You are on page 1of 9

c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 4 ) 1 e9

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect
Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex

Special issue: Research report

Emotional detachment in psychopathy:


Involvement of dorsal default-mode connections

Arjun Sethi a,b,*, Sarah Gregory b, Flavio Dell'Acqua a,


Eva Periche Thomas a, Andy Simmons c, Declan G.M. Murphy b,
Sheilagh Hodgins d, Nigel J. Blackwood b,1 and Michael C. Craig a,b,1
a
NatBrainLab, London, UK
b
Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, London, UK
c
NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
d
Department de Psychiatrie, Universite de Montreal, Canada

article info abstract

Article history: Criminal psychopathy is defined by emotional detachment [Psychopathy Checklist e


Received 29 November 2013 Revised (PCL-R) factor 1], and antisocial behaviour (PCL-R factor 2). Previous work has
Reviewed 31 March 2014 associated antisocial behaviour in psychopathy with abnormalities in a ventral temporo-
Revised 3 July 2014 amygdala-orbitofrontal network. However, little is known of the neural correlates of
Accepted 28 July 2014 emotional detachment. Imaging studies have indicated that the ‘default-mode network’
Published online xxx (DMN), and in particular its dorsomedial (medial prefrontal e posterior cingulate)
component, contributes to affective and social processing in healthy individuals.
Keywords: Furthermore, recent work suggests that this network may be implicated in psychopathy.
Psychopathy However, no research has examined the relationship between psychopathy, emotional
Emotional detachment detachment, and the white matter underpinning the DMN. We therefore used diffusion
Default-mode network tensor imaging (DTI) tractography in 13 offenders with psychopathy and 13 non-offenders
Cingulum to investigate the relationship between emotional detachment and the microstructure of
Diffusion MRI white matter connections within the DMN. These included the dorsal cingulum (containing
the medial prefrontal e posterior cingulate connections of the DMN), and the ventral
cingulum (containing the posterior cingulate e medial temporal connections of the DMN).
We found that fractional anisotropy (FA) was reduced in the left dorsal cingulum in the
psychopathy group (p ¼ .024). Moreover, within this group, emotional detachment was
negatively correlated with FA in this tract portion bilaterally (left: r ¼ .61, p ¼ .026; right:
r ¼ .62, p ¼ .023). These results suggest the importance of the dorsal DMN in the emotional
detachment observed in individuals with psychopathy. We propose a ‘dual-network’
model of white matter abnormalities in the disorder, which incorporates these with pre-
vious findings.
© 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

* Corresponding author. NatBrainLab, Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, 16 De Cres-
pigny Park, London, SE5 8AF, UK.
E-mail address: arjun.sethi@kcl.ac.uk (A. Sethi).
1
Joint last authors.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.018
0010-9452/© 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article in press as: Sethi, A., et al., Emotional detachment in psychopathy: Involvement of dorsal default-mode
connections, Cortex (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.018
2 c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 4 ) 1 e9

1. Introduction psychopathic personality. Mounting evidence suggests that


the ‘default-mode’ network (DMN) is linked to psychopathy,
Criminal psychopathy is characterised by antisocial behav- and may be related to emotional detachment in the disorder.
iour and a constellation of affective and interpersonal traits The DMN is a subdivision of the limbic system that has
including callousness, shallow affect, and manipulativeness largely been associated with introspective and self-referent
(R. D. Hare, 1991). These behavioural characteristics have a processing (Gusnard, Akbudak, Shulman, & Raichle, 2001;
significant negative impact on society. For instance, in- Johnson et al., 2006; Kelley et al., 2002). This network con-
dividuals with psychopathy constitute approximately 15e20% sists of a set of regions that are active and functionally
of the prison population, and commit a disproportionate intercorrelated under resting-state conditions (Raichle et al.,
number of violent and recidivistic offences (Hart & Hare, 1997; 2001). These include the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), the
Hemphill, Hare, & Wong, 1998) that cost the USA over $460 medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), the medial temporal lobe
billion a year (Kiehl & Hoffman, 2011). It has been proposed (MTL), and the angular gyrus (Fox et al., 2005; Fransson, 2005;
that psychopathy is a disorder of the ‘paralimbic’ system Raichle et al., 2001; Shulman et al., 1997). These regions are of
(Kiehl, 2006), including structures such as the amygdala and specific interest due to their overlap with areas involved in
ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Blair, 2008). affective processing (Kiehl et al., 2001; Maddock, Garrett, &
Investigation into the neural basis of psychopathy has Buonocore, 2003). This network has also been strongly impli-
been facilitated by the development of in vivo brain imaging cated in social processing (Buckner, Andrews-Hanna, &
and reliable, well-validated, instruments that permit quanti- Schacter, 2008; Vollm et al., 2006) and moral judgement
fication of specific traits and behavioural tendencies. Factor (Greene, Sommerville, Nystrom, Darley, & Cohen, 2001;
analysis of these traits, using the Psychopathy Checklist e Harrison et al., 2008). The DMN is therefore well placed to
Revised (PCL-R), suggests that they can be divided into di- play an important role in the profound emotional detachment
mensions of ‘emotional detachment’ (factor 1) and ‘antisocial in psychopathy.
behaviour’ (factor 2) (Hare, 1991, 2003; Hare et al., 1990; Harpur, The relevance of the DMN to psychopathy is further sup-
Hakstian, & Hare, 1988; Harpur, Hare, & Hakstian, 1989). Factor ported by recent functional MRI studies that have reported
2 antisocial scores have been reported to be negatively abnormal activation and connectivity within this network
correlated with the microstructure of the right uncinate among men with psychopathy (Glenn, Raine, & Schug, 2009;
fasciculus (Craig et al., 2009), a ventral limbic tract connecting Motzkin, et al., 2011; Pujol et al., 2011). Similarly, structural
the anterior temporal cortex and amygdala with orbitofrontal imaging studies of both adults with psychopathy (Boccardi
regions (Catani & Thiebault De Schotten, 2012). The relation- et al., 2011; Ermer, Cope, Nyalakanti, Calhoun, & Kiehl, 2012;
ship between psychopathy/antisocial behaviour and the un- Gregory et al., 2012; de Oliveira-Souza et al., 2008; Yang,
cinate has since been confirmed (Motzkin, Newman, Kiehl, & Raine, Colletti, Toga, & Narr, 2009) and boys with conduct
Koenigs, 2011; Sundram et al., 2012). Moreover, the associa- disorder and callous-unemotional traits (De Brito et al., 2009;
tion between specific regions within this network and psy- Rijsdijsk et al., 2010) have reported abnormal grey matter
chopathy and antisocial behaviour is supported by an volume in DMN regions. Importantly, a preliminary study has
increasing number of neuropsychological (Blair, Colledge, reported that the degree of functional connectivity within a
Murray, & Mitchell, 2001; Budhani & Blair, 2005; Budhani, network containing DMN regions was related to emotional
Richell, & Blair, 2006; Levenston, Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, detachment in individuals with psychopathy (Juarez, Kiehl, &
2000), lesion (Barrash, Tranel, & Anderson, 2000; Blair & Calhoun, 2012). These studies collectively point towards the
Cipolotti, 2000; Damasio, Grabowski, Frank, Galaburda, & importance of the DMN in psychopathy, and perhaps
Damasio, 1994; Harlow, 1993, 1999; Kluver & Bucy, 1997; emotional detachment in particular. However, it is unknown
Saver & Damasio, 1991), stimulation (King, 1961) and in vivo whether (i) previously observed functional differences in
brain imaging studies (Boccardi et al., 2011; Kiehl et al., 2001; psychopathy are associated with abnormalities in the white
Raine, Buchsbaum, & LaCasse, 1997; Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, matter anatomy of this network; (ii) whether any such dif-
LaCasse, & Colletti, 2000; Veit et al., 2002). ferences in this network are related specifically to emotional
These prior studies have been important first steps in un- detachment.
derstanding psychopathy. However, antisocial behaviour is Direct white matter connections between the medial DMN
not specific to psychopathic individuals (Harpur, et al., 1989; regions (Greicius, Supekar, Menon, & Dougherty, 2009) lie
Skeem & Cooke, 2010), and it is emotional detachment (fac- within the cingulum, a long association tract which can be
tor 1) that differentiates psychopathic personality from the subdivided into functionally and anatomically distinct por-
broader diagnosis of Antisocial Personality Disorder. More- tions. For the purposes of this study, we identify two distin-
over, emotional detachment in adults with psychopathy is guishable portions: (i) the dorsal cingulum, connecting the
presumed to reflect a heritable developmental trajectory from PCC to the mPFC, which is related to social and emotional
callous-unemotional traits in childhood (Barry et al., 2000; aspects of cognition; and (ii) the ventral cingulum, connecting
Forsman, Lichtenstein, Andershed, & Larsson, 2008; Frick, PCC to the MTL, which is involved in memory and spatial
Kimonis, Dandreaux, & Farell, 2003; Frick & Viding, 2009; orientation (Catani & Thiebault De Schotten, 2012).
Viding, Blair, Moffitt, & Plomin, 2005; Wootton, Frick, In the current study we used DTI tractography to analyse
Shelton, & Silverthorn, 1997). Investigating the neural corre- the dorsal and ventral cingulum in offenders with psychopa-
lates of factor 1 traits is therefore likely to be of central thy and age- and IQ-matched non-offenders. Based on prior
importance to understanding the neurodevelopment of work, we hypothesised that surrogate indices of

Please cite this article in press as: Sethi, A., et al., Emotional detachment in psychopathy: Involvement of dorsal default-mode
connections, Cortex (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.018
c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 4 ) 1 e9 3

microstructure in these tract portions, and the dorsal segment were required to have their saliva tested for alcohol use, and a
in particular, would be reduced in the psychopathy group and urine test to detect illicit drug use before the scanning session.
that this would be correlated with factor 1 scores. This revealed some offenders tested positive for substances,
despite being requested to refrain from substance use prior to
the scan. A forensic psychologist, and a member of the
2. Methods and materials research team (S.G.), assessed the participants to ascertain
their suitability to enter the scanning environment and
2.1. Ethics and consent adhere to the safety protocol. Participants were reimbursed
for their time at the National Minimum Wage rate.
This study was approved by the Camberwell St Giles NHS
research ethics committee (formerly the Joint South London
2.3. Scanning
and Maudsley and the Institute of Psychiatry Research Ethics
Committee/South East London REC 4) (reference 06/Q0706/87).
Scanning was performed using a GE 1.5T Sigma Excite MRI
Participants were fully informed of the study requirements
Scanner (actively shielded magnetic field gradients: max amp.
and risks and given the opportunity to ask any questions
40 mT m1) using a multi-slice doubly refocused spin echo EPI
before giving informed consent. This consent included
sequence acquisition based on (Jones et al., 2002) using body
authorising members of the research group to access partici-
coil RF transmission and 8 channel head coil NMR reception.
pants' official criminal records.
60 contiguous near axial slices were acquired with
2.5  2.5  2.5 mm voxels. Acquisition had an echo time of
2.2. Participants
101.3 msec, with an effective repetition time of 12e20 RR in-
tervals (using cardiac gating). A maximum diffusion weighting
All participants were right-handed men who spoke English as
of 1300 sec mm1 was used, and 64 diffusion-weighted brain
their first language and had a reading age of above 10 years
volumes with diffusion gradients uniformly distributed in
old. Participants had no history of significant head injury (i.e.,
space were collected at each slice location, as well as 7 vol-
leading to loss of consciousness for an hour or longer), no
umes without diffusion weighting applied.
neurological problems, or any significant visual or hearing
impairment. Patients had no history of claustrophobia or
other contraindications to MRI scanning as assessed by self- 2.4. Analysis
report. Participants were also screened for psychiatric disor-
ders using the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV Data was corrected for eddy currents and motion distortion,
(Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon, & First, 1992), and were excluded and the diffusion tensor was estimated. Whole brain trac-
if they had any lifetime history of Axis 1 major mental ill- tography (step size: .5 mm; FA threshold: .2; angle threshold:
nesses, or any substance use disorders in the previous month. 30 ) was then performed using Euler integration (Basser,
Participants were also administered the Weschler Adult In- Pajevic, Pierpaoli, Duda, & Aldroubi, 2000). Data was pro-
telligence Scale (Weschler, 1997). cessed using ExploreDTI. The dorsal and ventral cingulum
All offenders with psychopathy were recruited via the were identified and dissected on the axial plane (Fig. 1) and
National Probation Service, and had committed and been tract-specific measures were extracted using TrackVis. The
incarcerated for at least one serious violent crime including cingulum was initially defined by one region of interest (ROI)
murder, attempted murder, rape, and serious assault. on the sagittal plane and dissected bilaterally, with an ROI at
Forensic psychologists assessed psychiatric, medical and the midline to exclude callosal fibres. The division between
criminal history by preliminary screening with self-report the dorsal and ventral portions of the cingulum was defined
measures, probation files and probation officer reports. anatomically as the point above the splenium of the corpus
Criminal records were retrieved from the Police National callosum on the midline slice. An exclusion ROI was used at
Database. this slice, to exclude fibres of the ventral cingulum that
The PCL-R (Hare, 1991) was conducted by forensic psychi- continued into the region defined as the dorsal cingulum.
atrists and psychologists. Interviews were videotaped, and Dissections were performed blind to diagnosis by a member of
25% of these were randomly selected and rerated by a second the research team (AS), and then repeated by a second blinded
trained psychiatrist with a reasonable intraclass correlation member independently (EPT).
coefficient (ICC) for total PCL-R scores (.81). A PCL-R score of Tract measures of fractional anisotropy (FA; an indirect
25 was used to define offenders as having psychopathy. The measure of fibre myelination and axonal integrity and orga-
cut-off of 25 has been used in accordance with previous work nisation) and radial diffusivity (an ostensibly more specific
(Craig et al., 2009; Gregory et al., 2012), and based on obser- marker of fibre myelination) were examined for between
vations of cross-cultural differences in psychopathy (Cooke & group differences using one-way ANOVA, and correlations
Michie, 1999). Healthy non-offenders were recruited via with PCL-R measures were assessed using Pearson's product-
bulletin boards in local unemployment offices and commu- moment (two-tailed) correlation in SPSS. To control for vari-
nity websites. These individuals did not meet the PCL-R 25 ables of no interest, we performed an ANCOVA analysis to
criteria for psychopathy, and had never been convicted of a assess difference between the psychopathy and control group.
criminal offence. Dissection reliability was assessed using ICC analyses (two
Participants were encouraged to abstain from all substance way mixed absolute agreement model model) for FA mea-
misuse for a period of two weeks prior to the scan. Participants surements in our tracts of interest.

Please cite this article in press as: Sethi, A., et al., Emotional detachment in psychopathy: Involvement of dorsal default-mode
connections, Cortex (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.018
4 c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 4 ) 1 e9

Fig. 1 e (a) Fractional anisotropy colour maps with overlaid ROI for the dorsal (green) and ventral (blue) cingulum. (b) DTI
tractography reconstruction of the dorsal (green) and ventral (blue) cingulum.

Finally, to assess reliability of these dissections we


3. Results employed ICC analyses (two way mixed absolute agreement
model), which showed high agreement between raters (AS &
We compared 13 offenders with psychopathy (PCL-R ¼ 28 ± 2),
EPT) in all tracts (Left dorsal cingulum FA: ICC ¼ .99, p < .001;
aged 40 ± 10 years, with full scale IQ 90 ± 12, to 13 non-
Left ventral cingulum FA: ICC ¼ .96, p < .001; Right dorsal
offenders (PCL-R ¼ 4 ± 3), aged 34 ± 9 years, with full scale
cingulum FA: ICC ¼ .99, p < .001; Right ventral cingulum FA:
IQ 98 ± 13. The two groups did not significantly differ in age or
ICC ¼ .93, p < .001).
IQ.
One-way ANOVAs revealed that the psychopathy group
had significantly lower FA in the left dorsal cingulum [F (1,
24) ¼ 5.83, p ¼ .024] and ventral cingulum bilaterally [left: F (1, 4. Discussion
24) ¼ 7.60, p ¼ .011; right: F (1, 24) ¼ 4.81, p ¼ .038] (Fig. 2). The
observed FA differences in the left dorsal cingulum FA The results from this study extend our understanding of the
remained significant even after controlling for alcohol, role of the paralimbic system in psychopathy. We suggest a
cocaine and cannabis dependency (as determined by the SCID; model of the disorder where the main diagnostic features are
Table 1) [F (1, 15) ¼ 5.78, p ¼ .030]. FA differences in the ventral at least partly dissociable at the network level. We previously
cingulum were no longer significant at p ¼ .05 [left: F (1, reported that antisocial behaviour (factor 2) in psychopathy is
15) ¼ 3.00, p ¼ .104; right: F (1, 15) ¼ 1.46, p ¼ .245]. Radial associated with abnormalities in the microstructure of a
diffusivity did not differ significantly between the groups in ventral ‘temporo-amygala-orbitofrontal’ network (connected
either tract portion. by the uncinate fasciculus) (Craig et al., 2009). The current
We also report a negative correlation between PCL-R factor study extends that work, and suggests that emotional
1 scores and FA in the left [r (11) ¼ .61, p ¼ .026] and right [r detachment (factor 1) in psychopathy is associated with ab-
(11) ¼ .62, p ¼ .023] dorsal cingulum within the psychopathy normalities in a different neural circuit e the dorsal ‘default-
group. Importantly, there was no relationship between factor mode’ network (Fig. 3). Such a relationship is also supported
2 scores and dorsal cingulum FA, or between either factor and by previous functional imaging studies in individuals with
ventral cingulum FA (Table 2). psychopathy (Glenn et al., 2009; Juarez et al., 2012).

Table 1 e Prevalence of substance use disorders within psychopathy and non-offender groups.

Drug % Dependency Fischer's exact test p-value


Offenders with psychopathy Non-offenders
Alcohol 40% 0% 5.00 .087
Cannabis 30% 10% 1.25 .582
Cocaine 30% 0% 3.53 .211
Stimulants 0% 0% e e
Sedatives 0% 0% e e
Opioids 10% 0% 1.05 1.000
Hallucinogens 0% 0% e e
Other 0% 0% e e

N.B. Data incomplete for three participants from each group.

Please cite this article in press as: Sethi, A., et al., Emotional detachment in psychopathy: Involvement of dorsal default-mode
connections, Cortex (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.018
c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 4 ) 1 e9 5

Fig. 2 e Differences in Fractional Anisotropy (FA) between individuals with psychopathy and non-offenders *p < .05.

Our findings suggest that this relationship is specific to the abnormalities observed in psychopathy from those observed
dorsal component of the DMN. This is consistent with func- in other psychiatric populations. DMN differences have, for
tions associated with this subdivision of the DMN. The mPFC example, been observed in depression (Sheline et al., 2009),
is involved in introspection and social function, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Castellanos &
emotional reflection (Gusnard et al., 2001), and evaluating self- Proal, 2012; Sonuga-Barke & Castellanos, 2007), dementias (Bai
and other-emotional states (Ochsner et al., 2004). The PCC has et al., 2012; Filippi et al., 2013; Greicius, Srivastava, Reiss, &
been observed to be responsive to emotional stimuli (Maddock Menon, 2004), schizophrenia (Koch et al., 2013), and autism
et al., 2003), and is also activated by evaluation of emotional (Assaf et al., 2010). However, the contribution of the DMN has
states (Ochsner et al., 2004). Collectively, these regions are been conceptualised differently in each of these disorders. For
active during moral judgement (Greene et al., 2001; Harrison example, in ADHD, it has been suggested that they reflect a
et al., 2008), where they are understood to reflect ‘emotional difficulty ‘switching’ between default-mode and fronto-
engagement’ with moral dilemmas (Greene & Haidt, 2002; parietal control networks (Castellanos & Proal, 2012; Sonuga-
Greene et al., 2001). Similarly, both have also been observed Barke & Castellanos, 2007). In Alzheimer's disease, dysfunc-
to be active during socio-affective processing [i.e., empathy tion has been reported to be associated with the more ventral
(Vollm et al., 2006)]. Abnormalities in such functions could PCC-MTL component of this network (Greicius et al., 2004),
plausibly mediate emotional detachment in psychopaths. thus disconnecting the medial temporal memory system from
Indeed, this is not only consistent with the nature of the traits other regions.
described by emotional detachment (e.g., lack of guilt, shallow In psychopathy, abnormalities in a dorsal subdivision of
affect), but also with specific socio-affective (Blair, 2005; the DMN associated with introspection and social, moral and
Shamay-Tsoory, Harari, Aharon-Peretz, & Levkovitz, 2010) affective processing appear to contribute to emotional
and moral-affective (Koenigs, Kruepke, Zeier, & Newman, detachment. This is consistent with autistic spectrum condi-
2012) behavioural abnormalities observed in individuals with tions which exhibit a similar relationship to the DMN, with
psychopathy. social deficits related to dorsal mPFC and PCC subnetworks
The selective relationship between emotional detachment (Assaf et al., 2010). Further work is however required to
and the dorsal DMN also partially distinguishes the DMN

Table 2 e Psychopathy checklist revised correlations with


fractional anisotropy in the dorsal and ventral cingulum.

Measure Pearon's r DF p
Factor 1
Left
Dorsal cingulum .61 11 .026*
Ventral cingulum .08 11 .794
Right
Dorsal cingulum .62 11 .023*
Ventral cingulum .01 11 .979
Factor 2
Left
Dorsal cingulum .01 11 .988
Ventral cingulum .30 11 .322
Right
Dorsal cingulum .14 11 .646
Ventral cingulum .43 11 .146 Fig. 3 e The default mode network, connecting the mPFC
and PCC. The temporo-amygdala-orbitofrontal network
*p < .05.
connecting the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and amygdala.

Please cite this article in press as: Sethi, A., et al., Emotional detachment in psychopathy: Involvement of dorsal default-mode
connections, Cortex (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.018
6 c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 4 ) 1 e9

determine the exact contribution of overlapping functional between groups. This suggests that the reported association
anatomical networks to these disorders and others, and how between emotional detachment and FA in the dorsal
differences in distinct sub networks and networkenetwork cingulum cannot simply be explained by these environmental
interactions lead to the array of psychiatric manifestations factors. Nevertheless, future studies would benefit from a
associated with the DMN. Such work is essential for refining control group matched for substance misuse and include
future neuroanatomical models of these disorders. twin, and longitudinal, designs to tease apart putative envi-
The proposed ‘dual network’ model of psychopathy sup- ronmental and genetic influences more clearly.
ports a shift in the prevailing conceptualisation of psychopa- Further studies are also required to overcome other limi-
thy, which has focused on differences within, and between, tations of the current study. This includes the relatively small
the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex (Blair, 2008). One of the sample size, and the need to replicate our findings in a larger
main limitations of this earlier model was that similar dif- sample. The current study may have also benefited from
ferences had been reported in other, non-psychopathic, anti- assessing the connectivity profile of the angular gyrus, which
social populations (Hoptman et al., 2010; Sarkar et al., 2013; contributes to the DMN. Reconstruction of the angular gyrus'
Sundram et al., 2012). The current model can accommodate connections with other DMN regions is limited by the inability
these findings, and provide a putative explanation of the two of the tensor model to resolve crossing fibres (Greicius et al.,
core features of criminal psychopathy. Although it is probable 2009). However, this could potentially be addressed in future
that other regions/networks are relevant to psychopathy studies, as high angular resolution diffusion imaging (HARDI)
(Juarez et al., 2012), findings from this study and others techniques (Tuch et al., 2002), such as spherical deconvolution
strongly suggest the importance of the identified dorsal (Dell'Acqua et al., 2010; Tournier, Calamante, Gadian, &
(Ermer et al., 2012; Glenn et al., 2009; Gregory et al., 2012; Connelly, 2004), continue to develop. Lastly, the proposed
Juarez et al., 2012; Motzkin et al., 2011; de Oliveira-Souza model should be interpreted within the context of its predic-
et al., 2008; Pujol et al., 2011; Yang et al., 2009) and ventral tive limitations. For example, it is probable that there are other
(Blair, 2008; Craig et al., 2009; Motzkin et al., 2011) networks. In regions/networks which also contribute to factor 1 and 2 in
summary, this dual network model suggests a differential psychopaths. In addition, other neurobiological differences
contribution of ventral and dorsal paralimbic sub-networks to may contribute to functional abnormalities that are not
the discrete features of psychopathy. encapsulated by the PCL-R. For example, we also observed
The biological mechanism(s) underpinning these network differences in the ventral cingulum that did not relate to
abnormalities are not yet clear, though they may reflect a either PCL-R factor. This is consistent with our understanding
primary deficit in white matter maturation. FA increases with that this tract is more associated with memory and spatial
age during the first two decades of life, and in some tracts orientation, than emotional and social processing (Catani &
(including the cingulum and uncinate fasciculus) this process Thiebault De Schotten, 2012).
is protracted over a longer period (Lebel, Walker, Leemans,
Phillips, & Beaulieu, 2008). Therefore, reduced FA in psychop-
athy could be related to an aberrant maturational trajectory of 5. Conclusions
these limbic pathways. This is consistent with findings that
boys with conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits The current study, combined with previous work, suggests
exhibit a divergent pattern of white matter development from that it may be possible to fractionate the behavioural pheno-
typically developing controls (De Brito et al., 2009). However, type of psychopathy [that is, emotional detachment (PCL-R
white matter differences could also be secondary to develop- factor 1) and antisocial behaviour (factor 2)] on the network
mental changes within ventral and/or dorsal brain regions. level. Whilst differences in a ventral ‘temporo-amygdala-
Further work is needed to investigate these hypotheses, and to orbitofrontal’ network are related to antisocial behaviour in
characterise the aetiology of any such changes. psychopathy, we report that emotional detachment is related
Twin studies provide a unique method for studying to abnormalities in a dorsal ‘default-mode’ network. Further
development, and may provide clues as to the aetiology of work is required to determine the cause of these differences,
neurobiological differences in psychopathy. For example, and if they predict outcome. Due to the apparent heritability
using this approach, boys with callous-unemotional traits and early-emergence of affective and interpersonal traits, the
have been reported to exhibit heritable grey matter anomalies DMN may represent a more specific target for future studies
within the DMN (Rijsdijsk et al., 2010). This suggests that at aimed at understanding the neurodevelopmental trajectory of
least some differences within the DMN appear to be heritable psychopathy and potential treatments.
in psychopathy. However, it is also possible that the reported
differences in white matter microstructure could be influ-
enced by environmental factors. In particular, drug de- Funding
pendency is known to exert a significant effect on FA (Ashtari
et al., 2009; Harris et al., 2008; Lim, Choi, Pomara, Wolkin, & This research was funded by research grants from: The
Rotrosen, 2002; Pfefferbaum, Rosenbloom, Rohlfing, & Department of Health (the National Forensic Mental Health
Sullivan, 2009; Pfefferbaum & Sullivan, 2005). This potential R&D programme; MRD 12/102); The Ministry of Justice (a DSPD
confound does not appear to have influenced our results, programme grant); The Psychiatry Research Trust; The NIHR
however, as the observed reductions in FA remained signifi- Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley
cant after controlling for group differences in life-time di- NHS Foundation Trust, and Institute of Psychiatry (King's
agnoses of alcohol, cocaine, and cannabis dependence College London).

Please cite this article in press as: Sethi, A., et al., Emotional detachment in psychopathy: Involvement of dorsal default-mode
connections, Cortex (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.018
c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 4 ) 1 e9 7

Blair, R. J., & Cipolotti, L. (2000). Impaired social response reversal.


Conflict of interest A case of ‘acquired sociopathy’. Brain, 123(Pt 6), 1122e1141.
Blair, R. J., Colledge, E., Murray, L., & Mitchell, D. G. (2001). A
None. selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful
expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(6), 491e498.
Human and animal rights Boccardi, M., Frisoni, G. B., Hare, R. D., Cavedo, E., Najt, P.,
Pievani, M., et al. (2011). Cortex and amygdala morphology in
psychopathy. Psychiatry Research, 193(2), 85e92. http://
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.12.013.
work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant na- Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The
tional and institutional committees on human experimenta- brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to
tion and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in disease. Annuals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124,
2008. Informed consent was obtained for all human subjects. 1e38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.011.
Budhani, S., & Blair, R. J. (2005). Response reversal and children
with psychopathic tendencies: success is a function of
salience of contingency change. Journal of Child Psychology and
Acknowledgements Psychiatry, 46(9), 972e981. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-
7610.2004.00398.x.
We gratefully acknowledge the work of Ms. Sam Prior, Ms. Budhani, S., Richell, R. A., & Blair, R. J. (2006). Impaired reversal
but intact acquisition: probabilistic response reversal deficits
Clare Goodwin, Mr. William Wainwright, Mr. Ruben Azevedo,
in adult individuals with psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal
Mr. Francis Vergunst, Ms. Lucy Butler, Ms. Leila Niknejad, Dr. Psychology, 115(3), 552e558. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-
Anna Plodowski, Dr. Philip Baker, Dr. Timothy Rogers, Dr. 843x.115.3.552.
Preethi Chabbra, Dr. Stephen Attard, Dr. Seema Sukhwal, Dr. Castellanos, F. X., & Proal, E. (2012). Large-scale brain systems in
Nathan Kolla, Dr. Paul Wallang, and Dr. Clare Conway in ADHD: beyond the prefrontal-striatal model. Trends in
participant recruitment and assessment. Cognitive Sciences, 16(1), 17e26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.tics.2011.11.007.
Catani, M., & Thiebault de Schotten, M. (2012). Atlas of human brain
connections. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
references Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (1999). Psychopathy across cultures:
North America and Scotland compared. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 108(1), 58e68.
Ashtari, M., Cervellione, K., Cottone, J., Ardekani, B. A., Sevy, S., & Craig, M. C., Catani, M., Deeley, Q., Latham, R., Daly, E.,
Kumra, S. (2009). Diffusion abnormalities in adolescents and Kanaan, R., et al. (2009). Altered connections on the road to
young adults with a history of heavy cannabis use. Journal of psychopathy. Molecular Psychiatry, 14(10), 946e953. http://
Psychiatric Research, 43(3), 189e204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ dx.doi.org/10.1038/mp.2009.40, 907.
j.jpsychires.2008.12.002. Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M., &
Assaf, M., Jagannathan, K., Calhoun, V. D., Miller, L., Damasio, A. R. (1994). The return of Phineas Gage: clues about
Stevens, M. C., Sahl, R., et al. (2010). Abnormal functional the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science, 264(5162),
connectivity of default mode sub-networks in autism 1102e1105.
spectrum disorder patients. NeuroImage, 53(1), 247e256. http:// De Brito, S. A., Mechelli, A., Wilke, M., Laurens, K. R., Jones, A. P.,
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.067. Barker, G. J., et al. (2009). Size matters: increased grey matter
Bai, F., Shi, Y., Yuan, Y., Wang, Y., Yue, C., Teng, Y., et al. (2012). in boys with conduct problems and callous-unemotional
Altered self-referential network in resting-state amnestic type traits. Brain, 132(Pt 4), 843e852. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/
mild cognitive impairment. Cortex, 48(5), 604e613. http:// brain/awp011.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.02.011. Dell'Acqua, F., Scifo, P., Rizzo, G., Catani, M., Simmons, A.,
Barrash, J., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. W. (2000). Acquired Scotti, G., et al. (2010). A modified damped Richardson-Lucy
personality disturbances associated with bilateral damage to algorithm to reduce isotropic background effects in spherical
the ventromedial prefrontal region. Developmental deconvolution. NeuroImage, 49(2), 1446e1458. http://dx.doi.org/
Neuropsychology, 18(3), 355e381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/ 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.033.
S1532694205Barrash. Ermer, E., Cope, L. M., Nyalakanti, P. K., Calhoun, V. D., &
Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., DeShazo, T. M., McCoy, M. G., Ellis, M., & Kiehl, K. A. (2012). Aberrant paralimbic gray matter in criminal
Loney, B. R. (2000). The importance of callous-unemotional psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(3), 649e658.
traits for extending the concept of psychopathy to children. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026371.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(2), 335e340. Filippi, M., Agosta, F., Scola, E., Canu, E., Magnani, G., Marcone, A.,
Basser, P. J., Pajevic, S., Pierpaoli, C., Duda, J., & Aldroubi, A. (2000). et al. (2013). Functional network connectivity in the behavioral
In vivo fiber tractography using DT-MRI data. Journal of variant of frontotemporal dementia. Cortex, 49(9), 2389e2401.
Abnormal Psychology, 44(4), 625e632. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.017.
Blair, R. J. (2005). Responding to the emotions of others: Forsman, M., Lichtenstein, P., Andershed, H., & Larsson, H. (2008).
dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical Genetic effects explain the stability of psychopathic
and psychiatric populations. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(4), personality from mid- to late adolescence. Journal of Abnormal
698e718. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.06.004. Psychology, 117(3), 606e617. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-
Blair, R. J. (2008). The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal 843X.117.3.606.
cortex: functional contributions and dysfunction in Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van
psychopathy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of Essen, D. C., & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is
London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 363(1503), 2557e2565. http:// intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional
dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0027. networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of the Sciences of

Please cite this article in press as: Sethi, A., et al., Emotional detachment in psychopathy: Involvement of dorsal default-mode
connections, Cortex (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.018
8 c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 4 ) 1 e9

the United States of America, 102(27), 9673e9678. http:// cingulum diffusion tensor imaging deficits in alcoholism.
dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504136102. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32(6), 1001e1013.
Fransson, P. (2005). Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00661.x.
fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default Harrison, B. J., Pujol, J., Lo  pez-Sola
 , M., Herna
 ndez-Ribas, R.,
mode of brain function hypothesis. Human Brain Mapping, Deus, J., Ortiz, H., et al. (2008). Consistency and functional
26(1), 15e29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20113. specialization in the default mode brain network. Proceedings
Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., Dandreaux, D. M., & Farell, J. M. (2003). of the National Academy of the Sciences of the United States of
The 4 year stability of psychopathic traits in non-referred America, 105(28), 9781e9786. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/
youth. Behavioural Sciences & the Law, 21(6), 713e736. http:// pnas.0711791105.
dx.doi.org/10.1002/bsl.568. Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1997). Psychopathy: assessment and
Frick, P. J., & Viding, E. (2009). Antisocial behavior from a association with criminal conduct. In D. M. Stoff, J. Breiling, &
developmental psychopathology perspective. Developmental J. D. Maser (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behaviour. New York: John
Psychopathology, 21(4), 1111e1131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ Wiley & Sons, Inc.
s0954579409990071. Hemphill, J. F., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1998). Psychopathy and
Glenn, A. L., Raine, A., & Schug, R. A. (2009). The neural correlates recidivism: a review. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3(1),
of moral decision-making in psychopathy. Molecular 139e170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8333.1998.tb00355.x.
Psychiatry, 14(1), 5e6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/mp.2008.104. Hoptman, M. J., D'Angelo, D., Catalano, D., Mauro, C. J.,
Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral Shehzad, Z. E., Kelly, A. M., et al. (2010). Amygdalofrontal
judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Science, 6(12), 517e523. functional disconnectivity and aggression in schizophrenia.
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(5), 1020e1028. http://dx.doi.org/
Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional 10.1093/schbul/sbp012.
engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105e2108. Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., Touryan, S. R.,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1062872. Greene, E. J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2006). Dissociating medial
Gregory, S., Ffytche, D., Simmons, A., Kumari, V., Howard, M., frontal and posterior cingulate activity during self-reflection.
Hodgins, S., et al. (2012). The antisocial brain: psychopathy Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 1(1), 56e64. http://
matters: a structural MRI investigation of antisocial male dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl004.
violent offenders. Archives of General Psychiatry. http:// Jones, D. K., Williams, S. C., Gasston, D., Horsfield, M. A.,
dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.222. Simmons, A., & Howard, R. (2002). Isotropic resolution diffusion
Greicius, M. D., Srivastava, G., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2004). tensor imaging with whole brain acquisition in a clinically
Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer's acceptable time. Human Brain Mapping, 15(4), 216e230.
disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Juarez, M., Kiehl, K. A., & Calhoun, V. D. (2012). Intrinsic limbic
Proceedings of the National Academy of the Sciences of the United and paralimbic networks are associated with criminal
States of America, 101(13), 4637e4642. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/ psychopathy. Human Brain Mapping. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
pnas.0308627101. hbm.22037.
Greicius, M. D., Supekar, K., Menon, V., & Dougherty, R. F. (2009). Kelley, W. M., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., &
Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event-related
connectivity in the default mode network. Cerebral Cortex, fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(5), 785e794.
19(1), 72e78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn059. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/08989290260138672.
Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. Kiehl, K. A. (2006). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on
(2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental psychopathy: evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction.
activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Psychiatry Research, 142(2e3), 107e128. http://dx.doi.org/
Proceedings of the National Academy of the Sciences of the United 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.013.
States of America, 98(7), 4259e4264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/ Kiehl, K. A., & Hoffman, M. B. (2011). The criminal psychopath:
pnas.071043098. history, neuroscience, treatment, and economics. Jurimetrics,
Hare, R. D. (1991). The hare psychopathy checklist e Revised. Toronto, 51(4), 355e397.
Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B.,
Hare, R. D. (2003). The hare psychopathy checklist e Revised (PCLeR) Brink, J., et al. (2001). Limbic abnormalities in affective processing
manual (2nd ed.). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic
Systems. resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 50(9), 677e684.
Hare, R. D., Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., Forth, A. E., Hart, S. D., & King, H. E. (1961). Psychological effects of excitation of the limbic
Newman, J. P. (1990). The revised psychopathy checklist: system. In D. E. Sheer (Ed.), Electrical stimulation of the brain.
reliability and factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 2(3), Houston, Texas,: Hoggs Foundation, University of Texas Press.
338e341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.2.3.338. Kluver, H., & Bucy, P. C. (1997). Preliminary analysis of functions
Harlow, J. M. (1993). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar of the temporal lobes in monkeys. 1939. The Journal of
through the head. 1868. History of Psychiatry, 4(14), 274e281. Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 9(4), 606e620.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0957154x9300401407. Koch, K., Schultz, C. C., Wagner, G., Schachtzabel, C.,
Harlow, J. M. (1999). Passage of an iron rod through the head. 1848. Reichenbach, J. R., Sauer, H., et al. (2013). Disrupted white
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 11(2), matter connectivity is associated with reduced cortical
281e283. thickness in the cingulate cortex in schizophrenia. Cortex,
Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., & Hare, R. D. (1988). Factor structure 49(3), 722e729. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.02.001.
of the psychopathy checklist. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Koenigs, M., Kruepke, M., Zeier, J., & Newman, J. P. (2012).
Psychology, 56(5), 741e747. Utilitarian moral judgment in psychopathy. Social Cognitive &
Harpur, T. J., Hare, R. D., & Hakstian, A. R. (1989). Two-factor Affective Neuroscience, 7(6), 708e714. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/
conceptualization of psychopathy: construct validity and scan/nsr048.
assessment implications. Psychological Assessment, 1(1), 6e17. Lebel, C., Walker, L., Leemans, A., Phillips, L., & Beaulieu, C. (2008).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.1.1.6. Microstructural maturation of the human brain from
Harris, G. J., Jaffin, S. K., Hodge, S. M., Kennedy, D., Caviness, V. S., childhood to adulthood. NeuroImage, 40(3), 1044e1055. http://
Marinkovic, K., et al. (2008). Frontal white matter and dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.053.

Please cite this article in press as: Sethi, A., et al., Emotional detachment in psychopathy: Involvement of dorsal default-mode
connections, Cortex (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.018
c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 4 ) 1 e9 9

Levenston, G. K., Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage.
The psychopath as observer: emotion and attention in picture Neuropsychologia, 29(12), 1241e1249.
processing. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 373e385. Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Harari, H., Aharon-Peretz, J., &
Lim, K. O., Choi, S. J., Pomara, N., Wolkin, A., & Rotrosen, J. P. Levkovitz, Y. (2010). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in
(2002). Reduced frontal white matter integrity in cocaine affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with
dependence: a controlled diffusion tensor imaging study. psychopathic tendencies. Cortex, 46(5), 668e677. http://
Biological Psychiatry, 51(11), 890e895. dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.008.
Maddock, R. J., Garrett, A. S., & Buonocore, M. H. (2003). Posterior Sheline, Y. I., Barch, D. M., Price, J. L., Rundle, M. M.,
cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence Vaishnavi, S. N., Snyder, A. Z., et al. (2009). The default mode
from a valence decision task. Human Brain Mapping, 18(1), network and self-referential processes in depression.
30e41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10075. Proceedings of the National Academy of the Sciences of the United
Motzkin, J. C., Newman, J. P., Kiehl, K. A., & Koenigs, M. (2011). States of America, 106(6), 1942e1947. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/
Reduced prefrontal connectivity in psychopathy. Journal of pnas.0812686106.
Neuroscience, 31(48), 17348e17357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/ Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L.,
JNEUROSCI.4215-11.2011. Miezin, F. M., Raichle, M. E., et al. (1997). Common blood flow
de Oliveira-Souza, R., Hare, R. D., Bramati, I. E., Garrido, G. J., changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex.
Azevedo Ignacio, F., Tovar-Moll, F., et al. (2008). Psychopathy Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 648e663. http://
as a disorder of the moral brain: fronto-temporo-limbic grey dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648.
matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-based Skeem, J. L., & Cooke, D. J. (2010). Is criminal behavior a central
morphometry. NeuroImage, 40(3), 1202e1213. http://dx.doi.org/ component of psychopathy? Conceptual directions for
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.054. resolving the debate. Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 433e445.
Ochsner, K. N., Knierim, K., Ludlow, D. H., Hanelin, J., http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0008512.
Ramachandran, T., Glover, G., et al. (2004). Reflecting upon Sonuga-Barke, E. J., & Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Spontaneous
feelings: an fMRI study of neural systems supporting the attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological
attribution of emotion to self and other. Journal of Cognitive conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neuroscience &
Neuroscience, 16(10), 1746e1772. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/ Biobehavioural Reviews, 31(7), 977e986. http://dx.doi.org/
0898929042947829. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.005.
Pfefferbaum, A., Rosenbloom, M., Rohlfing, T., & Sullivan, E. V. Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., Gibbon, M., & First, M. B. (1992). The
(2009). Degradation of association and projection white matter structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R (SCID). I: history,
systems in alcoholism detected with quantitative fiber rationale, and description. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49(8),
tracking. Biological Psychiatry, 65(8), 680e690. http://dx.doi.org/ 624e629.
10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.039. Sundram, F., Deeley, Q., Sarkar, S., Daly, E., Latham, R., Craig, M.,
Pfefferbaum, A., & Sullivan, E. V. (2005). Disruption of brain white et al. (2012). White matter microstructural abnormalities in the
matter microstructure by excessive intracellular and frontal lobe of adults with antisocial personality disorder. Cortex,
extracellular fluid in alcoholism: evidence from diffusion 48(2), 216e229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.005.
tensor imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30(2), 423e432. Tournier, J. D., Calamante, F., Gadian, D. G., & Connelly, A. (2004).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300623. Direct estimation of the fiber orientation density function
Pujol, J., Batalla, I., Contreras-Rodriguez, O., Harrison, B. J., from diffusion-weighted MRI data using spherical
Pera, V., Hernandez-Ribas, R., et al. (2011). Breakdown in the deconvolution. NeuroImage, 23(3), 1176e1185. http://dx.doi.org/
brain network subserving moral judgment in criminal 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.037.
psychopathy. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience. http:// Tuch, D. S., Reese, T. G., Wiegell, M. R., Makris, N., Belliveau, J. W.,
dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr075. & Wedeen, V. J. (2002). High angular resolution diffusion
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., imaging reveals intravoxel white matter fiber heterogeneity.
Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 48(4), 577e582. http://
brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of the dx.doi.org/10.1002/mrm.10268.
Sciences of the United States of America, 98(2), 676e682. http:// Veit, R., Flor, H., Erb, M., Hermann, C., Lotze, M., Grodd, W., et al.
dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. (2002). Brain circuits involved in emotional learning in
Raine, A., Buchsbaum, M., & LaCasse, L. (1997). Brain antisocial behavior and social phobia in humans. Neuroscience
abnormalities in murderers indicated by positron emission Letters, 328(3), 233e236.
tomography. Biological Psychiatry, 42(6), 495e508. http:// Viding, E., Blair, R. J., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2005). Evidence for
dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00362-9. substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. Journal
Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L., & Colletti, P. (2000). of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(6), 592e597. http://
Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00393.x.
autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Archives Vollm, B. A., Taylor, A. N., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J.,
of General Psychiatry, 57(2), 119e127. discussion 128e119. McKie, S., et al. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind
Rijsdijsk, F. V., Viding, E., De Brito, S., Forgiarini, M., Mechelli, A., and empathy: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study
Jones, A. P., et al. (2010). Heritable variations in gray matter in a nonverbal task. NeuroImage, 29(1), 90e98. http://
concentration as a potential endophenotype for psychopathic dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.022.
traits. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(4), 406e413. http:// Weschler, D. (1997). WAIS-III: Administration and scoring manual.
dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.20. San Antonio, Texas: Harcourt Brace and Co.
Sarkar, S., Craig, M. C., Catani, M., Dell'acqua, F., Fahy, T., Wootton, J. M., Frick, P. J., Shelton, K. K., & Silverthorn, P. (1997).
Deeley, Q., et al. (2013). Frontotemporal white-matter Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: the
microstructural abnormalities in adolescents with conduct moderating role of callous-unemotional traits. Journal of
disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychological Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(2), 301e308.
Medicine, 43(2), 401e411. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ Yang, Y., Raine, A., Colletti, P., Toga, A. W., & Narr, K. L. (2009).
S003329171200116X. Abnormal temporal and prefrontal cortical gray matter
Saver, J. L., & Damasio, A. R. (1991). Preserved access and thinning in psychopaths. Molecular Psychiatry, 14(6), 561e562.
processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/mp.2009.12, 555.

Please cite this article in press as: Sethi, A., et al., Emotional detachment in psychopathy: Involvement of dorsal default-mode
connections, Cortex (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.018

You might also like