatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stim-
Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 207–233. ulants. Psychological Review, 91, 251–268.
Nichter, M., Nichter, M., Vuckovic, N., Quintero, G., & Ritenbaugh, C. Wahl, S. K., Turner, L. R., Mermelstein, R. J., & Flay, B. R. (2005).
(1997). Smoking experimentation and initiation among adolescent girls: Adolescents’ smoking expectancies: Psychometric properties and pre-
Qualitative and quantitative findings. Tobacco Control, 6, 285–295. diction of behavior change. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 7, 613– 623.
Oetting, E. R., & Donnermeyer, J. F. (1998). Primary socialization theory: Wang, M. Q., Fitzhugh, E. C., Eddy, J. M., Fu, Q., & Turner, L. (1997).
The etiology of drug use and deviance. I. Substance Use and Misuse, 33, Social influences on adolescents’ smoking progress: A longitudinal
995–1026. analysis. American Journal of Health Behavior, 21, 111–117.
Patton, G. C., Hibbert, M., Rosier, M. J., Carlin, J. B., Caust, J., & Bowes, Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali-
G. (1996). Is smoking associated with depression and anxiety in teen- dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS
agers? American Journal of Public Health, 86, 225–230. scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
Perkins, K. A., Jetton, C., & Keenan, J. (2003). Common factors across Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of
acute subjective effects of nicotine. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 5, mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219 –235.
869 – 875. Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general
Phan, K., Wager, T. D., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. (2004). Functional activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary consider-
neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies ations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social
in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage, 16, 331–348. Psychology, 76, 820 – 838.
Pickworth, W. B., Fant, R. V., Nelson, R. A., Rohrer, M. S., & Henning- Wikler, A. (1973). Dynamics of drug dependence: Implications of a con-
field, J. E. (1999). Pharmacodynamic effects of new denicotinized ditioning theory for research and treatment. Archives of General Psy-
cigarettes. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 1, 357–364. chiatry, 28, 611– 616.
Prokhorov, A. V., De Moor, C., Pallonen, U. E., Hudmon, K. S., Koehly, Wills, T. A., & Shiffman, S. (1985). Coping and substance use: A con-
L., & Hu, S. (2000). Validation of the modified Fagerstrom Tolerance ceptual framework. In S. Shiffman & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Coping and
Questionnaire with salivary cotinine among adolescents. Addictive Be- substance use (pp. 3–24). New York: Academic Press.
haviors, 25, 429 – 433. Wise, R. A. (1998). Drug-activation of brain reward pathways. Drug and
Robinson, M. L., Houtsmuller, E. J., Moolchan, E. T., & Pickworth, W. B. Alcohol Dependence, 51, 13–22.
(2000). Placebo cigarettes in smoking research. Experimental and Clin-
ical Psychopharmacology, 8, 326 –332.
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of Received May 17, 2006
emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–172. Revision received March 5, 2007
Shadel, W. G., Shiffman, R., Niaura, M., Nichter, M., & Abrams, D. B. Accepted March 5, 2007 䡲

You might also like