You are on page 1of 13

bs_bs_banner

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research doi: 10.1111/jir.12501


1

Contribution of parenting to complex syntax development


in preschool children with developmental delays or typical
development
C. T. Moody,1 B. L. Baker1 & J. Blacher2,1
1 Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
2 Graduate School of Education, University of California, Riverside, CA, USA

Abstract two-way interactions indicated differing effects of


parenting variables for children with or without DD.
Background Despite studies of how parent–child
Conclusions Results have implications for
interactions relate to early child language develop-
understanding of complex syntax acquisition in young
ment, few have examined the continued contribution
children, as well as implications for interventions.
of parenting to more complex language skills through
the preschool years. The current study explored how Keywords complex syntax, developmental delay,
positive and negative parenting behaviours relate to language, parent–child interaction, parenting,
growth in complex syntax learning from child age 3 to preschool
age 4 years, for children with typical development or
developmental delays (DDs). The transactional model of development (Sameroff
Methods Participants were children with or without 2009) asserts that the child and environment interact
DD (N = 60) participating in a longitudinal study of with one another to contribute to individual
development. Parent–child interactions were tran- developmental trajectories. Consistent with this
scribed and coded for parenting domains and child model, parenting is known to play a vital role in
language. Multiple regression analyses were used to promoting positive social–emotional, cognitive and
identify the contribution of parenting to complex adaptive skill outcomes in children (Landry et al.
syntax growth in children with typical development or 2001; Warren & Brady 2007; Fenning & Baker 2012).
DD. Additionally, among children at risk for poor
Results Analyses supported a final model, outcomes, parenting may be especially important in
F(9,50) = 11.90, P < .001, including a significant fostering developmental resilience (Baker et al. 2007).
three-way interaction between positive parenting be- As the acquisition of productive language is a
haviours, negative parenting behaviours and child milestone of universal importance for all children, this
delay status. This model explained 68.16% of the is a particularly potent outcome to examine.
variance in children’s complex syntax at age 4. Simple Accordingly, extensive literature documents the
effects of parenting behaviours, such as warmth, affect
and responsiveness, on early communication skills
before age 3 in both typical and vulnerable children
Correspondence: Ms Christine T. Moody, Department of
Psychology, University of California, 1285 Franz Hall, Los Angeles, (Hart & Risley 1992; Tamis-LeMonda et al. 2001;
CA 90095-1563, USA (e-mail: christinemoody@ucla.edu). Warren & Brady 2007). Despite this body of research

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
2
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

on parenting and early language development, few and receptive language in typically developing (TD)
studies have examined the continued contribution of toddlers and preschoolers (Keown et al. 2001;
parenting to more complex language skills. As present Pungello et al. 2009). While most studies have
knowledge does suggest that there is considerable supported inverse relationships between negative
variability in child acquisition of complex syntax past parenting behaviours and child outcomes, some
age 3 (Huttenlocher et al. 2002; Vasilyeva et al. 2008; research has indicated that negative parenting
Huttenlocher et al. 2010), it is crucial to examine the behaviours do not contribute independently over and
role of the parent–child interaction in these individual above positive parenting characteristics (Tamis-
differences. LeMonda et al. 2004).

Parenting interaction behaviours Children with developmental delays


Parent–child interactions have been examined Many studies have extended these findings to
thoroughly for specific parenting behaviours that may vulnerable populations, including children with a
promote child development. Although these variety of developmental disabilities (Siller & Sigman
behaviours have been given many labels, they 2002; Warren & Brady 2007; Warren et al. 2010). For
frequently measure similar constructs: responsiveness example, scaffolding and synchronicity have been
(e.g. contingent responding to the child’s linked to gains in language skills on standardised
communication), synchronicity (e.g. speech and assessments for both children born prematurely and
actions related to the child’s attentional focus), children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Siller
sensitivity (e.g. awareness of and attention to the & Sigman 2002; Dieterich et al. 2006; Siller & Sigman
child’s needs and wants) and scaffolding (e.g. 2008). Similarly, parental linguistic responses to both
provision of developmentally appropriate support). child attentional leads and joint attention in
These positive interaction strategies have been linked minimally verbal preschoolers with ASD were value-
to many positive outcomes before age 2, including age added predictors of later expressive and receptive
of first words, early vocabulary size, play skills, social– language outcomes (Yoder et al. 2015). Further,
emotional development and cognitive skills (Tamis- research indicates that children with developmental
LeMonda et al. 1996; Tamis-LeMonda et al. 2001; risk may be even more sensitive to variability in
Landry et al. 2006). In general, the research in typical parenting behaviour than TD children (Crnic &
development becomes less robust past age 3; however, Greenberg 1987; Denham et al. 2000; Baker et al.
there appears to be continued support for the positive 2007).
impact of these parent interaction strategies. Jackson However, parents of children with developmental
& Roberts (2001) found that children from more delays (DD) may also be more likely to utilise
responsive homes were more likely to use increasingly negative parenting behaviours than parents of
complex grammatical forms in their speech at ages 3 children with TD (Blacher et al. 2013). These
and 4. Similarly, maternal synchronous behaviours differences may be a result of the child’s interactive
were shown to be associated with child language, style, whereby infants and young children with delays
cognitive and social outcomes at 42 months (Landry evoke different parental responses. Consistent with
et al. 2000). this theory, the behaviour of mothers of infants with
Researchers have also examined interaction Down syndrome showed no differences at child age
strategies that have been associated with poor social, 8 weeks from parents of TD infants but diverged by
behavioural and academic outcomes (Heller & Baker age 20 weeks to be more remote and less sensitive
2000). These negative interaction strategies are often (Slonims & McConachie 2006). Analyses in this
defined as overtly negative, intrusive or controlling study indicated that the behaviour of mothers with
verbal and non-verbal behaviours (e.g. criticism, children with Down syndrome was indeed driven by
unnecessary instructions and interrupting child infant behaviour. Additionally, other research has
actions). When specifically examining language indicated that children with developmental and
outcomes, rates of negative parenting behaviours have language delays have a higher prevalence of behaviour
independently explained variation in both expressive problems, which subsequently predicts increased

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
3
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

parenting stress (Hastings 2002; Horwitz et al. 2003) complex syntax growth from age 3 to age 4. However,
and may also affect parenting behaviour. This we hypothesised that the effect of negative parenting
potentially altered parenting environment presents at age 3 would depend on the amount of positive
additional developmental risk that may amplify the parenting interaction strategies present, such that at
inherent risk in these populations. Expressive and high levels of positive parenting behaviours, negative
receptive language deficits occur across a variety of parenting behaviours would not have a significant
developmental disorders (Rice et al. 2005), which may impact. Conversely, at lower levels of positive
be at least partially explained by impaired intellectual parenting behaviours, higher rates of negative
ability (Cromer 1991). However, Rice et al. (2005) parenting behaviours would correspond to less
show evidence of unique language profiles in various growth in complex syntax. This two-way interaction
developmental disorders, which are unexplained by between positive and negative parenting behaviour
cognitive ability alone. Thus, it is possible that dimensions could explain why some studies have
environmental factors, such as parenting interaction shown independent effects of negative interaction
style, may have differing salience for different elements over and above positive elements, and some
populations and for different components of language have not (Tamis-LeMonda et al. 2004). We also
(e.g. syntax) within each population. expected these relationships to differ by child
developmental status, supporting a significant three-way
The current study interaction. Specifically, it was hypothesised that the
magnitude of the relationships would be larger for
There is ample literature supporting the critical role of
children with DD, similar to prior research (Crnic &
parenting for early childhood language milestones, such
Greenberg 1987; Denham et al. 2000; Baker et al. 2007).
as vocabulary. However, few researchers have looked
beyond child age 3 and examined complex syntax use in
naturalistic setting. Further, the majority of the studies
Methods
have been conducted with either TD children or
children with DDs, which limits insight into potential Participants
differences in the mechanisms of child language
Participants were a randomly selected sample (n = 60)
learning. In the current study, we sought to address these
of families drawn from a larger pool (N = 208) of
gaps by investigating the impact of multiple aspects of
English-speaking families enrolled in a longitudinal
parenting interaction behaviours (e.g. sensitivity, affect,
study of children with or without DD conducted
intrusiveness and engagement) on the development of
across three national sites (see Baker et al. 2002 for a
complex syntax in both TD and DD children.
thorough description of original study procedures). In
the current study’s sample, each of the three sites were
Aims and expectations
represented: University of California, Los Angeles
In preparation for the primary analyses, we planned to (n=29), the University of California, Riverside (n=15),
examine group differences with respect to syntax and Pennsylvania State University (n=16). Selection
development and parenting interaction behaviours. criteria required that the child be between 30 and
Consistent with prior literature (Rice et al. 2005), it was 39 months of age, be ambulatory and not be diagnosed
expected that DD children would show less complex with ASD. Families were recruited from community
syntax than TD children at ages 3 and 4. As parents of service providers and local preschools.
DD children have previously shown lower levels of A subsample (n = 60) was used due to the time-
positive parenting and higher levels of negative parenting intensive nature of transcription and language coding.
than parents of TD children (Slonims & McConachie Random selection was constrained to maintain equal
2006; Blacher et al. 2013), it was hypothesised that these distribution across the DD (n = 30) and TD (n = 30)
patterns would also emerge in the current sample. groups. Families were excluded from the current
Building upon studies demonstrating the effects of study selection if the participating child was
parenting behaviours on early language development, diagnosed with Down syndrome, because of their
we hypothesised that both positive and negative unique syntactic development (Rice et al. 2005), or if
parenting dimensions would also contribute to the child did not reach number of utterances required

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
4
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

Table 1 Demographic data of current study sample of TD children and DD children

TD percentage or DD percentage or
2
mean (SD) mean (SD) t or χ

Gender (% male) 60.00 73.33 1.20


Race (% Caucasian) 70.00 60.00 0.66
Family income (% <$50 000) 40.00 63.33 3.27+
Maternal education level (last grade completed, where 16 would refer to 16.27 (2.36) 14.23 (2.50) 3.24**
graduating from a 4-year university)
BSID-II MDI 108.23 (10.92) 68.03 (10.18) 14.75***

+
p < .10.
*p < .05.
**p < .01.
***p < .001.
BSID-II MDI, Bayley Scales of Infant Development II Mental Development Index; DD, developmental delay; TD, typically developing/typical development.

to obtain a valid Index of Productive Syntax (IPSyn) 10 min of joint problem-solving tasks (e.g. creating a
score (Scarborough 1990). Table 1 shows demographic replica of a lego tower, completing a jigsaw puzzle).
characteristics of the current sample. Families were from For all tasks, parents were provided brief instructions
diverse backgrounds and from a wide socio-economic by a research staff member and then left alone with
range. Overall, there were more male than female their child. For example, during free play, parents
children. On average, mothers of TD children had were instructed to act as though they were in their
achieved significantly higher levels of education [Mgrade own living room and were provided with a variety of
completed = 16.27, standard deviation (SD) = 2.36] than toys (e.g. ball, car wash, potatoheads). The
mothers of DD children (Mgrade completed = 14.23, procedures of this lab visit were repeated
SD = 2.50), t(58) = 3.24, P = 0.002. There were no other approximately 1 year later, when the child was 4 years
significant group demographic differences. Two old, with slight modifications to make the materials
children in the DD group were diagnosed with cerebral and tasks developmentally appropriate.
palsy; the rest had undifferentiated DDs.

Child transcription
Procedures
Child language during the videotaped mother–child
The participating universities’ institutional review interaction was transcribed by trained research
boards approved all study procedures. All assistants using Computerized Language Analysis
assessments were conducted by trained doctoral software and Child Language Database Exchange
students or research staff. At intake, child age 3, early System transcription conventions (MacWhinney
cognitive development was assessed in the home 2014). The following activities were transcribed, in
using the Mental Development Index (MDI) on the the listed order, until the number of spontaneous
Bayley Scales of Infant Development II (Bayley 1993). utterances required to assign a valid IPSyn score was
Children were classified as TD if they demonstrated reached: free play, clean-up, snack and problem-
normative cognitive development (MDI >85) or as solving tasks 1–3. The majority of children reached
DD if they demonstrated delayed cognitive this benchmark within the first three activities,
development (MDI <85). A classification of equivalent to 18 min of interaction, at age 3 (26 of the
intellectual disability was not given at this age. TD children and 24 of the DD children) and 4 (25 of
Following intake, families were scheduled for a the TD children and 26 of the DD children). The first
laboratory visit, which included a videotaped mother– author reviewed all transcripts to ensure accuracy.
child interaction, consisting of several parts: 10 min of Twenty percent of final transcripts were subsequently
free play, 3 min of clean-up, 5 min of snack and randomly selected for reliability checks by a research

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
5
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

assistant trained in transcription who had not these Likert ratings were converted into z-scores and
contributed to any of the final transcriptions. summed to form the two composites.
Reliability was calculated by dividing the number of
utterances in which the reliability judge agreed exactly
Child complex syntax
with the final transcript by total number of utterances.
Average reliability was 98.6%, with no one transcript As mean length of utterance (MLU) becomes less
below 92.8% agreement. sensitive to growth past age 3 and may overestimate
the language complexity of children with DD
Measures (Scarborough et al. 1991), the IPSyn (Scarborough
1990) was used to measure child complex syntax use.
Parenting interaction strategies
The IPSyn is a reliable measure of syntactic growth in
Global ratings of both positive and negative the preschool years that has been used with both TD
interaction features were coded at child age 3 using and DD children (Scarborough et al. 1991). An IPSyn
the Parent–Child Interaction Rating Scale (Belsky score is obtained by analysing a 100-utterance sample
et al. 1995). The Parent-Child Interaction Rating of non-imitative language for 60 unique syntactic
System measures six dimensions of parenting (Table 2), structures. For each syntactic structure measured, the
which create two composite scores, positive parenting child can receive up to 2 points (i.e. 0 points for no
and negative parenting, through a previously derived observed use, 1 point for one exemplar and 2 points
factor structure (Fenning et al. 2007). The positive for two or more exemplars). Thus, the highest
parenting factor score consisted of positive possible IPSyn score is 120. During measure
affect + sensitivity + stimulation of development, syntactic structures that showed
cognition ! detachment. The negative parenting factor predictable developmental progression were selected.
score consisted of intrusiveness + negative affect. Coding Therefore, a higher IPSyn score indicates a more
teams rated each mother in each dimension, in each developmentally advanced and diverse
interaction activity, using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not at morphosyntactic repertoire but does not necessarily
all characteristic, 5 = highly characteristic). Research correspond to a specific level of syntactic
assistants were trained until reliability was established, development, as the same score can be achieved with
defined as reaching a criterion over 70% exact agreement different items. Each child’s language was IPSyn
and 95% agreement within one scale point with the scored at ages 3 and 4 from the transcripts produced
criterion coder. Once reliable, two research assistants during the parent–child interaction. Coding teams
were paired to code the mother–child interactions as a consisted of two trained coders who established
team. Reliability was collected for 30% of the tapes. reliability in IPSyn coding by meeting 90% item
Kappa for inter-rater reliability was 0.71 (range = 0.68– agreement with the first author master coder on three
0.77), which is considered acceptable. Once collected, transcripts in a row. Twenty percent of transcripts

Table 2 Description of the six dimensions of parenting interactions coded in the PCIRS

Parenting interaction dimension Description

Positive affect Expression of positive regard, either verbal or non-verbal (e.g. smiling and praising)
Sensitivity Ability of parent to respond appropriately and consistently to the child’s needs
in a developmentally appropriate way, the extent to which parent is ‘child centred’
Stimulation of cognition Attempts to foster cognitive development (e.g. academic skills such as numbers
or letters and problem solving)
Detachment (reverse coded in analysis) Passivity or being unaware of child (e.g. mother reading magazine while child plays)
Negative affect Expression of negative regard, either verbal or non-verbal (e.g. scolding, tone of
voice and facial expressions)
Intrusiveness Imposition of adult agenda on child activities and overstimulation

PCIRS, Parent Child Interaction Rating System.

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
6
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

were then randomly selected to be coded twice. parenting behaviours on complex syntax growth while
Reliability was calculated as the percent item holding positive parenting behaviours constant at the
agreement with a master IPSyn scoresheet created mean, as well as ±1 SD.
through consensus. Average reliability was 97.4%,
with no transcripts falling below 91.7% agreement.
Results
Child complex syntax
Analysis plan
As expected, data revealed that as children age, their
Prior to exploring the three-way interaction of primary
language becomes more syntactically complex. Using
interest (delay status × positive parenting × negative
the complete sample, children at age 4 received
parenting), paired samples t-tests were used to confirm
significantly greater IPSyn scores (M = 67.7,
that there was indeed change in complex syntax from
SD = 13.7) than at age 3 (M = 52.0, SD = 17.9),
age 3 to age 4 for both TD and DD children, which
t(59) = 10.24, p < .001. Further, within each group,
provided the foundation for the regression analyses.
significant complex syntax growth from age 3 to age 4
One-way and repeated measures analyses of vaiance
occurred. TD children received significantly higher
(ANOVAs) were conducted to analyse TD/DD
IPSyn scores at age 4 (M = 75.9, SD = 8.0) than at
differences in complex syntax use at both age 3 and age
age 3 (M = 64.1, SD = 9.2), t(29) = 7.32, p < .001.
4, as well as differences in rates of change in IPSyn
Children with DDs also received significantly higher
scores from age 3 to age 4. With respect to parenting
IPSyn scores at age 4 (M = 59.4, SD = 13.4) than at
behaviours, one-way ANOVAs were used to examine
age 3 (M = 39.9, SD = 16.3), t(29) = 8.02, p < .001.1
group differences in the positive and negative parenting
Finally, the repeated measures ANOVA revealed a
factor scores. Analyses of covariance were also
significant time by delay status interaction,
conducted to parcel out any contribution of maternal
F(1,58) = 6.85, p = .011, suggesting that, on average,
education to parenting behaviour.
DD children showed faster growth in complex syntax
To address the primary hypothesis, a four-step
from child age 3 to age 4 than did TD children.
hierarchal regression was used. The outcome variable
With respect to group differences present at a
used was child IPSyn score at age 4. In the first step of
singular time point (Table 3), children with DD used
the analyses, child IPSyn score at age 3 was entered,
significantly less complex language than TD children
to control for baseline individual differences and
at both age 3, F(1,58) = 50.25, p < .001, and age 4,
isolate the effects of parenting on change in complex
F(1,58) = 33.88, p < .001. Although the assumption of
syntax from age 3 to age 4. Mother’s educational
homogeneity of variances was violated at both age 3
achievement was also entered as a covariate given
and age 4, the F-test is known to be robust against
significant group differences. In the second step, the
inequality of variances when sample sizes are equal
three variables of interest were entered: child delay
(Glass et al. 1972), as in these analyses.
status, positive parenting factor score and negative
parenting factor score. The third step included all
possible two-way interactions between the variables of Parenting interaction strategies
interest. The final step included the three-way Overall, parents used a range of both negative and
interaction of delay status, the positive parenting positive parenting interaction strategies. The average
factor score and the negative parenting factor score. positive parenting unstandardised factor score,
As the primary interest in this study was the three-way
interaction, all main effects and two-way interactions 1
It is difficult to qualify exactly what a given numerical IPSyn score
were necessarily included and not evaluated prior to means for an individual child’s syntax development. However, as a
the interpretation of the three-way interaction. To general statement, a score of 40 would usually indicate that a child is
further understand the three-way interaction, simple using simple sentences with one verb phrase (e.g. ‘I do this’), while a
score of 60 would likely support the ability to use two verb phrases in
two-way interactions between positive and negative
one sentence (e.g. ‘I want to go’ and ‘I’ll be the green and you be the
parenting were examined for both groups of children. blue’). Scores around 75 or higher are usually reached through use of
Simple slopes were calculated to probe these simple increasingly complex syntax structures, such as dependent clauses
two-way interactions, which test the effect of negative (e.g. ‘that’s what I told you’ and ‘this is the big one that we have too’).

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
7
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

Table 3 Descriptive statistics of the language and parenting variables of interest by group

TD children Children with DD

Mean (SD) Minimum–maximum Mean (SD) Minimum–maximum F

IPSyn score, age 3 64.10 (9.17) 46–81 39.93 (16.27) 13–72 50.25***
IPSyn score, age 4 75.93 (7.96) 60–93 59.40 (13.37) 30–82 33.88***
Positive parenting 0.63 (3.25) !6.25 to 0.63 !1.23 (2.82) !8.79 to 3.76 5.58*
Negative parenting !1.03 (1.46) !2.41 to 2.93 0.72 (1.61) !2.41 to 4.00 19.58***

Parenting variables collected at age 3. Positive parenting is equivalent to the sum of its respective standardised factor dimensions, which included positive
affect, sensitivity, stimulation of cognition and detachment reverse coded. Negative parenting is equivalent to the sum of its standardised factor dimensions,
which included negative affect and intrusiveness.
*p < .05.
**p < .01.
***p < .001.
DD, developmental delay; IPSyn, Index of Productive Syntax; SD, standard deviation; TD, typical development.

collapsing across DD and TD, was 13.32 (SD = 1.97),


Table 4 Results of multiple regression of child developmental delay
where the range of possible scores was 4–20. The
status, negative parenting and positive parenting at age 3 on child
average negative parenting unstandardised factor
complex syntax at age 4, controlling for child complex syntax at age 3
score across both groups was 3.87 (SD = 1.26), with and mother’s education at age 3
the range of possible scores being 2–10, suggesting a
low base rate of negative interaction strategies in this
Parameter β SE t
sample. When comparing the sum of the standardised
factor scores, results revealed that parents of TD
Constant (intercept) 71.02 2.04 34.88***
children (M = !1.03, SD = 1.46) were significantly IPSyn at age 3 0.45 0.09 5.06***
less negative than parents of DD children (M = 0.72, Maternal education level (grade) 0.65 0.51 1.27
SD = 1.61), F(1,58) = 19.58, p < .001 (Table 3). In Delay status !6.98 3.21 !2.18*
addition, parents of TD children (M = 0.63, Negative parenting 0.75 1.15 0.66
Positive parenting 0.65 0.76 0.86
SD = 3.25) were more likely to use higher levels of
Positive * negative 0.60 0.42 1.42
positive interaction strategies than parents of DD Positive * status 0.48 1.15 0.42
children (M = !1.23, SD = 2.82), F(1,58) = 5.58, Negative * status !2.53 1.78 !1.43
p = .022. After controlling for maternal education, Positive * negative * status !1.65 0.55 !3.00**
parents of DD children remained more likely to use
higher levels of negative interaction behaviours than Delay status: typically developing coded as 0.
*p < .05.
parents of TD children, F(1,57) = 10.16, p = .002.
**p < .01.
However, no differences in positive interaction ***p < .001.
behaviours were observed between the two groups of β, unstandardised regression coefficient; IPSyn, Index of Productive Syntax;
parents after controlling for maternal education, SE, coefficient standard error; t, obtained t-value.
F(1,57) = 1.64, p > .05.

variance in children’s IPSyn scores at age 4; however,


Multiple regression model
the bulk of the explained variance was accounted for
The final model, including the three-way interaction, by prior language at age 3 (R2IPSYN36 = 0.56). The
was highly significant, F(9,50) = 11.90, p < .001. addition of the three-way interaction in the fourth
Table 4 includes the unstandardised beta coefficients model resulted in a significant increase in explained
of each parameter in this model. The full set of variance, Fchange(1,50) = 8.98, p = 0.004. The
variables and interactions explained 68.16% of the significant three-way interaction indicates that

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
8
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

positive and negative parenting behaviours interact with strategies alone were examined. There were no
each other differently for TD and DD children. The significant main effects of positive parenting
simple two-way interactions were graphed for both TD behaviours, t(25) = !0.48, p > .05, or negative
and DD children (Figs 1, 2, respectively) to depict the parenting behaviours, t(25) = 0.85, p > .05. The only
association between negative parenting behaviours and significant predictor of child complex syntax at age 4
IPSyn scores at varying levels of positive parenting was complex syntax scores at age 3, t(28) = 2.84,
interaction strategies in the two groups. p = .008, accounting for 22.39% of the variance.
For TD children (Fig. 1), the simple two-way When examining the relationship between negative
interaction between positive and negative parenting parenting behaviours and complex syntax at various
behaviours was not significant, t(24) = 1.63, p > .05. levels of positive parenting behaviour for DD
Accordingly, each simple slope at each level of children, as seen in Fig. 2, results revealed that the
positive parenting behaviours was also non- simple two-way interaction was significant,
significant. This indicates no relationship is present t(24) = !2.53, p = .018. Thus, for DD children, the
between negative parenting behaviours and complex association between negative parenting behaviours
syntax at 4, while controlling for prior levels of and complex syntax growth from age 3 to age 4 was
complex syntax, at any of the three levels of the dependent on the level of positive parenting
positive parenting factor score. Because the two-way interaction strategies. Simple slopes analyses
interaction was not significant for TD children, main supported a significant negative association between
effects of positive and negative parenting interaction levels of negative parenting interaction elements at

85

80

75
IPSyn score at age 4

70

65

60

55

50

45
Low Average High Figure 1 Simple two-way interaction between positive and
Negative Parenting negative parenting for typically developing children. IPSyn,
Index of Productive Syntax.

85

80

75
IPSyn score at age 4

70

65
Figure 2 Significant simple two-way interaction between
60 positive and negative parenting for children with
developmental delays, t(24) = !2.53, P = 0.018. A
55
significant negative association between levels of negative
50 parenting at age 3 and complex syntax at age 4 was detected
for those developmental delay children whose parents were
45 above average on positive parenting interaction strategies,
Low Average High
t(24) = !2.14, P = 0.043. IPSyn, Index of Productive
Negative Parenting
Syntax.

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
9
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

age 3 and complex syntax at age 4 but only for those TD children. In contrast, for DD children, parenting
DD children whose parents are above average on behaviours, as coded here, remain important. As one
positive parenting interaction strategies, might expect, the most ideal parenting environment for
t(24) = !2.14, p = .043. This suggests that at high this population is one of high positive and low negative
levels of positive parenting behaviours (i.e. one SD interactional elements. However, results indicate that
above the mean), child complex syntax growth high positive parenting behaviours may not be able to
diminishes significantly as negative parenting buffer against negative parenting interactional
behaviours increase. However, at low and average elements, such that in the presence of high negative
levels of positive parenting behaviours, there was no parenting behaviours, the benefits of positive parenting
significant association with negative parenting behaviours are diminished.
interaction elements on complex syntax growth. For TD children, one interpretation of these results
As stated, the earlier results included maternal could be that complex syntax learning is fuelled
education level in the model as a covariate. However, simply by increased language input, irrespective of the
the model revealed that maternal education level did emotional context. Therefore, language learning at
not explain a significant amount of variance over and preschool age may be founded on basic statistical
above child complex syntax at age 3. Thus, the model learning, where children notice and generalise the
was also tested without maternal education level. grammatical patterns present in language input,
Overall, the results remained largely similar. rather than occurring through a dynamic interactional
However, without this covariate, the simple two-way process. This hypothesis would suggest that while
interaction between positive and negative parenting positive interaction strategies may be essential for
behaviours for TD children reached significance, initial word learning and early milestones (Tamis-
t(24) = 2.06, p = .050. Further, one simple slope LeMonda et al. 1996; Tamis-LeMonda et al. 2001;
approached significance, t(25) = 1.90, p = .069, Landry et al. 2006), they are not the sole avenue
indicating that for TD children in high positive towards syntactic learning. That is, syntactic learning
parenting strategy contexts, increases in negative can occur within positive and negative parent–child
parenting interaction elements corresponded to interaction contexts. These differences across periods
enhanced complex syntax growth from age 3 to age 4. of development are face valid, because very young
children may require additional interactional supports
to establish a level of engagement that enables
Discussion
learning. As TD children become older, these
We aimed to elucidate the relationship between interactional supports may not be necessary for
parenting interaction style and the continued children to cue into syntax and grammar rules, given
acquisition of language through the preschool years. that they already have the foundational knowledge
At this time, children move past simply expanding and ability to use language to interact socially.
their vocabulary and begin to learn the grammatical When maternal educational achievement was not
and syntactical rules that govern language use. included in the model, simple slopes suggest that for
Through complex syntax, children begin to TD children, negative parenting interaction
communicate and understand increasingly complex behaviours may actually have a positive benefit to
ideas and thoughts, an essential developmental task. complex syntax acquisition but only if high levels of
Our findings revealed a significant three-way positive parenting interaction behaviours are also
interaction, suggesting that the predictive power of present. This finding was unexpected and
parenting interaction characteristics on complex syntax inconsistent with previous literature, which has found
growth varies for children with TD versus children with negative or null associations between negative
DDs. Although the presence of a significant three-way parenting behaviours and language acquisition.
interaction is consistent with our hypotheses, the nature However, the current analyses offer an increasingly
of the interaction was different than expected. nuanced examination of this relationship within the
Specifically, findings indicate that parenting interaction context of positive parenting behaviours. Further, as
styles, neither positive nor negative, do not significantly both positive and negative parenting constructs
impact complex syntax growth from age 3 to age 4 in contain variables that may be confounded with

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
10
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

increased language input directed at the child (e.g. presence of a significant three-way interaction indicates
stimulation of cognition for positive and intrusiveness that there are indeed differences in how parenting
for negative), it is possible that this result may be a behaviours predict complex syntax growth children
function of increased language input for the child. with TD as compared with DD. Further, although
This result could also be interpreted as consistent results suggest that parenting behaviours at age 3 may
with prior research supporting a divergence model of impact complex syntax growth, particularly for children
emotion understanding development, which posits with DD, the nature of the analyses can not rule out
that TD children benefit when their two parents alternative explanations of the observed relationship
demonstrate different emotional reactions to between parenting behaviours and complex syntax. A
situations (McElwain et al. 2007), suggesting the second limitation is the use of unidirectional analyses,
possible value of using some amount of both positive examining parenting behaviours as a predictor of child
and negative strategies. Of note, however, parents in language. As stated in the transactional theory of
this sample used low overall levels of negative development (Sameroff 2009), there is likely a
parenting behaviours. Thus, it is also possible that bidirectional influence, whereby child language
more frequent and pervasive levels of negative predicts parenting behaviours, but that was not the
interaction strategies would elicit different effects. focus of this study. Similarly, the current study only
In contrast to TD children, these results indicate examined characteristics of mother–child interactions;
that parenting interaction strategies significantly however, at this age, children likely interact with other
predict language outcomes at age 4 for children with caregivers, including fathers, teachers and day-care
DDs. This may be simply a function of the fact that providers. These interactions may also have predictive
children with DD are at an earlier stage of language value for complex syntax learning.
development at age 3 than TD children. This
explanation may suggest that the effects of parenting
Future directions
on language acquisition matters most when children
are acquiring new skills and gradually erodes as To further explore the effect of parenting on complex
children solidify their language knowledge. However, syntax development in children, it would be important
these results are also consistent with prior research to examine the role of parent language input during this
that suggests that environment, including parenting preschool period. Parent language input
context, is especially important for children with high characteristics, such as the number of different words
developmental risk (Baker et al. 2007; Green et al. or MLUs spoken to the child, have been linked to larger
2014). In particular, it appears that interactions that child vocabularies (Tomasello et al. 1986; Huttenlocher
include high levels of positive affect, sensitivity, et al. 1991; Hart & Risley 1995) and more advanced
engagement and cognitive stimulation are especially complex syntax use (Huttenlocher et al. 2002;
beneficial for DD children. However, the advantages Huttenlocher et al. 2010). The limited body of research
conferred by positive parenting interactions are relating to complex syntax is in need of replication in
sharply diminished if high levels of negative parenting TD populations and extension to children with DDs, a
interactional elements (e.g. negative affect, population for which the impact of parental language
intrusiveness and control) are also present. input (e.g. parent MLU and telegraphic speech) has
Comparing this result to TD children, it appears that only preliminary support (Venker et al. 2015; Sandbank
syntax learning for DD children is dependent on the & Yoder 2016). Further, to our knowledge, no studies
emotional context of the interaction. have examined both parent language input and parent
interaction strategies simultaneously. Although past
research has demonstrated the benefits of input that
Limitations
follows a child’s lead (Siller & Sigman 2008), this
Although powered to detect moderate to large effect research could be expanded to account for other
sizes, the current study’s sample size may have limited interactive elements, such as affect and spatial
our ability to detect smaller effects present in the proximity. In the context of the current study’s findings
population. Even so, the age and child status with respect to TD children, examining the role of
differences were consistent and significant, and the input would determine whether amount of parent

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
11
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

language is indeed one of the propelling mechanisms Janet Quan, Yerim Oh, Mary Dizon, Amanda Tran,
behind syntactic growth and confirm that this process is Marco Gomez, Araceli Gonzalez, Tram Nguyen,
robust regardless of the emotional context of the Josefina Flores, and Stephanie Le. Lastly, the authors
parent–child interaction. would like to thank the children and families who
participated and made this research possible.
Implications
As such, the current study has particularly clear
implications for intervention efforts with preschool References
children with DDs and their families. As high levels of
Baker B. L., Blacher J., Crnic K. A. & Edelbrock C. (2002)
negative parenting have particularly deleterious
Behavior problems and parenting stress in families of
effects and parents of children with delays used more three-year-old children with and without developmental
negative parenting behaviours in this sample, delays. American Journal on Mental Retardation 107, 433–
reducing negative interaction elements may be an 44.
important treatment target, rather than just boosting Baker J. K., Fenning R., Crnic K., Baker B. L. & Blacher J.
positive interaction elements. Additionally, as positive (2007) Prediction of social skills in 6-year-old children
with and without developmental delays: contributions of
interaction strategies seem to offer continued benefit
early regulation and maternal scaffolding. American
through the preschool years in this population, these Journal on Mental Retardation 112, 375–91.
results provide support for continued parent Bayley N. (1993) Bayley Scales of Infant Development Second
education regarding how to adapt to their children as Edition: Manual. San Antonio: Psychological
they develop. In particular, constructs such as Corporation.
sensitivity, stimulation of cognition and engagement Belsky J., Crnic K. & Woodworth S. (1995) Personality and
may each require adjustment as children grow older. parenting: exploring the mediating role of transient mood
and daily hassles. Journal of Personality 63, 905–29.
Indeed, the effect of different types of caregiver
Blacher J., Baker B. L. & Kaladjian A. (2013) Syndrome
responsiveness is moderated by developmental
specificity and mother–child interactions: examining
language level in children with ASD (Haebig et al. positive and negative parenting across contexts and time.
2013). The feasibility of such approaches is bolstered Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 43, 761–74.
by prior parent training research, in which parents of Crnic K. A. & Greenberg M. T. (1987) Transactional
children with and without DD have successfully relationships between perceived family style, risk status,
increased contingent responding and enthusiasm, and mother–child interactions in two-year-olds. Journal of
Pediatric Psychology 12, 343–62.
while decreasing criticism (Schuhmann et al. 1998;
Cromer R. F. (1991) Language and Thought in Normal and
Kasari et al. 2010; Kaiser & Roberts 2013; Siller et al.
Handicapped Children. Blackwell, Cambridge MA.
2013; Sanders et al. 2014).
Denham S. A., Workman E., Cole P. M., Weissbrod C.,
Kendziora K. T. & Zahn-Waxler C. (2000) Prediction of
externalizing behavior problems from early to middle
Acknowledgements childhood: the role of parental socialization and emotion
This article utilizes data originally collected by the expression. Development and Psychopathology 12, 23–45.
Dieterich S. E., Assel M. A., Swank P., Smith K. E. &
Collaborative Family Study (CFS), supported by
Landry S. H. (2006) The impact of early maternal verbal
Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child scaffolding and child language abilities on later decoding
Health and Human Development, Grants 34879- and reading comprehension skills. Journal of School
1459 (PIs: K. A. Crnic, B. L. Baker, J. Blacher, C. Psychology 43, 481–94.
Edelbrock). The CFS was conducted at three sites: Fenning R. M. & Baker J. K. (2012) Mother–child interaction
Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, the and resilience in children with early developmental risk.
Journal of Family Psychology 26, 411–20.
University of California, Los Angeles, CA, and the
University of California, Riverside, CA. The authors Fenning R. M., Baker J. K., Baker B. L. & Crnic K. A.
(2007) Parenting children with borderline intellectual
would like to acknowledge Catherine Sandhofer, functioning: a unique risk population. American Journal on
PhD, for her consultation on this work and the Mental Retardation 112, 107–21.
research assistants who contributed to transcription Glass G. V., Peckham P. D. & Sanders J. R. (1972)
and coding, especially Yassaman Erfani, Gina Hana, Consequences of failure to meet assumptions underlying

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
12
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

the fixed effects analyses of variance and covariance. Landry S. H., Smith K. E., Swank P. R., Assel M. A. &
Review of educational research 42, 237–88. Vellet S. (2001) Does early responsive parenting have a
Green S., Caplan B. & Baker B. (2014) Maternal supportive special importance for children’s development or is
and interfering control as predictors of adaptive and social consistency across early childhood necessary?
development in children with and without developmental Developmental Psychology 37, 387–403.
delays. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 58, 691–703. Landry S. H., Smith K. E. & Swank P. R. (2006) Responsive
Haebig E., McDuffie A. & Weismer S. E. (2013) The parenting: establishing early foundations for social,
contribution of two categories of parent verbal communication, and independent problem-solving skills.
responsiveness to later language for toddlers and Developmental Psychology 42, 627–42.
preschoolers on the autism spectrum. American Journal of MacWhinney B. (2014) The CHILDES project: Tools for
Speech-Language Pathology 22, 57–70. analyzing talk, Volume II: The database. New York:
Hart B. & Risley T. R. (1992) American parenting of Psychology Press.
language-learning children: persisting differences in McElwain N. L., Halberstadt A. G. & Volling B. L. (2007)
family–child interactions observed in natural home Mother-and father-reported reactions to children’s
environments. Developmental Psychology 28, 1096–105. negative emotions: Relations to young children’s
Hart B. & Risley T. R. (1995) Meaningful Differences in the emotional understanding and friendship quality. Child
Everyday Experiences of Young Children. Paul H. Brookes Development 78, 1407–25.
Publishing, Baltimore, MD. Pungello E. P., Iruka I. U., Dotterer A. M., Mills-Koonce R.
Hastings R. P. (2002) Parental stress and behaviour problems & Reznick J. S. (2009) The effects of socioeconomic
of children with developmental disability. Journal of status, race, and parenting on language development in
Intellectual and Developmental Disability 27, 149–60. early childhood. Developmental Psychology 45, 544–57.
Heller T. L. & Baker B. L. (2000) Maternal negativity and Rice M. L., Warren S. F. & Betz S. K. (2005) Language
children’s externalizing behavior. Early Education and symptoms of developmental language disorders: an
Development 11, 483–98. overview of autism, Down syndrome, fragile X, specific
Horwitz S. M., Irwin J. R., Briggs-Gowan M. J., Heenan J. M. B., language impairment, and Williams syndrome. Applied
Mendoza J. & Carter A. S. (2003) Language delay in a Psycholinguistics 26, 7–27.
community cohort of young children. Journal of the American Sameroff A. J. (2009) The Transactional Model of
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 42, 932–40. Development: How Children and Contexts Shape Each Other.
Huttenlocher J., Haight W., Bryk A., Selzer M. & Lyons T. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
(1991) Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input Sandbank M. & Yoder P. (2016) The association between
and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48. parental mean length of utterance and language outcomes
Huttenlocher J., Vasilyeva M., Cymerman E. & Levine S. in children with disabilities: a correlational meta-analysis.
(2002) Language input and child syntax. Cognitive American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 25, 240–51.
Psychology 45, 337–74. Sanders M. R., Kirby J. N., Tellegen C. L. & Day J. J. (2014)
Huttenlocher J., Waterfall H., Vasilyeva M., Vevea J. & The Triple P-Positive Parenting Program: a systematic
Hedges L. V. (2010) Sources of variability in children’s review and meta-analysis of a multi-level system of
language growth. Cognitive psychology 61, 343–65. parenting support. Clinical psychology review 34, 337–57.
Jackson S. C. & Roberts J. E. (2001) Complex syntax Scarborough H. S. (1990) Index of productive syntax.
production of African American preschoolers. Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics 11, 1–22.
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44, 1083–96. Scarborough H., Rescorla L., Tager-Flusberg H., Fowler A.
Kaiser A. P. & Roberts M. Y. (2013) Parent-implemented & Sudhalter V. (1991) The relation of utterance length to
enhanced milieu teaching with preschool children who grammatical complexity in normal and language-
have intellectual disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, disordered groups. Applied Psycholinguistics 12, 23–45.
and Hearing Research 56, 295–309. Schuhmann E. M., Foote R. C., Eyberg S. M., Boggs S. R. &
Kasari C., Gulsrud A. C., Wong C., Kwon S. & Locke J. Algina J. (1998) Efficacy of parent–child interaction therapy:
(2010) Randomized controlled caregiver mediated joint interim report of a randomized trial with short-term
engagement intervention for toddlers with autism. Journal maintenance. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 27, 34–45.
of autism and developmental disorders 40, 1045–56. Siller M. & Sigman M. (2002) The behaviors of parents of
Keown L. J., Woodward L. J. & Field J. (2001) Language children with autism predict the subsequent development
development of pre-school children born to teenage of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and
mothers. Infant and Child Development 10, 129–45. Developmental Disorders 32, 77–89.
Landry S. H., Smith K. E., Swank P. R. & Miller-Loncar C. Siller M. & Sigman M. (2008) Modeling longitudinal change
L. (2000) Early maternal and child influences on in the language abilities of children with autism: parent
children’s later independent cognitive and social behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change.
functioning. Child Development 71, 358–75. Developmental psychology 44, 1691–704.

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
13
C. T. Moody, B. L. Baker & J. Blacher • Parenting and complex syntax

Siller M., Hutman T. & Sigman M. (2013) A parent- Vasilyeva M., Waterfall H. & Huttenlocher J. (2008)
mediated intervention to increase responsive parental Emergence of syntax: commonalities and differences
behaviors and child communication in children with ASD: across children. Developmental Science 11, 84–97.
a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Venker C. E., Bolt D. M., Meyer A., Sindberg H., Weismer
Developmental Disorders 43, 540–55. S. E. & Tager-Flusberg H. (2015) Parent telegraphic
Slonims V. & McConachie H. (2006) Analysis of mother– speech use and spoken language in preschoolers with
infant interaction in infants with Down syndrome and ASD. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
typically developing infants. American journal on mental 58, 1733–46.
retardation 111, 273–89. Warren S. F. & Brady N. C. (2007) The role of maternal
Tamis-LeMonda C. S., Bornstein M. H., Baumwell L. & responsivity in the development of children with
Melstein Damast A. (1996) Responsive parenting in the intellectual disabilities. Mental Retardation and
second year: specific influences on children’s language Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 13, 330–8.
and play. Early Development and Parenting 5, 173–83. Warren S. F., Brady N., Sterling A., Fleming K. & Marquis
Tamis-LeMonda C. S., Bornstein M. H. & Baumwell L. J. (2010) Maternal responsivity predicts language
(2001) Maternal responsiveness and children’s development in young children with fragile X syndrome.
achievement of language milestones. Child Development American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental
72, 748–67. Disabilities 115, 54–75.
Tamis-LeMonda C. S., Shannon J. D., Cabrera N. J. & Yoder P., Watson L. R. & Lambert W. (2015) Value-added
Lamb M. E. (2004) Fathers and mothers at play with their predictors of expressive and receptive language growth in
2- and 3-year olds: contributions to language and initially nonverbal preschoolers with autism spectrum
cognitive development. Child Development 75, 1806–20. disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
Tomasello M., Mannle S. & Kruger A. C. (1986) Linguistic 45, 1254–70.
environment of 1- to 2-year-old twins. Developmental
Psychology 22, 169–76. Accepted 1 April 2018

© 2018 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd

You might also like