You are on page 1of 14

MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001

Continuity or Discontinuity of Family Involvement Following


Residential Transitions of Adults Who Have Mental Retardation
Marsha Mailick Seltzer, Marty Wyngaarden Krauss, Jinkuk Hong, and Gael I. Orsmond

Abstract
Family involvement with adults who have mental retardation following a residential transition to
a nonparental living situation was examined. We found that aging mothers were highly involved
in the relocation process and had frequent contact and continued emotional involvement with
their adult child. Mothers became increasingly satisfied with their level of contact with their child
over time, less worried about the future, and had decreasing levels of direct caregiving and contact
with residential staff. Adult siblings reported improved sibling relationships over time. Siblings
whose brother or sister moved out of the parental home increased their shared activities and felt
less pessimistic about the future. Findings address a critical gap in knowledge about the life course
roles of families of persons with mental retardation.

There is now widespread recognition of the residential transitions from the parental home take
central role that families of persons with mental re- place in a more planned context in which different
tardation occupy throughout their lifespan (Fujiura options can be evaluated to arrive at an optimal and
& Braddock, 1992). The majority of adults with paced residential change (Butterworth, Steere, &
mental retardation continue to live at home with Whitney-Thomas, 1997).
their parents well into adulthood, and this increas- There is a dearth of literature on family in-
ingly recognized normative pattern has been the volvement in the lives of their adult child with
subject of considerable research (Englehardt, Bru- mental retardation following a planned residential
baker, & Lutzer, 1988; Heller & Factor, 1994; M. change away from the parental home, resulting in
Seltzer & Krauss, 1989). Key issues that have been a critical gap in our knowledge about the ongoing
investigated include the benefits and challenges to roles of families throughout the life course. In our
families of providing long-term care (Krauss & Selt- research we provide insight into this increasingly
zer, 1999; Smith, Fullmer, & Tobin, 1994), the fac- common, though as yet undocumented, phenome-
tors associated with the psychological and social non by reporting on the experience of families
well-being of the parents (Heller & Factor, 1993; whose son or daughter with mental retardation
Krauss & Seltzer, 1993), the role of adult siblings lived at home well into adulthood and who moved
in the caregiving context (Griffiths & Unger, 1994; to a nonparental living situation while the mother
Pruchno, Patrick, & Burant, 1996; G. Seltzer, Be- was healthy enough to support her son or daughter
gun, Seltzer, & Krauss, 1991), and the factors that through this transition. These families were part of
lead to a change in the living circumstances of the a longitudinal research study in which family in-
family member with mental retardation (Essex, volvement was tracked prior to and after the resi-
Seltzer, & Krauss, 1997; Heller & Factor, 1991). dential relocation of the adult son or daughter with
Parental death or incapacitation may prompt a cri- mental retardation (Krauss & Seltzer, 1999).
sis in the family and often results in a hasty relo- Understanding continuity or discontinuity in
cation of the adult with mental retardation to an- family involvement following a change in the living
other setting (Gordon, Seltzer, & Krauss, 1997). situation of the son or daughter with mental retar-
However, it is far preferable for all involved to have dation is important from several perspectives. For

qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation 181


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

families of adolescents and young adults whose same since placement. No relation was found be-
child with mental retardation lives at home, infor- tween length of time in placement and level or de-
mation about the experiences of older families who gree of family involvement (Baker et al., 1996), sug-
have experienced this transition provides valuable gesting a pattern of continuity rather than abate-
insights regarding how such a major life change can ment of family contacts over time.
be navigated and managed. For residential service In another longitudinal study of family involve-
providers, information on family involvement fol- ment, mothers of 53 children with severe levels of
lowing such residential relocations can assist in an- mental retardation in residential settings were in-
ticipating how to make the initial and subsequent terviewed at approximately 1 year, 2 years, and 3.5
adjustment period to a new residence smoother for years postplacement (Blacher et al., 1999). The set-
both the adult with mental retardation and his or tings into which the children were placed ranged
her family. For policymakers charged with planning from medium-sized (7 or fewer residents) foster
for the resolution of extensive waiting lists for res- homes to large group homes, large private residen-
idential services now reported around the country tial schools, and state developmental centers, about
(Lakin, 1998; Lakin, Anderson, Prouty, & Polister, half of which were at least 30 miles from their fam-
1999), information on family involvement patterns ily’s home. The sample included mothers of chil-
may suggest new opportunities for partnerships with dren between the ages of 4 and 17 years at the time
families that reflect parental and sibling preferences of the first interview. They found that across the
for ongoing roles. Finally, for researchers, studies of three measurement points, the vast majority of fam-
family involvement after their adult son or daughter ilies (in excess of 80%) reported some type of con-
with mental retardation moves away from home tact with their son or daughter at least monthly.
will begin to fill the knowledge gap that now exists They also found stability over time in the family’s
regarding the ways in which families manage their level of behavioral, emotional, and cognitive in-
atypical parenting responsibilities over the full life volvement with their placed son or daughter. Spe-
course of the family (Marshak, Seligman, & Pre- cifically, families reported feeling less guilty over
zant, 1999). time regarding the decision to place their child,
The most extensive recent literature on family continued to think about their child at least daily
involvement following the transition of an individ- and to talk with others about their child, and per-
ual with mental retardation away from the parental ceived their child’s placement as permanent. They
home has been reported by Blacher and Baker in also found that families reported a range of benefi-
their studies of children with mental retardation cial changes in their family life following place-
and/or psychiatric disorders in residential treatment ment, such as greater opportunities for recreational
settings (Blacher & Baker, 1994; Baker, Blacher, & activities and improvements in family relationships.
Pfeiffer, 1993, 1996; Baker, Heller, Blacher, & Pfeif- Extending their research to siblings, Eisenberg,
fer, 1995; Blacher, Baker, & Feinfield, 1999). A Baker, and Blacher (1998) contrasted three groups:
central question in their studies was whether, over siblings of children with mental retardation who
time, parental contact with and emotional attach- continued to live at home (n 5 25), siblings whose
ment to their placed child decreases. In general, brother or sister had been placed (n 5 20), and
Blacher and Baker have concluded that parents re- siblings whose brother or sister did not have a dis-
main attached to and involved with their child af- ability (n 5 28). The siblings in this study ranged
ter placement. For example, in Baker et al.’s (1996) in age from 9 to 20 years (mean about 13). These
research on children and adults in private residen- three groups showed very similar profiles of psycho-
tial treatment centers in Arizona, California, and logical adjustment, self-esteem, and perceptions of
Florida, about two thirds of the 163 families in the the family environment but were quite different in
study reported at least weekly phone contact with their appraisals of the sibling relationship. Those
the residential treatment facility (although not nec- whose brother or sister had mental retardation
essarily with their child). Family visits to the treat- showed less warmth and less conflict than norma-
ment center and visits home by the individual with tive sibling relationships, but they did not differ in
disabilities occurred between 3 (the mode) and 6 the importance they placed on the sibling relation-
(the median) times per year. Further, most respon- ship or their satisfaction with it. Of particular rel-
dents said that their feelings of attachment to their evance to the present study, there were no differ-
family member with the disability had remained the ences between siblings whose brother or sister with

182 qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

mental retardation lived at home or was placed, increasingly rare among families of children with
suggesting that placement out of the home did not mental retardation and nonnormative from the per-
alter the sibling relationship, at least with respect spective of family life course theories (Carter &
to the measures included in the Eisenberg et al. McGoldrick, 1999).
(1998) study. In light of the prevalence of long-term family
In an investigation of a markedly different pop- caregiving responsibility, it is important to extend
ulation, Stoneman and Crapps (1990) reported low our knowledge about familial roles in the lives of
rates of family involvement with a sample of per- the family member with mental retardation. Our
sons with mental retardation (most between the longitudinal study of families of adults with mental
ages of 18 and 45 years) living in licensed foster retardation offers an opportunity to examine pro-
homes (family care homes) in Georgia. Although spectively the patterns of family involvement fol-
approximately 90% of the sample had at least one lowing relocation from the parental home to an-
living family member (mother, father, or sibling), other living situation. In some of our earlier anal-
fewer than half had contact with their families yses, we studied relocation prompted by parental ill-
(based on data collected from the home careprov- ness or death (Essex et al., 1997; Gordon et al.,
ider). Among the explanations for this low level of 1997). In this paper, in contrast, we examined a
family contact were that the average distance be- different pattern of residential relocation by adults
tween the foster home and the family’s own resi- with mental retardation, a pattern not prompted by
dence was well over 100 miles and that only 14% a parental health crisis. Rather, the adults in the
of the sample had lived with their family prior to present analysis moved to a nonparental residence
moving to their current residence (the others while the mother was still healthy. The residential
moved there from institutions, group homes, or oth- transitions we report on here are, therefore, more
er foster homes). The findings suggest that the prior consistent with normative patterns of the ‘‘launch-
place of residence was an important factor condi- ing’’ of adult children, albeit occurring in our study
tioning family involvement because individuals in at a later point in the family life course than is
the Stoneman and Crapps study who moved to characteristic of typical families and with a greater
their current residence directly from the parental degree of familial and professional support.
home had the most family participation in the Family life course theorists note that the
placement process and the most family involvement launching stage, defined as the period when children
after the residential transition. They also found that move out of the parental home to establish a life
among residents who visited their siblings, most of independent from the family of origin, is a period
the visits were with sisters. Indeed, the home care- of considerable disequilibrium because it precipi-
providers rated their residents as having stronger tates profound changes in the relationships between
ties with their siblings than with their parents. parents and their children (Aldous, 1996; Carter &
This brief review of the literature is instructive McGoldrick, 1999). This period is particularly dif-
in documenting the persistence of family involve- ficult for mothers who must cope with the loss of
ment following placement of individuals with men- the maternal role. Although families of adults with
tal retardation into private residential treatment mental retardation who live at home have typically
settings or into publicly supported settings of vari- experienced the launching of their other children
ous sizes. The Stoneman and Crapps (1990) study without disabilities, the launching stage is not fully
suggests the fragility of family involvement when completed until the adult with mental retardation
geographical distance is great, when family bonds has also moved from the parental home. One pur-
may have been challenged by multiple prior place- pose of our research is to examine whether the dis-
ments, and when family members are not involved equilibrium that is associated with the launching
in the placement process. Counterbalanced against stage is also manifest when full launching has been
these findings is the recognition that the literature postponed.
on family involvement after the son or daughter As we have reported elsewhere, the families in
moves away from the parental home is limited in our 12-year longitudinal study avowed an explicit
quantity and scope, consisting primarily of studies preference for family-based care (Krauss & Seltzer,
of families who placed their minor child in insti- 1999), formed the nucleus of the social support net-
tutional or community-based residential programs works of their son or daughter with mental retar-
during childhood or adolescence, a practice that is dation (Krauss, Seltzer, & Goodman, 1992), and in-

qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation 183


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

cluded siblings without disabilities, who were often location data from the sample. The sample for this
deeply committed to the current and future welfare paper is restricted to the 117 families in our overall
of their brother or sister with mental retardation sample whose son or daughter moved away from
(Krauss, Seltzer, Gordon, & Friedman, 1996; M. home after the study began and in which the moth-
Seltzer, Greenberg, Krauss, Gordon, & Judge, er remained healthy enough to continue to partic-
1997). Over a 12-year period, about one quarter of ipate in at least one research interview after the son
the families in our study sought and secured an al- or daughter moved. This analysis excludes families
ternative living situation for their son or daughter in which the mother died or became too incapaci-
with mental retardation. Although the sample on tated to participate in the first research interview
which the findings reported here is small (in num- after relocation. Thus, this paper does not represent
bers), and select (with respect to securing an alter- the patterns characteristic of all families, just those
native setting while the mother was healthy enough with potential for continued maternal involvement
to orchestrate the transition), our study provides an after relocation.
initial prospective investigation of the continuities
and discontinuities of family involvement among Sample
persons with mental retardation who have lived The sample for the first research question,
with family for most of their lives. which assessed the extent of maternal involvement
We addressed three research questions: What is during the process of relocation, consisted of these
the extent of maternal involvement in the process 117 mothers. At the time their son or daughter
of relocation, including selecting the residence and moved away from home, they ranged from 58 to 87
facilitating the transition from the parental home? years of age, with an average age of 71.0 years. Over
How do patterns of maternal involvement change two thirds (68.9%) rated their health as good or
during the first 3 to 4 years after the adult son or excellent, and 60.8% were still married. Of those
daughter moved away from home? How do patterns who were not married, most (36.3%) were widows;
of sibling involvement change after the adult with only a few (2.9%) were divorced. Although the
mental retardation moved away from the parental sample was virtually all European American (99%),
home? there was diversity in socioeconomic status, with
42% earning less than $20,000 per year and 15%
earning more than $50,000. The sons and daughters
Method with mental retardation ranged in age from 21 to
Since 1988, we have collected data from 461 66 years when they moved out of their parents’
families from Massachusetts and Wisconsin who, at home, with an average of 38.9 years. A little over
the outset of the study met two criteria: the family half (53.8%) were sons. Most (78.1%) had mild or
included a mother between the ages of 55 and 85 moderate mental retardation; 21.9% had severe or
and a son or daughter with mental retardation who profound levels of retardation.
lived at home with her. In total, the study encom- Following the transition away from the paren-
passed eight research interviews with all families, tal home, the 117 individuals with mental retar-
each scheduled 18 months apart. Because the dation moved to a variety of types of residences.
eighth set of interviews is not yet available for anal- Most (64.1%) moved to fully supervised community
ysis, this present paper is based on data from the residential settings (group homes and foster homes).
first seven interviews with the families in our study, Fewer (17.5%) moved to more independent living
conducted between 1988 and 1999. At each point arrangements (partially staffed or unstaffed apart-
of data collection, interviews were held with the ments) or to congregate care facilities, such as nurs-
mother, and supplemental data were collected from ing homes or public or private institutions (14.5%).
fathers. In addition, at Time 3 and Time 6, the The remaining 4 individuals moved to the homes
‘‘most involved’’ sibling, as designated by the moth- of other relatives.
er, was surveyed. Participation rates were high, with We used different subsamples of these 117 fam-
fully 88.6% of the families still participating at the ilies for the analysis of the second and third re-
seventh wave of data collection. search questions. For the second research question,
The prospective design of our study is uniquely in which we examined the extent of longer term
tailored to address the question of continued family family involvement with the son or daughter after
involvement because we collected pre- and postre- relocation, the sample consisted of 64 families in

184 qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

which the mother was healthy enough to partici- indicators of maternal involvement: (a) the fre-
pate in three consecutive research interviews after quency of contact between the mother and adult
the son or daughter’s move. In addition, we ran- with mental retardation, either in person or by tele-
domly selected a comparison sample of 64 families phone (using the previously described scale); (b)
whose son or daughter remained at home through- the mother’s rating of her satisfaction with her level
out the study period. of contact with her son or daughter (0 5 completely
For the third research question, concerning the dissatisfied, 1 5 somewhat dissatisfied, 2 5 somewhat
extent of sibling involvement before and after the satisfied, 3 5 completely satisfied); (c) the frequency
adult with mental retardation moved away from the of contact between the mother and the staff at the
parental home, the sample consisted of 26 siblings residential setting (also using the same scale); (d)
who participated in our two-wave sibling study direct caregiving by the mother (measured by the
(Time 3 and Time 6) and whose brother or sister number of caregiving tasks performed by the moth-
with mental retardation moved away from the pa- er, ranging from 0 to 31), (e) the level of emotional
rental home after the Time 3 point of sibling data closeness reported by the mother in her relationship
collection and before Time 6. These siblings con- with her son or daughter (measured by the Positive
stituted 63.4% of the siblings who were eligible for Affect Index, a 5 .71 (Bengtson & Black, 1973);
this analysis. The siblings ranged from 22 to 56 and (f) the extent of the mother’s worries about the
years of age, with an average age of 38.2 years at future care of her son or daughter with mental re-
Time 3. Nine of these siblings without disabilities tardation. This last variable was measured using the
were brothers; 17 were sisters. A large majority Pessimism subscale of the Questionnaire on Re-
(76.9%) were married. Many had children of their sources and Stress—F—QRS-F, a 5.80 (Friedrich,
own (69.2%). All of then lived apart from their Greenberg, & Crnic, 1983).
parents. Half (49.9%) lived within an hour’s drive For the third research question, we used four
of their parents’ home. In addition, we randomly indicators of sibling involvement. First, we em-
selected a comparison group of siblings whose ployed the previously described Positive Affect In-
brother or sister remained in the parental home dex (Bengtson & Black, 1973), reflective of the
throughout this study period. (More details about emotional closeness of the sibling with the brother
the sampling design are available from the authors.) or sister with mental retardation. Second, we used
the sibling’s rating of how well he or she got along
Data Collection and Measures with the brother or sister. Third, we used the sib-
A core set of questions was posed to all mothers ling’s rating of how often he or she ‘‘did things to-
in our study, regardless of where the son or daughter gether’’ with the brother or sister. For the latter two
lived at any given point of data collection. In ad- measures, ratings were made on a 6-point scale (1
dition, a special set of questions addressing contin- 5 not at all to 6 5 extremely). Finally, we measured
ued maternal involvement was asked of mothers siblings’ worries about the future of their brother or
whose son or daughter had moved to a nonparental sister with mental retardation using the same mea-
residence. sure (Pessimism subscale) as used with mothers.
For the first research question, we used data on
three measures that describe the mother’s and the Data Analysis
adult’s preparation for and involvement in the pro- The analysis conducted for the first research
cess of relocation to a nonparental living arrange- question (‘‘What is the extent of maternal involve-
ment: (a) whether the adult was on a waiting list ment in the process of relocation, including select-
for residential services prior to moving away from ing the residence and facilitating the transition
the parental home (0 5 no, 1 5 yes), (b) the num- from the parental home?’’) involved descriptive sta-
ber of visits to the new residence made by the tistics (means and percentages) of maternal in-
mother and the adult, and (c) the frequency of con- volvement in the residential relocation process.
tact (in person or by telephone) between the moth- The analysis for the second research question
er and the adult after the move. Frequency of con- (‘‘How do patterns of maternal involvement change
tact was measured on the following scale: 0 5 no during the first 3 to 4 years after the adult son or
contact, 1 5 less than weekly, 2 5 weekly, 3 5 5 to daughter moved away from home?’’) involved com-
8 contacts a month, 4 5 daily. parison of two groups of mothers: (a) the 64 moth-
For the second research question, we used six ers whose son or daughter moved during the study

qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation 185


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

period and who were healthy enough to participate itating the transition from the parental home?’’ For
in our study for three postrelocation interviews and this analysis, we examined data from the first re-
(b) a randomly selected comparison group of 64 search interview after the adult with mental retar-
mothers whose son or daughter remained in the pa- dation moved away from the parental home. On
rental home throughout this time period. Two types average, this interview occurred 9.2 months after
of analyses were conducted for this research ques- the relocation. There was no significant correlation
tion. First, for the analysis of questions asked only between the time between the relocation and the
of mothers whose adult child had relocated to a new next interview and the variables we report on in
residence, we conducted a series of one-way repeat- the discussion that follows. The sample for this
ed measures ANOVAs tracking the relocation analysis was the 117 mothers described earlier.
group across the first three research interviews con- The evidence suggests a very high level of ma-
ducted after the move. Second, for the analysis of ternal involvement in the relocation process. For
measures obtained both from mothers whose son or the majority of families, maternal involvement be-
daughter had relocated and from mothers whose gan before the adult moved out of the parental
adult child continued to live at home, we conduct- home. Well over half of the mothers (61.0%) re-
ed a series of 2 3 4 repeated measures ANOVAs, ported that their son or daughter was on a waiting
with the factors being Residential Status (continued list for residential services prior to the move. In
co-residence vs. relocation) and Time (Wave 1 was addition, it was the norm for the mother to visit
the research interview conducted prior to the move the residence before the son or daughter moved,
of the son or daughter away from the parental with fully 86.8% of the mothers having made at
home; Waves 2, 3, and 4 were the first, second, and least one visit. Mothers typically visited the resi-
third research interviews conducted after the move, dence about three times prior to the move (mean
respectively). The points of data collection for the 5 2.97). It was also common for mothers to facil-
comparison group were linked temporally with itate visits by the adult with mental retardation to
those in the relocation group. Post-hoc comparisons the residence, with over three fourths of the adults
were conducted to examine the significance of the (78.3%) making such visits. The average number of
degree of change from the first point of data collec- visits made by the son or daughter to the new res-
tion (Wave 1) to each subsequent point. idence was 3.26, of which about one (mean 5 1.30)
The third research question was ‘‘How do pat- was an overnight visit.
terns of sibling involvement change after the adult During the initial period following the move
with mental retardation moved away from the pa- away from the parental home, there was a dominant
rental home?’’ In order to provide a comparison of pattern of continued contact between the mother
the trajectory of sibling involvement following the and the son or daughter with mental retardation.
move away from the parental home, we randomly Over one quarter (27.4%) of the mothers reported
selected 26 siblings whose brother or sister had re- daily contact either in person or by phone with
mained in the parental home between Time 3 and their son or daughter, another third (35.7%) re-
Time 6. These siblings constituted the comparison ported between five and eight contacts per month,
group for Research Question 3. A series of 2 3 2 two fifths (21.4%) reported weekly contact, and the
repeated measures ANOVAs was conducted for this remaining 14.3% had less than weekly contact.
research question, with the factors being residential Only one mother had no contact with her adult
status at Time 6 (continued residence in the paren- child after the move away from home. Thus, fre-
tal home versus moved to an out-of-home setting) quent contact between the mother and the adult
and time (Time 3 and Time 6). son or daughter was the norm for the families in
our study, during the period immediately after the
move.
Findings To summarize the findings regarding the first
research question, our data show that most mothers
Maternal Involvement in the Relocation were highly involved in the relocation process and
Process facilitated actively the transition from the parental
Our first research question was, ‘‘What is the home to the adult’s new residence by visiting be-
extent of maternal involvement in the relocation forehand, arranging for the son or daughter to visit,
process, including selecting the residence and facil- and by having frequent contact either in person or

186 qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

by phone with the son or daughter during the pe- .034. Mothers’ frequency of contact with the staff
riod immediately after he or she moved away from from the new residence also changed over the study
home. period, with significantly more contact reported
right after the move than several years later. Post-
Changes in Maternal Involvement Following hoc comparisons indicate a significant decrease in
Relocation frequency of contact with staff from Wave 1 to
Our second research question was ‘‘How do Wave 2, p 5 .002, as well as from Wave 1 to Wave
patterns of maternal involvement change during 3, p 5 .007.
the first 3 years after the adult son or daughter Table 2 portrays the results of two-way repeated
moved away from home?’’ For this analysis, we ex- measures ANOVAs to contrast mothers whose son
amined data from the 64 mothers who participated or daughter moved with those in the comparison
in the first three research interviews after the adult group. Not surprisingly, mothers provided signifi-
with mental retardation moved away from the pa- cantly less hands-on care for their son or daughter
rental home, each 18 months after the previous after he or she moved away from home than before.
one. Whereas the number of caregiving tasks performed
Our goal in selecting this subsample was to in- by mothers in the comparison group (Group 1) was
vestigate levels of involvement among mothers who stable over time (i.e., those mothers provided assis-
were in relatively good health when their son or tance with about 11 daily living tasks at each of the
daughter moved away from home and who re- four research interviews), for the families in which
mained in fairly good health during the 3- to 4-year the adult had moved, maternal caregiving decreased
period after relocation. This sampling decision re- from a prior (i.e., co-resident) level of help to help
flected our interest in investigating maternal in- with about one task as measured at each of the
volvement in the context of more normative pat- three research interviews conducted after reloca-
terns of relocation (i.e., launching) rather than re- tion. The residential status by time interaction ef-
location in the context of a crisis (such as declining fect for the number of caregiving tasks performed
maternal health). We checked mothers’ health rat- by the mother was highly significant, reflective of
ings across the points of data collection used in this the divergent caregiving pattern characteristic of
analysis and found that mothers of individuals who the two groups over time. Post-hoc comparisons in-
moved away from home, on average, were stable or dicate that there was a significant decrease from the
improved in their health status during this study pretransition point of data collection to each of the
period. (Data are available from the authors.) posttransition points in the number of caregiving
As shown in Table 1, which portrays the results tasks performed by mothers of those who moved
of the one-way repeated measures ANOVAs, we away from home (all post-hoc contrasts were at the
found stability in the frequency of contact between p , .001 level). The caregiving tasks most likely to
the mother and the son or daughter with mental be performed by mothers after the son or daughter
retardation, as measured at the first three research moved away from home were assisting the adult
interviews after the adult moved away from home. with shopping and helping him or her with money
Mothers averaged more than one contact each management.
week by phone or in person with their son or Next, we examined whether the mother’s level
daughter at each of the three points of data collec- of emotional involvement with the son or daughter
tion following relocation. Although there was no changed over time. In contrast to the sharp de-
change in frequency of contact over the study pe- crease in the extent of hands-on care following the
riod, there was a statistically significant increase move, there was a gradual increase in the mother’s
during this time in maternal satisfaction with the level of emotional involvement with her adult child
amount of contact she had with her son or daugh- across the four research interviews (see Table 2).
ter, perhaps reflecting an increasing level of comfort This pattern was characteristic of both mothers
with her adult child’s new living arrangements. whose adult lived at home throughout the study
Post-hoc comparisons indicate that although ma- period (Group 1) and mothers of adults who moved
ternal satisfaction was stable between Waves 1 and away from home (Group 2). These data suggest sus-
2, there was a significant increase in satisfaction be- tained and growing emotional involvement by the
tween Wave 1 (immediately after the residential mother with her son or daughter with mental re-
transition) and Wave 3 (36 months later), p 5 tardation over time, regardless of residential status.

qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation 187


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

Table 1 One-Way Analysis of Variance of Maternal Involvement During the First Three Research Interviews
After Relocation
Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3
Maternal involvement Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD F (time)
Frequency of contact be-
tween mother and adult
child 3.56 1.28 3.56 1.24 3.40 1.07 0.757
Maternal satisfaction with
contact 2.44 0.85 2.51 0.78 2.68 0.47 3.173*
Frequency of contact be-
tween mother and resi-
dential staff 3.38 1.00 2.79 1.27 2.87 1.25 6.279**
Note. This table does not include data from the research interview prior to relocation because the measures
of maternal contact and satisfaction apply only to the postrelocation period. Wave 1 refers to the first point
of data collection after relocation. Wave 2 refers to the second point of data collection after relocation.
Wave 3 refers to the third point of data collection after relocation.
*p , .05. **p , .01.

For both groups, post-hoc comparisons indicate that Wave 4 were statistically significant, ps 5 .035 and
the increase in emotional involvement between .023, respectively.
Wave 1 and Wave 2 was marginal, p 5 .069, but Finally, we examined changes in the mothers’
that the increases from Wave 1 to Wave 3 and level of worries about her son or daughter’s future

Table 2 Two-Way Analysis of Variance of Maternal Involvement With Co-Resident Versus Relocated Adults
With Mental Retardation
F
(Residen-
F tial
Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3 Wave 4
Maternal (residential F Status
involvement Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD status) (time) 3 Time)
No. of caregiving
tasks
Group 1 11.21 6.45 11.19 6.33 11.00 6.40 10.90 6.17 64.73, 104.70, 114.60,
Group 2 11.86 5.90 0.73 1.55 1.22 2.18 1.37 2.20 p,.001 p,.001 p,.001
Level of emotional
involvement by the
mother
Group 1 25.78 3.04 26.33 3.22 26.56 2.87 26.74 2.80 1.37, 5.96, 0.49,
Group 2 25.39 3.06 25.78 2.96 25.75 3.02 25.94 2.95 p5.245 p5.017 p5.487
Level of worries/
pessimism
Group 1 6.13 2.83 6.10 2.96 5.52 3.18 6.02 2.83 0.71, 25.36, 15.76,
Group 2 7.68 2.72 5.75 3.22 5.14 3.12 5.68 3.20 p5.791 p,.001 p,.001
Note. Wave 1: all co-resident; Wave 2: Group 1 5 co-resident, Group 2 5 relocated; Wave 3: Group 1 5 co-
resident, Group 2 5 relocated; Wave 4: Group 1 5 co-resident, Group 2 5 relocated.

188 qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

care, using the Pessimism scale of the QRS-F Changes in Sibling Involvement
(Friedrich et al., 1983). As shown in Table 2, prior Our third research question was ‘‘How do pat-
to the move, mothers whose son or daughter later terns of sibling involvement change after the adult
moved away from home (Group 2) had a higher moves away from the parental home?’’ For this anal-
level of pessimism than did mothers whose son or ysis, we included those 26 siblings who participated
daughter remained living at home throughout the in both waves of our sibling study and whose broth-
study period (Group 1). Notably, following reloca- er or sister with mental retardation moved away
tion, these mothers’ worries about the future de- from the parental home during the interval between
clined dramatically and dropped below the level of these two waves. As noted, we compared them (us-
worries of co-residing mothers. Post-hoc compari- ing repeated measures ANOVAs) with a randomly
sons indicate that there was a significant reduction selected group of 26 siblings whose brother or sister
of worries between Wave 1 and each of the subse- remained at home during this period. (Due to the
quent waves (all contrasts were at the p , .001 small-size of the sample of siblings whose brother or
level). sister moved away from home between the Time 3
Thus, in response to our second research ques- and Time 6 point of data collection [n 5 26], we
tion, the data suggest that there is stable and fre- checked the pattern of findings reported in Table 3
quent contact between the mother and the son or using nonparametric statistics [Mann-Whitney U
daughter after relocation, a concomitant increase in Test]. The overall pattern of findings that emerged
maternal satisfaction with this level of contact, and in the parametric analysis was replicated with the
a decrease in maternal contact with residential staff. nonparametric tests [data available from authors].)
Although mothers relinquish the hands-on caregiv- As shown in Table 3, the two groups differed
ing role to the formal service system after the son in the sibling’s feelings of emotional involvement
or daughter moves elsewhere, they remain highly with the brother or sister with mental retardation.
involved emotionally with their son or daughter Those siblings whose brother or sister remained liv-
and experience a reduction in their level of worries ing in the parental home throughout the study pe-
about his or her future care. riod reported greater levels of emotional involve-

Table 3 Two-Way Analysis of Variance of Sibling Relationships of Siblings With Co-Resident Versus Re-
located Siblings With Mental Retardation
F
F (Res. Status
Time 3a Time 6b
Sibling (residential F 3 Time
relationships Mean SD Mean SD status) (time) interaction)
Siblings’ level of emo-
tional involvement
Group 1 24.46 3.82 25.96 3.28 7.25, 6.46, 0.45,
Group 2 22.02 4.35 23.29 3.88 p5.010 p5.014 p5.833
Siblings get along
Group 1 4.88 0.95 5.00 0.75 0.92, 3.79, 0.95,
Group 2 4.54 0.99 4.88 1.14 p5.343 p5.057 p5.335
Siblings ‘‘do things to-
gether’’
Group 1 3.23 1.36 3.15 1.19 1.47, 2.78, 5.05,
Group 2 2.56 1.19 3.08 1.04 p5.232 p5.102 p5.029
Worries/pessimism
about the future
Group 1 6.39 2.74 6.56 2.97 1.40, 2.22, 3.84,
Group 2 6.34 2.61 5.04 2.68 p5.243 p5.143 p5.056
All co-resident. bGroup 1 5 co-resident, Group 2 5 relocated.
a

qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation 189


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

ment than those whose brother or sister moved well-documented, change in the family: the
away from home. In addition to this group differ- planned residential relocation of an adult with
ence, there was a significant increase for both mental retardation from the family home while the
groups from Time 3 to Time 6 in the sibling’s feeling mother was well enough to help orchestrate the
of emotional involvement in the sibling relation- move and provide continued support. This transi-
ship. The latter pattern was also found with respect tion typically occurs in most families who do not
to the sibling’s report of how well he or she got have a child with a lifelong disability when the
along with the brother or sister with mental retar- child is in his or her late teens or early 20s and, in
dation, with a trend-level increase, p 5 .057, in family life course theory, is called the ‘‘launching
both groups of siblings’ report of how well they got stage’’ (Carter & McGoldrick, 1999). In the case of
along at Time 6 as compared with Time 3, regardless families of adults with mental retardation, the
of residential status. launching stage is often postponed until the adult
In two other respects, there was a divergent child is in his or her 30s or 40s, primarily due to
pattern over time evidenced by siblings whose parental preference and the limited availability of
brother or sister moved away from the parental suitable options within the community-based resi-
home between Time 3 and Time 6 as compared dential service system.
with siblings whose brother or sister remained at Although most past research on residential
home. First, there was a significant increase in the transitions for persons with mental retardation has
frequency with which siblings ‘‘do things together’’ used the term placement, we have deliberately used
from before to after the time that the brother or the term residential relocation in this paper. It is cur-
sister moved away from the parental home, but no rently more prevalent than in the past for an adult
change in frequency of shared activities among with mental retardation to move from the parents’
those whose brother or sister with mental retarda- home and establish an adult lifestyle characterized
tion continued to live with the parents. Second, by greater independence from the family. The
there was a trend for siblings to feel less worried or launching stage described in this paper occurred
pessimistic about the future care of the brother or while the mothers were healthy enough to help
sister with mental retardation following relocation, plan and oversee the residential transition, provide
p 5 .056, but no change in pessimism for siblings enduring emotional support to their son or daughter
whose brother or sister remained in the parental following the transition, and continue involvement
home. This finding regarding the reduction of sib- in monitoring the quality of care for their son or
ling pessimism following relocation mirrors the pat- daughter. This is an increasingly normative life pat-
terns reported earlier regarding the reduction of ma- tern for adults with mental retardation, and we con-
ternal pessimism following this transition. tend that the language used by social scientists who
Thus, in response to Research Question 3, we study the life patterns of this population also needs
found that sibling relationships tend to improve af- to be ‘‘normalized.’’
fectively over time, regardless of the place of resi- Proponents of life course theory note that tran-
dence of the adult with mental retardation, al- sitions between life stages (in this case, from co-
though the sibling relationship is closer when the residing with parents to being launched into a non-
adult continues to live in the parental home. parental residence) are often times of disequilibrium
Among those adults who moved, there was an in- and stress, as relationships between family members
crease in frequency of shared activities and a de- are re-calibrated and altered by the changing family
crease in the sibling’s pessimism about the brother circumstances (Carter & McGoldrick, 1999). Fur-
or sister’s future care. ther, our analyses indicate that both mothers and
siblings of adults with mental retardation who
moved from the family home, indeed, were more
Discussion worried about the future for their family member
In our longitudinal study of older families of than were mothers and siblings of those who re-
adults with mental retardation, we investigated the mained at home. This may indicate one motivation
impact of lifelong caregiving and the circumstances for the relocation, namely, an acute level of concern
that propel a change in the residential status of and stress about the adult’s future welfare, or, alter-
adults with mental retardation. The present analysis natively, it may reflect normative parental (and in
focused on an increasingly common, though less this instance, sibling) anxiety associated with the

190 qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

launching of a family member. We speculate that adults with mental retardation in this sample is the
the emotional disequilibrium hypothesized in life mothers’ age. At the time of their son or daughter’s
course theory regarding the stresses associated with initial move out of the home, these mothers aver-
the launching stage are as applicable to families of aged 71 years of age. Their personal challenge at
adults with disabilities as they are to families whose this stage of life was to facilitate their adult child’s
children do not have disabilities. adjustment to a life outside of the family home. By
Our analyses indicate that the transition from remaining an active source of emotional support for
the parental home to a nonparental setting is often their son or daughter and maintaining a visible
a planned event, including several steps. Most of presence in his or her new life, these aging mothers
the adults with mental retardation who moved had fulfilled the oft-stated desire of older parents that
their name included on a waiting list for residential before they became ill or died they wanted to see
services, had mothers who visited the prospective their adult child established in a new residence
home several times prior to the move, had them- (Freedman, Krauss, & Seltzer, 1997; Smith, Tobin,
selves visited the new setting (including overnight & Fullmer, 1995). The increased involvement of
trial visits), and had mothers who continued active siblings may have helped to bolster the mother’s
involvement with them and the staff at their new confidence in the future, fostering the transmission
home on a frequent basis throughout the 3- to 4- of caregiving responsibility from one generation to
year study period. The degree of continuity of fam- the next.
ily involvement was, indeed, impressive. This research has a number of implications for
Our research indicates that residential reloca- service providers and policymakers in the field of
tion involves an orchestrated set of events, with a mental retardation/developmental disabilities. First,
gradual transmission of direct caregiving from the the value of planned transitions is highlighted by
family to the residential setting staff. Although the our findings. Advance planning makes it possible
mothers quickly relinquished the responsibility for for both the mother and the individual with mental
direct care, they maintained frequent contact with retardation to have some familiarity with the future
their son or daughter, their emotional involvement
residence before the transition, which may well en-
continued, and their previously elevated worries
hance the likelihood of continued family involve-
about the future for their son or daughter subsided.
ment. However, it was notable that the individual
Furthermore, the siblings of the adults who relocat-
with mental retardation averaged only three pre-
ed remained involved with their brother or sister
transition visits, which may be fewer than the num-
and even increased the frequency with which they
ber of visits made by many adults in the general
did things together after the move. The relocation
population when they decide on a new home to buy
was also associated with a decrease in siblings’ wor-
or rent. Service providers should, therefore, consid-
ries about the future.
er encouraging more advance visits, which may en-
Past research has underscored the importance
of family relationships for the optimal development hance feelings of control and choice in families and
and social/psychological well-being of individuals in the individual with the disability as they nego-
with mental retardation (Hauser et al., 1999; Krauss tiate this significant life transition.
et al., 1992). Our data show that even when indi- Our findings also confirm the importance of
viduals with mental retardation move away physi- siblings to the quality of life of adults with mental
cally from the parental home, they do not move retardation. Siblings’ levels of worry about the fu-
out of the zone of influence of the family. One issue ture mirror those of their mothers, with elevated
to examine in future research is whether family in- levels prior to a move and an abatement of worries
volvement after relocation fosters continued devel- after. That their level of involvement with their
opment of new skills in adults with mental retar- brother or sister with mental retardation increases
dation, in the same way that warm and involved after the move signals their availability as long-term
parents have been shown to have this type of effect sources of support. Service providers would be wise
on their child with developmental disabilities dur- to facilitate sibling involvement, recognizing that
ing the co-resident period (Hauser-Cram et al., the sibling tie is the longest lasting of all relation-
1999). ships, and it is this source of support that will re-
Another distinctive feature of the family con- main constant for the individual with mental re-
text surrounding the residential relocation of the tardation for years to come and may buffer the stress

qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation 191


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

of high staff turnover that is characteristic of the tion; and the abatement of concerns about the fu-
contemporary residential service system. ture for mothers and siblings of adults with mental
Finally, the findings might be useful in educat- retardation following a planned and orchestrated re-
ing service providers about the fidelity of family in- location of the adult with retardation that we have
volvement following lifelong family care. Many reported warrants investigation in studies with larg-
agencies that provide services to individuals with er samples. It is also critical that future research
mental retardation were created in the era of de- include the perspectives of the adults with mental
institutionalization and may still serve individuals retardation themselves. The limitations of the pre-
who previously lived in state institutions whose sent study are counterbalanced, in our view, how-
families may not be highly involved. Therefore, ever, by its prospective design, which permitted us
some service providers may view families with am- to study family interaction patterns before and after
bivalence. As Stoneman and Crapps (1990) report- the relocation occurred and to contrast those pat-
ed earlier, and as confirmed by the present research, terns with families in which a relocation did not
there is a markedly different level of family involve- occur. The low attrition rate in our study, and the
ment when the adult with mental retardation has inclusion of objective and subjective measures of
lived at home throughout his or her life as com- involvement from both mothers and siblings, also
pared with when the adult’s involvement with the enhance the potential generalizability of our results.
family was already disrupted by institutionalization.
Thus, it is important for service providers to ac-
tively encourage the involvement of families, rec- References
ognizing that historical patterns of involvement Aldous, J. (1996). Family careers: Rethinking the de-
may not be characteristic of families whose son or velopmental perspective. Thousand Oaks, CA:
daughter is making the transition today. Sage.
Our study had several limitations and, there- Baker, B., Blacher, J., & Pfeiffer, S. (1993). Family
fore, caution is needed regarding the generalization involvement in residential treatment of chil-
of the findings. First, the samples of mothers and dren with psychiatric disorder and mental re-
particularly of siblings were quite small, and, thus, tardation. Hospital and Community Psychiatry,
there may have been changes that we were not able 44, 561–566.
to detect due to limited statistical power. Second, Baker, B., Blacher, J., & Pfeiffer, S. (1996). Family
we examined a postrelocation period lasting 3 to 4 involvement in residential treatment. American
years. Our data do not indicate the extent of longer Journal on Mental Retardation, 101, 1–14.
range patterns of family involvement. Third, the Baker, B., Heller, T. L., Blacher, J., & Pfeiffer, S. I.
comparison groups differed in critical ways before (1995). Staff attitudes towards family involve-
the adult family member transitioned out of the ment in residential treatment centers for chil-
home, including an elevation in worries about the dren. Psychiatric Services, 46, 60–65.
future for mothers and a lower level of emotional Bengtson, V. L., & Black, K. D. (1973). Solidarity
involvement and frequency of contact for siblings between parents and children: Four perspectives on
of those who moved during the study period. Thus,
theory development. Paper presented to the The-
the between-group comparisons should be inter-
ory Development Workshop, National Council
preted with these differences in mind. Fourth, crit-
on Family Relations, Toronto.
ical voices in the family were not heard in this anal-
Blacher, J., & Baker, B. (1994). Family involvement
ysis, including the fathers and the individuals with
mental retardation. Finally, although there was so- in residential treatment of children with retar-
cioeconomic diversity in the sample, it lacked racial dation: Is there evidence of detachment? Jour-
and ethnic diversity. Therefore, the rates of family nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 505–
involvement among the European American sam- 520.
ple studied here might either underestimate or over- Blacher, J., Baker, B., & Feinfeld, K. A. (1999).
estimate the rates of involvement among families of Leaving or launching? Continuing family in-
color. volvement with children and adolescents in
The pattern of continuity of family relation- placement. American Journal on Mental Retar-
ships; sustained emotional and social involvement dation, 104, 452–465.
among parents, siblings, and adults with retarda- Butterworth, J., Steere, D. E., & Whitney-Thomas,

192 qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

J. (1997). Using person-centered planning to opment in young children with Down syn-
address personal quality of life. In R. L. Schal- drome. Child Development, 70, 97–99.
ock (Ed.), Quality of life, Vol. II: Application to Heller, T., & Factor, A. (1991). Permanency plan-
persons with disabilities (pp. 5–24). Washington, ning for adults with mental retardation living
DC: American Association on Mental Retar- with family caregivers. American Journal on
dation. Mental Retardation, 96, 163–176.
Carter, B., & McGoldrick, M. (1999). The expanded Heller, T., & Factor, A. (1993). Aging family care-
family life cycle: Individual, family and social per- givers: Support resources and changes in bur-
spectives (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. den and placement desire. American Journal on
Eisenberg, L., Baker, B., & Blacher, J. (1998). Sib- Mental Retardation, 98, 417–426.
lings of children with mental retardation living Heller, T., & Factor, A. (1994). Facilitating future
at home or in residential placement. Journal of planning and the transitions out of the home.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 355–363. In M. M. Seltzer, M. W. Krauss, & M. P. Janicki
Englehardt, J., Brubaker, T. H., & Lutzer, V. (1988). (Eds.), Life course perspectives on adulthood and
Older caregivers of adults with mental retar- old age (pp. 39–52). Washington, DC: Ameri-
dation: Service utilization. Mental Retardation, can Association on Mental Retardation.
26, 191–195. Krauss, M. W., & Seltzer, M. M. (1993). Current
Essex, E. L, Seltzer, M. M., & Krauss, M. W. (1997). well–being and future plans of older caregiving
Residential transitions of adults with mental re- mothers. Irish Journal of Psychology, 14, 47–64.
tardation: Predictors of waiting list use and Krauss, M. W., & Seltzer, M. M. (1999). An un-
placement. American Journal on Mental Retar- anticipated life: The impact of lifelong caregiv-
dation, 101, 613–629. ing. In H. Bersani, Jr. (Ed.), Responding to the
Fujiura, G. T., & Braddock, D. (1992). Fiscal and challenge: Current trends and international issues
demographic trends in mental retardation ser- in developmental disabilities (pp. 173–187). Cam-
vices: The emergence of the family. In L. Row- bridge, MA: Brookline Books.
itz (Ed.), Mental retardation in the year 2000 (pp. Krauss, M. W., Seltzer, M. M., & Goodman, S. J.
(1992). Social support networks of adults with
316–338). New York: Springer.
retardation who live at home. American Journal
Freedman, R. I., Krauss, M. W., & Seltzer, M. M.
on Mental Retardation, 96, 432–441.
(1997). Aging parents’ residential plans for
Krauss, M. W., Seltzer, M. M., Gordon, R., & Fried-
adult children with mental retardation. Mental
man, D. (1996). Binding ties: The roles of adult
Retardation, 35, 114–123.
siblings of persons with mental retardation.
Friedrich, W. N., Greenberg, M. T., & Crnic, K.
Mental Retardation, 34, 83–93.
(1983). A short form of the Questionnaire on
Lakin, K. C. (1998). On the outside looking in:
Resources and Stress. American Journal of Men-
Attending to waiting lists in systems of services
tal Deficiency, 88, 41–48.
for people with developmental disabilities.
Gordon, R., Seltzer, M. M., & Krauss, M. W. Mental Retardation, 36, 157–162.
(1997). The aftermath of parental death: Lakin, K. C., Anderson, L., Prouty, R., & Polister,
Changes in the context and quality of life. In B. (1999). Community residential services
R. L. Schalock (Ed.), Quality of life, Volume II: would require expansion of 72% to serve ev-
Application to persons with disabilities (pp. 25– eryone in community settings. Mental Retarda-
42). Washington, DC: American Association tion, 37, 251–254.
on Mental Retardation. Marshak, L. E., Seligman, M., & Prezant, F. (1999).
Griffiths, D., & Unger, D. G. (1994). Views about Disability and the family life cycle. New York: Ba-
planning for the future among parents and sib- sic Books.
lings of adults with mental retardation. Family Pruchno, R. A., Patrick, J. H., & Burant, C. J.
Relations, 43, 221–227. (1996). Mental health of aging women and
Hauser-Cram, P., Warfield, M. E., Shonkoff, J. P., children who are chronically disabled: Exami-
Krauss, M. W., Upshur, C. C., & Sayer, A. nation of a two-factor model. Journal of Ger-
(1999). Family influences on adaptive devel- ontology: Social Sciences, 51B, S284–S296.

qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation 193


MENTAL RETARDATION VOLUME 39, NUMBER 3: 181–194 JUNE 2001
Family involvement M. Seltzer et al.

Seltzer, G. B., Begun, A., Seltzer, M. M., & Krauss, Received 6/5/00, first decision 8/22/00, accepted 9/26/
M. W. (1991). Adults with mental retardation 00.
and their aging mothers: Impacts of siblings. Editor in charge: Steven J. Taylor
Family Relations, 40, 310–317.
Seltzer, M. M., Greenberg, J. S., Krauss, M. M.,
This manuscript is based on a paper presented at the
Gordon, R. M., & Judge, K. (1997). Siblings of
annual meeting of the American Association on Mental
adults with mental retardation or mental ill-
Retardation, May 27, 1999, New Orleans. Support for
ness: Effects on lifestyle and psychological well-
the preparation of this manuscript was provided by the
being. Family Relations, 46, 395–405.
National Institute on Aging (Grant No. R01
Seltzer, M. M., & Krauss, M. W. (1989). Aging par-
AG08768) and the National Institute of Disability and
ents with adult mentally retarded children:
Rehabilitation Research through the Rehabilitation Re-
Family risk factors and sources of support.
search and Training Center on Aging With Develop-
American Journal on Mental Retardation, 94,
303–312. mental Disabilities at the University of Illinois at Chi-
Smith, G. C., Fullmer, E. M., & Tobin, S. S. cago (Grant No. H133B0069).
(1994). Living outside the system: An explo-
ration of older families who do not use day pro-
grams. In M. M. Seltzer, M. W. Krauss, & M. Authors:
P. Janicki (Eds.), Life course perspectives on adult- Marsha Mailick Seltzer, PhD, Acting Director and
hood and old age (pp. 19–37). Washington, DC: Professor, Waisman Center and School of Social
American Association on Mental Retardation. Work (E-mail: mseltzer@waisman.wisc.edu) and
Smith, G. C., Tobin, S. S., & Fullmer, E. M. Jinkuk Hong, PhD, Assistant Researcher, Wais-
(1995). Elderly mothers caring at home for off- man Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison,
spring with mental retardation: A model of per- 1500 Highland Ave., Madison, WI 53705-2280.
manency planning. American Journal on Mental Marty Wyngaarden Krauss, PhD, Associate Dean
Retardation, 99, 487–499. and Professor, Brandeis University, Heller School,
Stoneman, Z., & Crapps, J. (1990). Mentally re- Waltham, MA 02454-9239. Gael I. Orsmond,
tarded individuals in family care homes: Rela- PhD, Assistant Professor, Boston University, Sar-
tionships with the family of origin. Mental Re- gent College, 635 Commonwealth Ave., Boston,
tardation, 94, 420–430. MA 02215.

194 qAmerican Association on Mental Retardation

You might also like