You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 2006 33; 489–495

Subjective and objective perception of upper incisors


S . W O L F A R T * , A . C . Q U A A S †, S . F R E I T A G ‡, P . K R O P P §, W . - D . G E R B E R § &
M . K E R N * *Department of Prosthodontics, Propaedeutics and Dental Materials, School of Dentistry, Christian-Albrechts
† ‡
University at Kiel, Department of Prosthodontics, School of Dentistry, Albert-Ludwig University, Freiburg, Institute of Medical
§
Informatics and Statistics, Christian-Albrechts University at Kiel and Institute of Medical Psychology, Christian-Albrechts University at
Kiel, Germany

SUMMARY The purpose of this study was to evaluate photographs were taken. No gender dependent
the subjective judgment (SJ) of patients on their differences could be found for the objectively
own dental appearance and to correlate the results measured parameters (median): OM1, 10Æ7 mm;
with objective measurements (OM) of their OM2, 8Æ1 mm; OM3, 0Æ81; OM4, 0Æ79. However,
dentition concerning the appearance of the upper significant correlations between subjective and
incisors. Seventy-five participants (30 men and objective results (SJ1/OM1, SJ2/OM2, SJ3/OM3)
45 women) with normal well-being were included could be shown for men, but not for women. The
in the study. In a questionnaire they judged the maximum of the calculated regression-curves for
appearance of their upper incisors. Furthermore, men reflect ‘golden standard values’ well known
OM were evaluated by the investigator with regard from the literature. The degree of satisfaction con-
to the following points: (i) absolute length of the cerning appearance of anterior incisors in accord-
upper central incisors, (ii) their length exposed ance with golden standard values is higher for men
during laughing, (iii) width-to-length ratio of cen- than for women.
tral incisors and (iv) the proportion between the KEYWORDS: upper incisors, aesthetics, golden propor-
width of the lateral and central incisors. The tion, central incisor, survey, gender
subjective results were registered on visual-ana-
logue scales. For the objective results standardized Accepted for publication 10 September 2005

authors have presented guidelines regarding anterior


Introduction
aesthetics which include recommendations for the
Restoring teeth successfully within the aesthetical zone optimal anterior tooth proportions and tooth lengths
results in a clearly positive effect on patient’s self- (3–8). There are golden standard values concerning
esteem and quality of life (1). A successful therapy the optimal length (10–12 mm) (2, 3) and the width-
depends largely on good interaction between the to-length ratio of the upper central incisors [66–80%
clinician and his or her patient, especially in situations (9) or 75–85% (10), respectively]. Other studies
of complex restorations (2–6). The wishes and needs (11, 12) proposed that dental aesthetic is ideal if the
of patients have to be considered in the same way widths of laterals-to-centrals are in golden proportion
as aesthetic guidelines known from the scientific when the patient is viewed head-on. This ratio is
literature. approximately 1Æ61803:1, i.e. the smaller term is
Dental appearance is influenced by a variety of about 62% of the larger term. Elsewhere these
different factors. These factors determine a harmoni- tooth-to-tooth proportions showed the best results
ously balanced smile which arises as a result of the concerning attractive appearance within a range of
ideal interaction of dental and gingival beauty cri- 56–68% (10). A further beauty criterion is the fact
teria. In order to achieve excellent aesthetics several that both the upper anterior teeth and approximately

ª 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2842.2005.01581.x


490 S . W O L F A R T et al.

1 mm of the gingiva are visible when smiling. When neither crowns, fixed partial dentures (FPDs), fillings
>3 mm of gingiva are exposed most of the patients or periodontal problems on their upper anterior teeth.
find the resulting smile less attractive (13). Group B participants had aesthetical problems with
In studies evaluating golden standard values partic- their upper anterior teeth, especially because of
ipants judged the appearance of various dentitions insufficient fillings, an irregular tooth position or
from digitally manipulated photos (10) or from sket- periodontal problems, but they had no prosthodontic
ches (9), but never they analysed their own dentition. restorations on their upper anterior teeth. Group C
Several studies showed that the above mentioned participants had crowns or FPDs on at least one of
aesthetical guidelines differed from the anatomic their upper anterior teeth. Group D participants had
average values in men (14, 15) and some authors removable partial dentures replacing at least the
have reservations about ‘magic numbers’ (14, 16). upper anterior teeth.
With regard to these arguments the question arises Participants were interviewed individually.
whether patients are sensitive to these values when Throughout the survey the investigator’s interest in
they judge their own dental appearance and whether a specific dental feature was not revealed. The survey
the results of this self-assessment are in accordance was performed after the routine check-up visit in a
with golden standard values known from the special- class room at the dental hospital. For help the
ized literature. This might be an important evidence participants could use a mirror to check their dental
for the question whether golden standard values are appearance.
the useful elements that should be used when clini-
cians interact with their patients.
Well-being
It is of further interest to seek an answer to the
question whether there are any gender related differ- To ensure that the participants did not differ notably
ences in the self assessment. For it was shown previ- from the general population in well-being, a long-
ously that dental appearance is more important for established and highly reliable test was used, which
women than for men (17), and women also appeared to contains 28 items (Befindlichkeitsbogen; Beltz Test,
be more oriented to and less satisfied with their Germany) (19, 20). Only participants with normal well-
outward appearance than men (18). being were accepted for the study.
The purpose of this study was to evaluate whether
participants’ SJ of their own dental appearance with
Objective measurements
respect to different standard aesthetic parameters cor-
relate with OM of the patients’ dentitions. Further- For OM two standardized frontal photographs (Dental
more, the results were compared with golden standard Eye III*) were taken perpendicularly to the axes of each
values and anatomic average values, respectively, participant’s upper anterior teeth: (i) lips and teeth
otherwise known from the scientific literature. during strong smiling, (ii) anterior teeth without lips.
Additionally, a tape measure was photographed with
the same magnification. All photographs were taken
Material and methods
with standardized adjustments (magnification 1:1Æ8)
and were digitized with 1200 dpi (Perfection 3200
Participants
Photo,†). The analysis of the photographs was per-
Eighty participants (33 men and 47 women) were formed with Adobe Photoshop (‡) to measure four
selected. They were all patients at the dental hospital parameters of the upper anterior teeth (Fig. 1): length
facility in Kiel (Christian-Albrechts University at Kiel, of the upper central incisors (OM1), length of the upper
Germany). The participants were unpaid volunteers central incisors exposed during laughing (OM2), width-
and took part in the survey during a routine check- to-length ratio of the upper central incisors (OM3) and
up visit. To ensure a heterogeneous group of partic- proportion between the widths of lateral and central
ipants representative of restorative dentistry they
were selected from four different subgroups (20 *Yashica, Tokyo, Japan.
participants in each group): group A participants †
Epson, Tokyo, Japan.

had a complete and healthy dentition. They had Adobe, San Jose, CA, USA.

ª 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 33; 489–495


PERCEPTION OF UPPER INCISORS 491

Statistical analysis

e Statistical tests for gender-dependent differences in


Lip-lin
OM1 results were performed by using the Wilcoxon-rank
W1 W2
OM2 sum test. As appropriate, linear or non-linear regres-
OM4 = w2/w1 sion models were used to evaluate the relationship
between subjective and objective measurements.
OM3 = w1/OM1 Model fitting was analyzed by using coefficients of
determination and F-tests. To determine the reliability
Fig. 1. For objective measurements (OM) the following parame-
of the subjective measurements (SJ1–SJ4), Pearson’s
ters were measured: OM1, length of the upper central incisors;
OM2, length of the upper central incisors exposed during
correlation coefficient between the main evaluation
laughing; OM3, width-to-length ratio of central incisors; OM4, and the second survey was calculated, and a paired t-
proportion between the widths of central and lateral incisors. test of equivalence with a 10% range of agreement
(21) was performed. For all tests the level of signi-
incisors (OM4). The real length of the different lines ficance was set to 5%.
could be calculated using the tape measure. In all cases,
a value for the right side (teeth: 13–11) and left side
Results
(teeth: 21–23) was calculated, as the mean of both
values was used for evaluation.
Participants

Of the original 80 participants 75 participants (94%)


Subjective measurements
showed a normal well being [mean age 47 year
To determine their SJ the participants were asked to standard deviation (17); range 19–79 year; 44 males
answer four questions (SJ1–SJ4) concerning different and 31 females]. They fell into the following profes-
standard aesthetic parameters of their own teeth. The sional categories: 19 pensioners, three housewives,
answers were registered on visual-analogue scales 14 students, 30 employees and nine academics.
(VAS) with a length of 100 mm and the endpoints The other five participants showed a depressive well-
‘don’t like it at all’ and ‘very nice’. These results were being (three men and two women) and were excluded
later transformed to numbers from 0 (‘don’t like it at from the study.
all’) to 100 (‘very nice’). The following questions were
asked in German and were translated for this publica-
Objective and subjective measurements
tion into English:
(SJ1): Do you like the absolute length of your upper The median values of objective and subjective results
anterior teeth? were analysed for men and women and are shown in
(SJ2): Do you like the length of your teeth exposed Table 1. Gender dependent differences (P > 0Æ05) could
during laughing? be shown for none of the groups (OM1–OM4 and SJ1–
(SJ3): Do you like the proportions of your upper SJ4).
anterior teeth? Regression models were fitted to evaluate the corre-
(SJ4): Do you like the harmony of the widths of your lation between OM (OM1–OM4) and SJ (SJ1–SJ4)
upper anterior teeth? (Figs 2–5). A significant relationship between objective
During this main evaluation the participants were and subjective results (Figs 2–4) could be shown for men
not allowed to take any notes, and no information (r2 between 0Æ26 and 0Æ35, P £ 0Æ05), but not for women
about the planned second survey was given. Approxi- (P > 0Æ05). The maxima of the calculated regression
mately after 1 year the SJ test was repeated with the curves indicate the objective values which were assessed
same questions (SJ1–SJ4) and 18 randomly chosen as most attractive by the participants; they are marked by
participants (20% of all participants, 10 males and 8 shadowed boxes in the figures. The maximum is around
females) were asked to answer them. Only patients 12 mm as far as the length of the upper central incisors is
with no changes in their dental status – as compared concerned (Fig. 2). As regards the central incisors during
with the main evaluation – were included. laughing, the results demonstrate that the higher the

ª 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 33; 489–495


492 S . W O L F A R T et al.

Table 1. Median of objective measurements (OM) and subjective judgements (SJ) (n ¼ 75)

OM* SJ‡
Anatomic
Variable Men Women average values† Men Women

Absolute length of upper 10Æ8 mm (9Æ9, 11Æ7) 10Æ6 mm (9Æ9, 11Æ3) 10Æ1–11Æ7 mm 69 (45, 92) 62 (30, 97)
central incisor (OM1 & SJ1)
Length of central incisor exposed 7Æ5 mm (5Æ5, 8Æ3) 8Æ4 mm (6Æ4, 10Æ1) 75–100% of full 69 (46, 94) 66 (35, 98)
during laughing (OM2 & SJ2) length of upper
anterior teeth
Width-to-length ratio of upper 0Æ82 (0Æ76, 0Æ86) 0Æ81 (0Æ73, 0Æ86) 0Æ78–0Æ87 77 (52, 99) 72 (46, 98)
central incisor (OM3 & SJ3)
Proportion between width of lateral 0Æ82 (0Æ70, 0Æ91) 0Æ79 (0Æ69, 0Æ90) 0Æ78–0Æ85 68 (44, 96) 68 (44, 93)
and central incisor (OM4 & SJ4)

*On standardized photographs aesthetic parameters were measured.



Anatomic average values known from the literature (14, 15, 23).

Visual-analogue scales (0, don’t like it at all; 100, very nice). Median and 25th and 75th percentile (in brackets) are shown. For none of
the groups a gender dependent difference could be shown (P > 0Æ05, Wilcoxon-rank sum test).

Men Women
100 100

80 80 Fig. 2. Regression analyses to eval-


VAS (units on scale)

uate the relationship between objec-


60 60 tive measurement (OM) (y-axis) and
subjective judgement (SJ) (x-axis)
40 40
concerning OM1/SJ1. The shadowed
box in the figure marked the maxima
20 20
r 2 = 0·26 of the calculated regression curve
Observations
P = 0·017 P = n.s. and indicates the objective values
0 0 Regression-curve
6 8 10 12 14 16 6 8 10 12 14 16 which were assessed as most
Absolute length of upper central incisors (mm) attractive.

Men Women
100 100

80
VAS (units on scale)

80

60 60

40 40

Fig. 3. Regression analyses to eval-


20 20
r 2 = 0·28 uate the relationship between objec-
Observations
P = 0·003 P = n.s.
0 Regression (linear) tive measurement (OM) (y-axis) and
0
4 6 8 10 12 4 6 8 10 12 subjective judgement (SJ) (x-axis)
Length of upper central incisors exposed during laughing (mm) concerning OM2/SJ2.

visible part of the teeth was, the better was the quoting
Reproducibility of subjective judgment
(Fig. 3). The maximum width-to-length ratio of the
upper central incisors was around 0Æ75 (Fig. 4). The Concerning the 18 randomly chosen participants, the
maximum proportion of the widths lateral-to-central SJ of the main evaluation and of the second survey
incisors was around 0Æ65 (Fig. 5). were statistically equivalent (P £ 0Æ05, 10% range of

ª 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 33; 489–495


PERCEPTION OF UPPER INCISORS 493

Men Women
100 100

80 80

VAS (units on scale)


Fig. 4. Regression analyses to eval-
uate the relationship between objec- 60 60
tive measurement (OM) (y-axis) and
subjective judgement (SJ) (x-axis) 40 40
concerning OM3/SJ3. The shadowed
box in the figure marked the maxima 20 20
r 2 = 0·35 Observations
of the calculated regression curve
P = 0·003 P = n.s. Regression-curve
and indicates the objective values 0 0
which were assessed as most 0·5 0·6 0·7 0·8 0·9 1·0 1·1 1·2 0·5 0·6 0·7 0·8 0·9 1·0 1·1 1·2

attractive. Width-to-length proportion of upper central incisors

Men Women
100 100

80 80
VAS (units on scale)

Fig. 5. Regression analyses to eval-


uate the relationship between objec- 60 60
tive measurement (OM) (y-axis) and
subjective judgement (SJ) (x-axis) 40 40
concerning OM4/SJ4. The shadowed
box in the figure marked the maxima 20 20
of the calculated regression curve r 2 = 0·18 Observations
P = 0·069 P = n.s
and indicates the objective values Regression-curve
0 0
which were assessed as most 0·5 0·6 0·7 0·8 0·9
0·5 0·6 0·7 0·8 0·9
attractive. Proportion between width of lateral and central incisors

agreement) with Pearson’s correlation coefficients Evaluating the OM Table 1 shows that there was no
ranging from 0Æ81 to 0Æ97 for all questions. Therefore, gender related difference for any of the parameters
it can be concluded that the participants’ SJ about the (OM1–OM4) and that the median values reflected
appearance of upper incisors are consistent. anatomic average values known from the literature:
studies analyzed the anatomical crown form of unre-
stored upper central incisors and found mean width-to-
Discussion
length ratios of 87% for worn and 78% for unworn
Many authors agree that the upper central incisors in central incisors (14) or 85–86% for ‘normal Caucasian
particular are the key determinants in evaluating subjects’ (15). The mean length of a central incisor was
anterior dental aesthetics (3, 4, 11, 22). This can be 11Æ7 mm (14). In natural dentitions, the mean width of
taken as implying that these teeth probably play the the anatomic crowns of upper central incisors is 9Æ1 mm
subconscious key role in people’s judgements concern- and for upper lateral incisors it is 7Æ1 mm (14). Based on
ing dental aesthetics. Thus, the questions SJ1–SJ4 were these values mean tooth-to-tooth proportion of laterals-
all linked to the length or proportion of the upper to-centrals of 78% can be calculated. Previous studies
incisors and were correlated to OM concerning the showed that an average smile (69% of all participants)
central incisor. Moreover, we assumed we would exhibits approximately 75–100% of the full length of
obtain reproducible and stable results when asking the maxillary anterior teeth (23). It can be concluded
questions on this key region for dental appearance. that the OM in the present study did not differ notably
That this was really the case was shown by the fact that from the general population and was not gender
the answers were reproducible in a second survey, related. Furthermore, men and women did not differ
which demonstrated consistency in the aesthetic ratings significantly in their perception concerning their SJ
of participants concerning these questions. (Table 1, SJ1–SJ4).

ª 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 33; 489–495


494 S . W O L F A R T et al.

On this basis, it was expected that the results of the relations is he able to ask the right and useful questions
regression analysis were not gender related. However, a in the dialogue with the patient and lead him or her to
significant correlation (OM1/SJ1, OM2/SJ2, OM3/SJ3) the best possible aesthetic result.
or at least a trend (OM4/SJ4) between objective and Given the limitations of this study the following can
subjective results could only be shown for men, but not be summarized. (i) The OM of this study reflect
for women. The maxima of the regression curves, anatomic average values known from the scientific
which indicate the most attractive values, corresponded literature and concerning these results no gender
to golden standard values known from the literature, dependent difference could be found. (ii) For men a
which were already explained in the introductory statistically significant correlation between OM and SJ
section. Nevertheless, it has to be considered that the could be shown. However, this does not apply to
coefficients of determination lie in a range from 0Æ26 to women. Therefore, when using these single values in
0Æ35, meaning that only about one-third of the vari- the interaction between clinicians and patients, the
ation in the data can be explained by the regression influence of all other aesthetic relevant factors, known
models. from various guidelines on aesthetics has to be taken
Why were the latter results dependent upon gender? into account.
Two arguments seem to be important: first, the
perception of men and women may be different –
References
men seem to be more rational and women more
intuitive and emotional. These general differences 1. Davis LG, Ashworth PD, Spriggs LS. Psychological effects of
between men and women were previously discussed aesthetic dental treatment. J Dent. 1998;26:547–554.
2. Rufenacht CR. Fundamentals of esthetics. 1st edn. Chicago:
in psychological publications (24) and might have
Quintessence; 1990.
influenced the answers and self-assessment concerning 3. Chiche GJ, Pinault A. Esthetics of anterior fixed prosthodon-
the questions SJ1–SJ4. Secondly, women are perhaps tics. 1st ed. Chicago, Quintessence; 1994.
more influenced by the overall aesthetic appearance of 4. Goldstein RE. Change your smile. 3rd edn. Chicago: Quint-
their teeth and their smile than men. If they are not essence; 1997.
5. Magne P, Belser U. Natural oral esthetics. In: Bonded
satisfied with this general view, they cannot judge
porcelain restorations in the anterior dentition: a biomimetic
‘objectively’ a single factor, for example the absolute approach. 1st edn. Chicago: Quintessence; 2002:57–96.
length of the upper incisors. This finding is supported 6. Fradeani M. Esthetic rehabilitation in fixed prosthodontics.
by the results of psychological studies, which showed London: Quintessence; 2004.
that women appeared to be more oriented to and less 7. Magne P, Belser UC. Esthetic improvements and in vitro
satisfied with their outward appearances and that they testing of in-ceram alumina and spinell ceramic. Int J
Prosthodont. 1997;10:459–466.
had higher body awareness and symptom perception
8. Ward DH. Proportional smile design using the recurring
compared with men (18, 25). To summarize, it can be esthetic dental (red) proportion. Dent Clin North Am.
assumed that women might be more influenced by 2001;45:143–154.
emotions and that they have more critical judgement 9. Brisman AS. Esthetics: a comparison of dentists’ and patients’
concerning beauty criteria. The complexity of dental concepts. J Am Dent Assoc. 1980;100:345–352.
10. Wolfart S, Thormann H, Freitag S, Kern M. Assessment of
appearance often can not be determined or analyzed
dental appearance following changes in incisor proportions.
using single parameters. This meets the criticism of Eur J Oral Sci. 2005;113:159–165.
some authors, who were more cautious about the 11. Lombardi RE. The principles of visual perception and their
application of ‘magic numbers’ (14), because they often clinical application to denture esthetics. J Prosthet Dent.
neglect differences between patients, which are com- 1973;29:358–382.
pletely individual (16). 12. Levin EI. Dental esthetics and the golden proportion.
J Prosthet Dent. 1978;40:244–252.
In the interaction between patients and clinicians
13. Allen EP. Use of mucogingival surgical procedures to enhance
isolated values such as the length of the upper anterior esthetics. Dent Clin North Am. 1988;32:307–330.
teeth have to be used with caution as regards the 14. Magne P, Gallucci GO, Belser UC. Anatomic crown width/
aesthetical judgment of dental appearance, because for length ratios of unworn and worn maxillary teeth in white
example the length interacts with the proportion of the subjects. J Prosthet Dent. 2003;89:453–461.
15. Sterrett JD, Oliver T, Robinson F, Fortson W, Knaak B, Russell
tooth, the tooth-to-tooth proportions and the incisal
CM. Width/length ratios of normal clinical crowns of the
line (5, 6). Only when the clinician understands these

ª 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 33; 489–495


PERCEPTION OF UPPER INCISORS 495

maxillary anterior dentition in man. J Clin Periodontol. 21. Berger D, Roger L, Hus X, Jason C. Bioequivalence trials,
1999;26:153–157. intersection-union tests, and equivalence confidence sets. Stat
16. Rufenacht CR. Principles of esthetic integration. 1st edn. Sci. 1996;11:283–319.
Chicago: Quintessence; 2000. 22. Rosenstiel SF, Ward DH, Rashid RG. Dentists’ preferences of
17. Carlsson GE, Wagner IV, Odman P et al. An international anterior tooth proportion – a web-based study. J Prosthodont.
comparative multicenter study of assessment of dental 2000;9:123–136.
appearance using computer-aided image manipulation. Int J 23. Tjan AH, Miller GD, The JG. Some esthetic factors in a smile.
Prosthodont. 1998;11:246–254. J Prosthet Dent. 1984;51:24–28.
18. Bekker MHJ, Croon MA, Vermaas S. Inner body and outward 24. Pease A, Pease B. Why men don’t listen & women can’t read
appearance – the relationship between orientation toward maps. London: Pieper; 2001.
outward appearance, body awareness and symptom percep- 25. Roth M. Gender differences in the adolescent’s body image
tion. Pers Individ Dif. 2002;33:213–225. and their relevance for general self-esteem. Prax Kinder-
19. Von Zerssen D, Koeller D-M. Klinische Selbstbeurteilungs- psychol Kinderpsychiatr. 2002;51:150–164.
Skalen (KSB-S) aus dem Muenchener Psychiatrischen Infor-
mations-System. Die Befindlichkeitsskala. Weilheim: Beltz;
1976. Correspondence: Dr Stefan Wolfart, Department of Prosthodontics,
20. Baumann U, Von Zerssen D. Klinische Selbstbeurteilungs- Propaedeutics and Dental Materials, School of Dentistry, Christian-
Skalen (KSb-S) (Tests und Untersuchungsmethoden). Diag- Albrechts University, Arnold-Heller-Str. 16, D-24105 Kiel, Germany.
nostica. 1977;23:283–285. E-mail: swolfart@proth.uni-kiel.de

ª 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 33; 489–495

You might also like