You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Travel Research http://jtr.sagepub.

com/

Reference Group Influences among Chinese Travelers


Cathy H. C. Hsu, Soo K. Kang and Terry Lam
Journal of Travel Research 2006 44: 474
DOI: 10.1177/0047287505282951

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jtr.sagepub.com/content/44/4/474

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Travel and Tourism Research Association

Additional services and information for Journal of Travel Research can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jtr.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jtr.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://jtr.sagepub.com/content/44/4/474.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Apr 7, 2006

What is This?

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


Reference Group Influences
among Chinese Travelers
CATHY H. C. HSU, SOO K. KANG, AND TERRY LAM

Interpersonal influence is known to contribute to an indi- Gabel 1992; Venkatesan 1966). Stafford and Cocanougher
vidual’s psychological formation (e.g., personal value, norm, (1977) contended that consumer behavior cannot be fully
attitude, perception). Reference group influence in the services comprehended unless significant consideration is given to the
sector, however, has received limited attention, with most effects of interpersonal influence on the development of atti-
works conducted in the setting of tangible products, not of tudes, norms, values, aspirations, and purchase behavior.
services. In particular, reference group influences are exerted According to Wernick’s (1994) typology, a trip or vaca-
on a traveler when communication among group members tion is a symbolized commodity, which tends to be planned
provides the opportunity to share direct experiences of others and carried out with a great deal of interpersonal influence.
about a particular destination or service and/or induces the This notion is supported by the finding that word of mouth
selection of a destination or other travel services. Therefore, (WOM) from friends, family members, and relatives is the
the purpose of the study was to segment travelers based on most frequently sought information source among potential
their perceptions of various reference groups’ influences about travelers when preparing for a trip (Bansal and Voyer 2000;
visiting Hong Kong as a destination and to profile each seg- Bieger and Laesser 2004; Dodd 1998; Gitelson and Crompton
ment according to travelers’ benefits sought, attitudes, behav- 1983; Sarigollu and Huang 2005). Sarigollu and Huang (2005)
iors, and sociodemographic characteristics. A TwoStep® cluster used the benefits sought approach to segment travelers to
analysis generated three distinctive segments with different Latin America and concluded that regardless of the segment
benefits sought, perceived behavioral control, attitudes, and generated from a bundle of benefits sought included, WOM
behavioral intentions. Marketing implications for each seg- from friends and family is the most effective information
ment were provided. source for travel decisions. Fodness and Murray (1999) indi-
cated that personal experience, travel agencies, and friends or
Keywords: reference group influence; benefits sought; relatives were the three sources most likely to be used alone.
Chinese travelers; perceived behavioral con- Because the interpersonal influence is known to contribute to
trol; segmentation a potential traveler’s psychological formation (e.g., personal
value, norm, attitude, and perception) and lead to his or her
The characteristics of services—intangibility, perishability, notable decision-making behaviors, marketers may need to
heterogeneity, and simultaneous production and consumption— capitalize on favorable WOM in planning and executing their
lead services to possess higher perceived risks with few objec- marketing strategies and tactics.
tively measurable qualities before purchasing (Mehta, Lalwani,
and Ping 2001). These unique features of service make con-
sumer information search more difficult than that of goods, LITERATURE REVIEW
which leads to a more complex consumer decision-making and
evaluation process (Lovelock 1991).
An important determinant of an individual’s decision- It has been estimated that as much as 80% of all buying
making process is often others’ influences. The fact that con- decisions are influenced by someone’s direct recommenda-
sumers obtain information about products and services from tion (Voss 1984). Although the formal communication chan-
other people, particularly family members, friends, and neigh- nels or mass media aim at persuading prospective buyers
bors, is well documented in the marketing literature (Bayus
1985). Consumers particularly tend to accept information pro- Cathy H. C. Hsu, PhD, is a professor, associate head, and grad-
vided by their peer groups and conform to the group norm on uate programs director in the School of Hotel and Tourism
the quality, style, and other product attributes, which are difficult Management at the Hong Kong Polytechnic University. Soo K.
to evaluate objectively. Perry and Hamm (1969) enunciated Kang, PhD, is an assistant professor in the Department of Food
that the greater the perceived risk of a purchase decision, the Science and Human Nutrition, Colorado State University in Fort
Collins. Terry Lam, PhD, is an associate professor in the School of
higher the degree of personal influence. Consumers who wish Hotel and Tourism Management at the Hong Kong Polytechnic
to reduce prechoice uncertainty may be compelled to seek University. This research was financially supported by the Hong
information from other individuals who have experienced the Kong Polytechnic University (Project No. A-PD25).
service directly or indirectly (Mehta, Lalwani, and Ping 2001). Journal of Travel Research, Vol. 44, May 2006, 474-484
Consumers appear to act in a manner that is consistent DOI: 10.1177/0047287505282951
with the social group with which they identify (Leigh and © 2006 Sage Publications

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 475

through paid advertising, brochures, sales promotions, public consumers than on American consumers for both luxuries and
relations, and the Internet, much research suggested that necessities. Findings of the study also reported that in general,
interpersonal information channels, also known as WOM, for privately consumed goods, familial influence on brand
are as influential on purchase decisions as the formal chan- decisions was stronger in both countries because of the lim-
nels (Bearden and Etzel 1982; Childers and Rao 1992; Leigh ited opportunity to observe the brand preference of peers. As
and Gabel 1992) because of an extensive amount of infor- an extension of Bearden and Etzel’s (1982) study, Mehta,
mation accessible to individuals informally through their Lalwani, and Ping (2001) investigated the relationships between
family, friends, and reference groups, or the groups of people consumers’ susceptibility to reference group influence and the
with whom they interact at work and socially (Middleton perceived risk of services on consumer behavior for four
2002). service categories (i.e., fine dining, beauty care services, a
Reference groups are defined as social groups that are haircut, and dental care services) among working women in
important to a consumer and against which he or she compares Singapore. Findings of the study showed that fine dining was
him or herself in forming attitudes and behaviors (Escalas identified as the most susceptible decision to be influenced
and Bettman 2003). Among various reference groups exert- by reference groups. They concluded that the finding is espe-
ing significant influences on the consumer decision-making cially common in Asian societies, where consumers are less
process, parents, teachers, and friends (or peers) are represen- individualistic and adhere more to social norms, and where
tatives of normative referents. Furthermore, family remains their closely knit family structure and ties may help explain
as the most significant reference group because an individ- this finding.
ual’s values and expectations are built through family influ- Hispanic consumers are subject to more family and
ences (Moutinho 1987). Reference groups are also claimed peer influences compared to white consumers (Hoyer and
as a major source of personal norms, attitudes, and values Deshpande 1982). Black consumers were also found to be
through direct interactions (Kelly 1947; Kemper 1968; Park strongly attached to their family values, thus being influ-
and Lessig 1977), which was empirically confirmed and sup- enced strongly by their reference groups’ opinions compared
ported by numerous marketing, psychology, and sociology to their white counterparts (Fisher 1996). Reference groups
research studies (e.g., Bearden and Etzel 1982; Burnkrant and with high credibility, such as those having presumed exper-
Cousineau 1975; Childers and Rao 1992; Johar and Sirgy tise (e.g., travel agents), often serve as sources of information-
1991; Leigh and Gabel 1992; Lessig and Park 1978; Mehta, based influence for uncertain or uninformed consumers
Lalwani, and Ping 2001). (Childers and Rao 1992), whereas family and peers tend to
be sources for utilitarian and value-expressive functions
(Johar and Sirgy 1991). Kim and Kang (2001) investigated
Reference Group Influence Research three main ethnic consumer groups (i.e., blacks, Hispanics,
The degree of reference group influences was reported and whites) in their clothes and small electronics purchasing
to vary across different product types and social-cultural decisions. Although no significant differences were found
groups. With regard to product types, one of the seminal among the three groups on familial reference group influ-
works on reference group influence in consumer marketing is ences, white and black consumers were more influenced
Bearden and Etzel’s (1982) research, which has served as a than Hispanics by their friends and coworkers overall, which
foundation for various works in the arena (e.g., Childers and was inconsistent with findings from Hoyer and Deshpande
Rao 1992; Leigh and Gabel 1992; Mehta, Lalwani, and Ping (1982).
2001). They examined how reference group influences var-
ied for products consumed publicly versus privately and for Reference Group Influences in
products considered as luxuries versus necessities. Results of Travel and Tourism Research
their study showed that products and brands selected by indi-
viduals could be influenced to different degrees by their ref- Numerous studies have been conducted on reference group
erence groups. Specifically, the reference group influence influence in conjunction with information search behavior
was stronger both in publicly consumed necessities (e.g., a during a trip- or vacation-planning process (e.g., Fodness and
haircut) and luxuries (e.g., fine dining) than in privately con- Murray 1997; Moutinho 1987; Nicholas and Snepenger 1988).
sumed products (e.g., a toothbrush). This finding may be Assael (1987) enunciated that different information search
explained by the fact that consumers were able to observe the strategies can serve as a useful basis to categorize vacationers
products and brands purchased by reference group members because this is the means by which service and product offer-
or to interact with referents regarding the appropriate prod- ings are communicated to the vacationers. As a result, an exam-
ucts and brands to buy. Johar and Sirgy (1991) also reported ination of information sources in traveler decision making has
similar findings supporting this result that the purchase of been popularly used as a market segmentation criterion (e.g.,
publicly consumed or socially visible products, due to their Bieger and Laesser 2004; Dodd 1998; Fodness and Murray
value expressiveness, received more reference group influ- 1997).
ence than did the purchase of privately consumed products. Snepenger et al. (1990) investigated three information search
From a social-cultural perspective, Childers and Rao strategies among first-time Alaska visitors (i.e., destination-
(1992) studied the simultaneous effects of ethnicity and prod- naïve vacationers). Findings of the study indicated that travel
uct on consumers’ purchase decision making by examining agents were a pivotal source of information sought among
two types of reference group influences—familial and peer respondents. In particular, those who used travel agents were
based—with consumers in Thailand and the United States. mostly women, and they differed from others who used differ-
Peer influence was stronger on products considered as a lux- ent information sources in terms of benefits sought, trip char-
ury for American consumers compared to their Thai counter- acteristics (e.g., expenditure and length of stay), and the degree
parts, whereas familial influence was more significant on Thai of activity involvement.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


476 MAY 2006

Gitelson and Crompton (1983) reported that family influences and its consequences on travel/tourism marketing
groups rely more on media information for their vacation should be investigated and highlighted.
information, whereas people older than 50 consulted travel
agents for information, and college-educated travelers used
specific destination-produced brochures, materials, and/or RESEARCH PURPOSES
literature. Woodside and Ronkainen (1980) identified that AND OBJECTIVES
white-collar travelers appeared to use travel agencies exten-
sively compared to their counterparts. Shoemaker (1984) The purpose of the study was to segment travelers based
found that older travelers were influenced by travel agents on their perceptions of various reference groups’ influences
regarding their during-trip decisions, such as travel modes, about selecting Hong Kong as a travel destination, and to pro-
attractions, and lodging. In addition, families and friends file each segment according to travelers’ benefits sought, atti-
were identified as a great influence source on vacation des- tudes, behaviors, as well as sociodemographic characteristics.
tinations for those older than 60. Specific objectives of the study were to (1) segment travelers
Although WOM is a significant information source among based on respondents’ perceptions about three major reference
travelers, no existing study was found regarding how reference groups’ influences and their likelihood to comply, (2) investi-
group influences affected travelers’ decisions in choosing gate the relationships between traveler benefits sought and
a travel destination and further their associations with other reference group segments, (3) identify differences on per-
perceptual-behavioral variables. This one-dimensional approach ceived behavioral control by the emergent segments, (4) exam-
could be complemented by examining in detail the reference ine differences of attitudes and behavioral intentions among
group influence construct. the resultant segments, and (5) identify differences among the
segments by their sociodemographic characteristics and past
travel experiences.
RATIONALE FOR RESEARCH

As mentioned earlier, the recognition of reference group METHODOLOGY


influence has led to a proliferation of research in psychology
(Batra, Homer, and Kahle 2001; Quellett and Wood 1998), Research Instrument
sociology (e.g., Cochran et al. 2004), and consumer behavior
(e.g., Bearden and Etzel 1982; Childers and Rao 1992; Hoyer A questionnaire was developed based on previous research
and Deshpande 1982; Johar and Sirgy 1991). Reference group in the fields of tourism marketing and behaviors. The con-
influence in the services sector has, however, received limited struct of reference group influence was assessed from two
attention, with most works conducted in the setting of tangible perspectives: (1) a respondent’s perception about specific ref-
products, not of services (Mehta, Lalwani, and Ping 2001). In erents’ (i.e., family, friends/relatives, and travel agents) opin-
the services sector, the topic has been investigated within a cer- ions on whether a respondent should or should not visit Hong
tain boundary, mainly focusing on beauty services, life insur- Kong, and (2) a respondent’s willingness to comply with each
ance, legal services, dry cleaners, and banks (Mitchell and referent’s opinion about visiting Hong Kong in the future. The
Greatorex 1993). Limited literature on travel and tourism perception about referents’ opinions (“My family, friends/
products and services regarding reference group influences relatives, or travel agent thinks I should/should not choose
was identified. Hong Kong as a travel destination”) was measured with a
Travel decisions are strongly affected by external factors 7-point Likert-type scale (7 = should and 1 = should not). The
because travel, one of the exemplary service products, is a likelihood to comply with what referents say about traveling
purely intangible consumption (Moutinho 1987). In addition, to Hong Kong (“The likelihood for me to listen to what my fam-
WOM from reference groups is one of the primary sources ily, friends/relatives, travel agents, say(s) about my visit to
from which travelers acquire information when making Hong Kong is . . .”) was assessed with another 7-point Likert-
purchasing decisions. Reference group influences are exerted type scale (7 = extremely likely and 1 = extremely unlikely).
on a traveler when communication among group members The 16-item benefits-sought measurement was adapted
induces the selection of a destination or other services, and/or from Zhang and Lam’s (1999) study that investigated Chinese
provides the opportunity to share direct experiences of others travelers’ motivation to visit Hong Kong. Respondents were
about a particular destination or service (Moutinho 1987). asked to rate the importance of each benefit-sought statement
Although the type of information sources sought by travelers during their visit to Hong Kong with a 7-point Likert-type
is a popular variable in tourist segmentation studies, no stud- scale (7 = very important and 1 = very unimportant).
ies attempted to segment travelers by the reference group Perception about behavioral control on the possibility of
influences. This study also delineated the segments in terms visiting Hong Kong was defined as the perception of whether
of benefits sought, attitude, behavioral intention, and sociode- one possesses necessary resources and opportunities to visit
mographic characteristics. The profiling of segments by dif- Hong Kong (Ajzen 1985). This construct was evaluated
ferent perceptual and behavioral interests complements the by using five statements. One example of the statements is
reference group influence and helps destination marketers and “There are factors outside of my control that could prevent
suppliers to develop more attractive packages and pro- me from visiting Hong Kong,” rated from 7 (strongly agree)
motional strategies. These are more likely to be successful to 1 (strongly disagree). A set of semantic differential scales
because they are based on travelers’ benefits sought, attitudes, was used to assess respondents’ attitudes toward Hong Kong
and opinions, and are more conducive to predicting travel as a travel destination. Five statements were used to measure
behavior (Abbey 1979; Sarigollu and Huang 2005; Woodside the concept “All things considered, I think visiting Hong
and Pitts 1976). Therefore, the importance of reference group Kong would be . . .”; these were (1) enjoyable-unenjoyable,

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 477

(2) positive-negative, (3) fun-boring, (4) pleasant-unpleasant, First, a series of descriptive analysis, including frequencies,
and (5) favorable-unfavorable. In addition, three questions means, and standard deviations, was conducted for all survey
were used to identify behavioral intention to visit Hong Kong, questions. Second, a factor analysis with VARIMAX rotation
with a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 7 (strongly was carried out on the 16 benefits-sought items to detect under-
agree) to 1 (strongly disagree). A sample statement is “I want lying dimensions. Third, TwoStep® cluster analysis with a
to visit Hong Kong.” Euclidean distance method was conducted to segment respon-
Demographic questions, including age, gender, education, dents into mutually exclusive groups according to their per-
income, and marital status, were also included. Past travel ception on reference groups’ opinions about visiting Hong
experience was identified with two questions adapted from Kong and their likelihood to comply with those opinions.
Quellett and Wood (1998). Respondents were first asked to The TwoStep® cluster analysis is a relatively new cluster-
indicate whether they had ever visited Hong Kong, and the ing technique consisting of two stages: (1) preclustering the
choices were “Yes” and “No.” If the response was positive, cases (or records) into many small subclusters by constructing
respondents were then asked of the frequency of their visit(s) a cluster features (CF) tree, and (2) clustering the subclusters
with the question “How many times have you visited Hong resulting from the first stage into a desired number of clusters—
Kong in the past?” Five frequency categories were provided: a probabilistic hierarchical cluster analysis (Chiu et al. 2001;
once, 2-3 times, 4-5 times, 6-10 times, and more than 10 times. SPSS 2003). One of the advantages of using the TwoStep®
Responses from the two questions were recoded into five cat- cluster analysis is that it is applicable with a relatively large
egories, never, once, 2-3 times, 4-5 times, 6-10 times, and data set (n = 464 in this study), compared to conventional
more than 10 times. Respondents who had visited Hong Kong cluster analyses, in reducing processing time. The TwoStep®
were also asked to identify the time of last visit with five cluster analysis automatically chooses the “best” number of
choices, within the past 6 months, 7 months to 1 year ago, 2 clusters (like hierarchical cluster analysis) by examining the
to 3 years ago, 4 to 5 years ago, and more than 5 years ago. Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) values. In interpreting
BIC scores, the smaller values of the BIC indicate better
models, and the “best” cluster solution has the smallest BIC.
Data Collection In addition, ratios of BIC changes and ratios of distance
Brislin’s (1976) blind translation-back-translation method measures are evaluated to determine the “best” number of
was used to translate the questionnaire into Chinese. A pilot clusters.
study with 28 mainland Chinese residents was conducted. The A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was then
instrument was revised based on feedback to enhance the for- carried out to determine whether significant differences existed
mat and layout of the scales and question clarity. A second among emergent clusters by factor mean scores. MANOVAs
pilot test was then carried out with 20 Chinese travelers in were also used to identify significant differences among the
Hong Kong. Three cities—Beijing, Shanghai, and Guangzhou, emergent three clusters by respondents’ perceived behavioral
China—were chosen for data collection. These three cities control of, attitudes toward, and behavioral intention of visit-
were selected because the respective disposable income ing Hong Kong. Finally, a series of chi-square analyses was
per capita in Beijing and Shanghai municipalities as well as conducted to examine the emergent clusters by sociodemo-
Guangdong Province, of which Guangzhou is the capital, was graphic characteristics and past travel experiences.
among the highest in China (National Bureau of Statistics of
China 2003).
A self-administered questionnaire was used to collect the RESULTS
data. In China, travel agencies have grown rapidly and become
the essential agents for handling outbound travel arrangements Demographic Profile
(Zhang 2004). To identify potential travelers, individuals in
shopping malls where travel agencies were located were inter- Almost equal numbers of each gender participated in the
cepted with no particular pattern to invite their participation in study (male = 50.4% and female = 48.9%). Slightly more than
the study. The acceptance rate of the invitation was approxi- half (54.0%) of the respondents were younger than 30, with
mately 60%. Once potential respondents agreed, they were 38.6% between 30 and 49. Approximately half (50.1%) of the
screened by asking if they had heard of Hong Kong and knew respondents were single, with 42.6% being married. About
where it was. Only individuals who had some awareness two-thirds (62.4%) were college/university educated or higher,
of Hong Kong were asked to complete the questionnaire. and 70.0% reported a monthly income of less than 3,001RMB
Interestingly, all potential respondents knew that Hong Kong (approximately US$370).
is a special administrative region of China, bordering the main- When asked about their previous travel experience to Hong
land Chinese city of Shenzhen. Once potential respondents Kong, almost three-fourths (74.2%) indicated that they had
were selected, a briefing was then given to introduce the pur- never visited Hong Kong. Of those who had visited Hong
pose of the study and provide directions for completing the Kong (24.4%), nearly half (47.9%) of them had visited once,
questionnaire. To ensure a high return and usable rate, the and 25.2% had visited 2 to 3 times. Also, three-fourths (73.1%)
questionnaires were collected onsite and checked for com- of the previous visitors indicated that their last trip to Hong
pleteness. A total of 464 questionnaires were completed and Kong had occurred during the past year.
used in ensuing data analysis.
Benefits-Sought Dimensions
Data Analysis (Factor Analysis)
All data collected were analyzed using the Statistical The appropriateness of factor analysis was determined
Package for Social Sciences for Windows 12.0 (SPSS 2003). by examining the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


478 MAY 2006

TABLE 1
RESPONDENTS’ DEMOGRAPHIC AND PAST TRAVEL CHARACTERISTICS

Characteristics (N = 464) %

Gender (n = 460) Male 48.9


Female 50.4
Age (n = 456) 18 or younger 5.3
19-29 48.7
30-39 25.0
40-49 13.6
50-59 5.9
60 or older 1.3
Marital status (n = 455) Single 50.1
Married 42.6
Divorced 4.4
Widowed 1.3
Education (n = 452) Primary school or below 3.6
Secondary school 11.9
Technical institution 22.3
Tertiary college 39.6
University or higher 22.8
Average monthly income (n = 451) Not steady 4.4
1,000 or less RMBa 10.6
1,001 to 2,000 RMB 26.2
2,001 to 3,000 RMB 28.8
3,001 to 4,000 RMB 14.2
4,001 or more RMB 15.7
Previous visit to Hong Kong (n = 454) Yes 24.4
No 74.2
Number of trips to Hong Kong (n = 119) Once 47.9
2-3 times 25.2
4-5 times 11.8
6-10 times 4.2
More than 10 times 10.1
Last trip to Hong Kong (n = 115) Within the past 6 months 47.9
7 months–1 year ago 25.2
2 to 3 years ago 11.8
4 to 5 years ago 4.2
More than 5 years ago 10.1

a. RMB 1,000 = US$123.00.

sampling adequacy and the Bartlett’s test of sphericity. A value group influences. A three-cluster option was chosen because
of .60 or higher from the KMO measure of sampling adequacy it had the smallest BIC (1,418.12) value, with a relatively
test indicates that the data are appropriate for factor analysis large ratio of BIC change (.353) and of distance measure
(Tabachnick and Fidel 1989). A significant Bartlett’s test of (2.711) (see Table 3). Specifically, the first cluster accounted
sphericity (3,017, p < .000) also fulfilled the requirement for for 43.3% (n = 201) of the respondents, the second cluster
factor analysis. represented 28.7% (n = 133), and the third cluster included
Results of the factor analysis with VARIMAX rotation 28.0% (n = 130).
generated three factors with Eigenvalues greater than 1.0 and Overall, among the three clusters, respondents in cluster
each loading value exceeding .40 (Hair et al. 2002; Hatcher 1 showed the strongest belief that their reference groups had
and Stepansk 1994). None of the items had a loading lower positive opinions about visiting Hong Kong, whereas those
than .40 on any of the three factors. As a result, the three fac- in cluster 3 perceived that their reference groups were least
tors represented 16 items with 63.82% of the total variance positive about them traveling to Hong Kong in the future
explained (see Table 2). Reliability alphas of the three factors (see Figure 1). With respect to respondents’ likelihood to
were .86, .88, and .88, respectively. The three factors emerged comply with what their reference groups opined about their
were labeled general exposure, culture/system experience, and visit to Hong Kong, members in cluster 2 indicated that they
socialization/entertainment, respectively. were more likely to comply with their reference groups’
views. Alternatively, those in cluster 3 showed the least desire
Reference Group Influences to comply with their referents’ opinions.
(Cluster Analysis) Among the three types of reference groups examined, family,
friends/relatives, and travel agents, cluster 1 members per-
The TwoStep® cluster analysis was conducted to seg- ceived most positive opinions from all three referents, whereas
ment respondents by their perceptions on three reference those in cluster 3 indicated the lowest ratings about whether

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 479

TABLE 2
FACTOR ANALYSIS OF BENEFITS SOUGHT
a
Benefits Sought Factor Loading Eigenvalue % of Variance Factor Mean by Clusterb

General exposure (a = .860) 6.471 24.90


To fulfill own curiosity about Hong Kong .806 C1 = 4.80
To sightsee .734 C2 = 5.24
To visit the metropolitan city .687 C3 = 4.10
To interact with Hong Kong people .675
To experience a new and different lifestyle .665
To see movie stars and celebrities .599

Culture/system experience (a = .884) 1.386 22.74


To see the city under one-country-two-systems .859 C1 = 4.78
To experience the capitalist society .829 C2 = 5.18
To experience different culture .816 C3 = 4.02
To experience the mysterious city .645

Socialization/entertainment (a = .883) 1.078 16.18


To shop .861 C1 = 4.73
To share travel experience with friends .702 C2 = 4.89
To visit the Ocean Park .514 C3 = 3.66
To visit friends and relatives .429

Total variance explained 63.82


Total scale reliability .92
Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) = .909
Bartlett’s Test = 3,017, p < .000

a. Scale: 7 = very important; 1 = very unimportant.


b. C1: family-influenced visitors. C2: friends/relatives-influenced visitors. C3: independent-thinking visitors.

TABLE 3
CLUSTER SELECTION WITH BIC VALUES

Schwarz’s Bayesian Ratio of BIC Ratio of Distance


Number of Clusters Criterion (BIC) BIC Changea Changesb Measuresc
1 2,000.399
2 1,643.922 –356.477 1.000 2.156
3 1,418.119 –125.803 .353 2.711
4 1,475.194 –42.925 .120 1.544
5 1,473.359 –1.836 .005 1.284
6 1,488.229 140.870 –.042 1.090
7 1,507.957 19.728 –.055 1.110
8 1,533.042 25.085 –.070 1.193
9 1,565.986 32.944 –.092 1.191
10 1,605.459 39.473 –.111 1.025
11 1,645.753 40.294 –.113 1.005
12 1,686.219 40.467 –.114 1.135
13 1,730.634 44.415 –.125 1.170
14 1,779.299 48.666 –.137 1.062
15 1,829.430 50.130 –.141 1.083

a. Changes are from the previous number of clusters in the table.


b. Ratios of changes are relative to the change for the two-cluster solution.
c. Ratios of distance measures are based on the current number of clusters against the previous
number of clusters.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


480 MAY 2006

FIGURE 1
THREE CLUSTERS BY PERCEPTION OF REFERENCE GROUPS AND LIKELIHOOD TO COMPLY

Family Family Family Friends/relatives


6 Influenced Influenced Influenced Influenced
Friends/relatives Friends/relatives
Influenced 5.66 Influenced
5.56 5.60 5.53
5 5.26
5.12

Independent Independent Independent


4
Thinking Thinking Thinking
3.48 3.47 3.50
3

0
Family Influence Friends/Relatives Influence Travel Agents Influence

Scale: 7 = should, 1 = should not

Friends/relatives Friends/relatives
Influenced Friends/relatives
6 Influenced
Influenced
5.85
Family 5.66 5.50
5 Influenced Family
Influenced Family
4.61 Independent Independent
4 Influenced
Thinking 4.13 Thinking Independent
3.64 Thinking
3 3.41 3.45
3.02

0
Comply to Family Comply to Friends/Relatives Comply to Travel Agents

Scale: 7 = extremely likely, 1 = extremely unlikely

they should visit Hong Kong. With regard to respondents’ three factor mean scores. Findings of the analysis indicated
willingness to comply, members of the three clusters showed that the three factor scores were significantly different among
more differences, even though motivation to comply with the three clusters (F = 19.62, p < .000). For all three benefits-
travel agents’ opinions was the lowest for all three clusters. sought factors, respondents in the friends/relatives-influenced
Those in cluster 1 showed that they were more likely to be visitors cluster indicated the highest importance on all three
attentive to what their family says about visiting Hong Kong. dimensions, whereas those in the independent-thinking visitors
Consequently, cluster 1 was labeled family-influenced visi- cluster indicated the lowest importance (see factor means of
tors. Respondents in cluster 2 indicated that they were more each cluster in Table 2).
likely to listen to their friends’/relatives’ opinions about visit-
ing Hong Kong. Therefore, this cluster was named friends/ Perceived Behavioral Control (MANOVA)
relatives-influenced visitors. cluster 3 was named independent-
thinking visitors because respondents in this cluster indicated Perceived behavioral control is the perception of ease or
that they were neutral in their motivation to comply with their difficulty in performing a behavior (Ajzen 1991; Shim et al.
reference groups. 2001). Therefore, the level of perceived behavioral control
To identify whether each cluster showed differences on the that an individual feels can play the role of a motivator or
benefits-sought factors, a MANOVA was conducted with the barrier in purchasing decisions (e.g., Shim et al. 2001).

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 481

TABLE 4
PERCEIVED BEHAVIORAL CONTROL BY CLUSTER

Friends/
Family-Influenced Relatives-Influenced Independent-Thinking
Perceived Behavioral Control Visitors Visitors Visitors F Value
The likelihood that I shall be able to visit 5.42e 5.13f 4.44ef 14.07***
Hong Kong in the future is . . .a
Visit to Hong Kong is . . .b 4.89e 4.81f 4.17ef 8.10***
If I want to, I could easily visit 4.31f 4.08 3.58f 5.15*
Hong Kong from now on.c
There are plenty of opportunities for me 3.78e 3.80f 3.16ef 4.77*
to visit Hong Kong.c
The amount of control I have on whether or not 5.00 4.74 4.56 2.03
I shall visit Hong Kong in the future is . . .d
Wilks’s lambda = .926
F = 3.54
p < .000

a. Scale: 7 = very likely; 1 = very unlikely.


b. Scale: 7 = very easy; 1 = very difficult.
c. Scale: 7 = strongly agree; 1 = strongly disagree.
d. Scale: 7 = complete control; 1 = very little control.
e. Means in the same row followed by the same superscript were significantly different at p < .05.
f. Means in the same row followed by the same superscript were significantly different at p < .05.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

To identify how the emergent segments differed on their Results of the MANOVA on respondents’ intention
perceived behavioral control about visiting Hong Kong, a of visiting Hong Kong also indicated that the three clusters
MANOVA was conducted with cluster membership as an were significantly different on all three statements (Wilks’s
independent variable and the five statements of perceived lambda = .836, F = 13.20, p < .000). Those in the independent-
behavioral control as dependent variables. Findings of the thinking visitors cluster, again, showed the least positive res-
analysis (see Table 4) indicated that three segments differed on ponse for future trips to Hong Kong, whereas respondents in
four of the five statements (Wilks’s lambda = .926, F = 3.54, the other two clusters indicated stronger intention to visit Hong
p < .000). Kong in the future.
Specifically, family-influenced visitors indicated higher
ratings on four behavioral control statements compared to the Profile by Sociodemographics
other visitor segments. Independent-thinking visitors, in con-
trast, showed the lowest means for all perceived behavioral and Past Travel Experiences
control items. This finding appeared to be parallel to the ref- (Chi-square Analysis)
erence group influence perceptions (see Figure 1). Independent-
No significant differences were found on demographic
thinking travelers perceived an overall lower level of behav-
characteristics that could distinguish the three clusters. This
ioral control and motivation to comply with reference groups finding was interesting because previous studies indicated that
when it came to the decision of visiting Hong Kong. These social-economic status may dictate to some degree the level of
travelers may have different psychographic characteristics reference group influences and susceptibility (Kemper 1968;
that may affect their perceived self–behavioral control as well Leigh and Gabel 1992; Perry and Hamm 1969). Because few
as external influences exerted by others. studies on reference group influences were conducted in Asian
countries, previous study results may only apply to Western
Overall Attitude and Intention cultures.
to Visit (MANOVA) In addition, no significant differences were found among
the three clusters by past travel experiences to Hong Kong.
A series of MANOVAs used cluster membership as the This result was also unexpected because people with more
independent variable, and five and three statements on travel- travel experience tend to rely more on their internal informa-
ers’ attitude toward and behavioral intention of visiting Hong tion (i.e., retained memory) and are less likely to be influ-
Kong, respectively, as the dependent variables (see Tables 5 enced by external information sources (Hayes-Roth 1977;
and 6). First, in the examination of significant differences on Moutinho 1987). In contrast, those who have no experience
respondents’ attitude of visiting Hong Kong, results of the with a product are more likely to be influenced by personal
MANOVA showed significant differences on attitude among and/or social sources, such as salespersons, agents, reference
the clusters (Wilks’s lambda = .792, F = 10.83, p < .000). groups, or family members who are trustworthy and easy to
Overall, respondents in the independent-thinking visitors cluster understand (Alba and Hutchinson 1987; Bettman and Park
showed the least positive attitude toward visiting Hong Kong, 1980). The unique characteristics of the sample—a major-
whereas their counterparts in the other two clusters showed ity of them had never visited Hong Kong (74%) and were
more positive attitudes on all five statements. younger than 30 (54%)—may offer some level of explanation

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


482 MAY 2006

TABLE 5
ATTITUDE OF VISITING HONG KONG BY CLUSTER

All Things Considered, I Think Family-Influenced Friends/Relatives-Influenced Independent-Thinking


Visiting Hong Kong Would Be . . .a Visitors Visitors Visitors F Value
b c bc
Enjoyable—Unenjoyable 5.48 5.62 4.35 39.86***
Positive—Negative 5.53b 5.48c 4.41bc 32.42***
Fun—Boring 5.60b 5.30c 4.39bc 26.22***
Pleasant—Unpleasant 5.55b 5.56c 4.63bc 22.67***
Favorable—Unfavorable 5.67b 5.73c 4.57bc 30.31***
Wilks’s lambda = .792
F = 10.83
p < .000

a. Scale: 7 = positive descriptor, 1 = negative descriptor


b. Means in the same row followed by the same superscript were significantly different at p < .05.
c. Means in the same row followed by the same superscript were significantly different at p < .05.
***p < .001.

TABLE 6
BEHAVIORAL INTENTION ON VISITING HONG KONG BY CLUSTER

Friends/
Family-Influenced Relatives-Influenced Independent-Thinking
Behavioral Intentiona Visitors Visitors Visitors F value
I intend to visit Hong Kong within 4.51b 4.25c 3.20bc 21.35***
the next 12 months.
I want to visit Hong Kong. 5.78b 5.95c 4.73bc 29.35***
It is likely that I will visit Hong Kong 4.45b 4.59c 3.82bc 9.29***
in the next 12 months.
Wilks’s lambda = .836
F = 13.20
p < .000

a. Scale: 7 = extremely agree; 1 = extremely disagree.


b. Means in the same row followed by the same superscript were significantly different at p < .05.
c. Means in the same row followed by the same superscript were significantly different at p < .05.
***p < .001.

for this result. More research should be conducted to delineate when it comes to the decision of choosing Hong Kong as a
possible cultural implications on this issue. travel destination. Respondents were more likely to comply
with their primary reference group’s (i.e., family and friends/
relatives in this study) opinions than their secondary reference
group’s (i.e., travel agents) views regarding visiting Hong
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS Kong. Findings of this study also indicated that the three trav-
eler segments, categorized by respondents’ perceptions about
Consumers are more likely to use reference groups for reference groups’ opinions and susceptibility to their opinions,
services than for goods (Guseman 1981). Perry and Hamm showed different benefits sought, perceived behavioral con-
(1969) also stated that the greater the perceived risk of a pur- trol, overall attitude, and intent to visit a destination. This
chase decision (e.g., service), the higher the degree of per- supported the assertion that reference group influences are
sonal influence becomes, emphasizing the importance of strongly related to an individual’s value, attitude, motivation,
interpersonal (reference group) influences in the service perception, and other psychological traits in the travel and
consumption process. The dearth of research in travel and tourism context (Bieger and Laesser 2004; Middleton 2002).
tourism literature about reference group influences, however, Therefore, destination managers should monitor how their
suggests that limited information is available to marketers target markets interact with their reference groups in dissemi-
and other professionals in understanding the magnitude and nating or collecting travel-related information. Results of the
nature of interpersonal influences in the traveler decision- study also made an important contribution to the literature on
making process. reference group influences by placing them as an important
This study investigated Chinese travelers’ perception on segmentation criterion that may be conducive to explain trav-
three reference groups’ opinions and motivations to comply elers’ perceptual and behavioral differences.
with them regarding the trip decision to Hong Kong. Overall, In contrast with what was reported earlier, sociodemo-
different reference groups’ opinions were perceived differently graphic characteristics (i.e., gender, age, income, education,

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 483

and marital status) and previous travel experiences were not LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH
significantly different among the three cluster groups sur-
veyed in this study. Particularly, it was reported that women
are more communal in nature with a stronger affiliation ori- Several cautionary remarks should be made in interpreting
entation (McClelland 1975), thereby showing a higher level the results of the study. First, this study used only three refer-
of susceptibility to reference group influences; this was not ence groups, excluding other reference group influences that
confirmed in this study. These unprecedented findings sug- may affect Chinese travelers’ destination selection decisions.
gested that objective descriptors, such as sociodemographic The inclusion of external promotional communications to
characteristics used in this study, could not fully explain the compare the extent of their influence with that of the reference
level of reference group influences, which may differ by groups will generate a more comprehensive picture to judge
decision types. Some consumers are also more independent the importance of reference group influences in the traveler
and autonomous in nature. This provision warrants future decision-making process.
studies that include psychological or personal traits, which Hong Kong was the only travel destination included in the
were not investigated in the study, to further enunciate ref- questionnaire. Because Hong Kong is part of China’s territory,
erence group influences in the travel and tourism arena. even though traveling to Hong Kong still requires special
The friends/relatives-influenced visitors were most likely travel documents for some, trips to Hong Kong may not have
to comply with all groups’ opinions. These individuals are an been seen as having a high level of risk as compared to travel-
important target market due to their susceptibility to peer pres- ing to other countries. Therefore, comparison with other domes-
sure and the market size (43.3% of respondents). Because no tic or international travel destinations will provide important
significant differences were found between the friends/ benchmarks to contextualize the understanding of the findings.
relatives-influenced visitors and family-influenced visitors in Future studies may also use more diverse nationalities to inves-
terms of perceived control, attitude, and intention, these two tigate cultural differences on travel decisions to provide cross-
potential markets could be targeted together before further cultural implications. In addition, as more mainland Chinese
distinguishing factors become available. To induce these two travel to Hong Kong, the nature of information sought will
market segments, marketers should be attentive to existing change; people will seek more specific information regarding
WOM activities in the marketplace. activities to participate in, restaurants to dine at, hotels to stay
Traveling to Hong Kong is still regarded as a luxury among in, places to visit, and shopping outlets, as opposed to a gen-
some Chinese. For luxury services, financial and psychologi- eral inquiry about whether they should visit Hong Kong. Thus,
cal risks are considered high. Although it is difficult to mini- future research could include various trip/travel components to
mize financial risks because prices are influenced by a number further delineate reference group influences as an important
of factors, including costs, quality perception, and competi- decision-making determinant. Furthermore, this study only
tion, psychological risks can be reduced by positive WOM considered perceived behavioral control as a generic concept;
from family members, relatives, friends, and colleagues. Travel more research is warranted to delineate the exact nature of
agents appeared to exert the least influence among Chinese “control” factors, such as time, resources, logistics, and travel
travelers; thus, travel agents should encourage their existing permission.
customers to engage in WOM activities about visiting Hong Due to the small sample size from limited data collection
Kong as positive experience carriers rather than try to persuade sites (i.e., shopping malls), generalization of results to all
customers directly. Another tactic they may use is to use testi- potential Chinese travelers to Hong Kong is uncalled for.
monials from their previous customers as a pull motivation Probabilistic sampling design, such as geographical cluster
trigger. and random digit dialing, could be considered for future
Respondents in the independent-thinking visitors cluster research. Additional studies could also expand the geo-
were least susceptible to reference groups and had the least graphic coverage in data collection because residents in rural
favorable attitude toward and intention to visit Hong Kong. areas may have different opinions and travel behavior. Finally,
It is, however, too risky to conclude that they are not subject the majority of respondents in this study were relatively
to other types of reference group influences. Therefore, it is young (younger than 40); therefore, future studies with a
the marketers’ task to identify what would make those trav- more representative sample could further fine-tune the results
elers interested in visiting Hong Kong by examining other of this study.
factors that have been known to influence travelers’ destination
selection. REFERENCES
The unexpected findings contradicting results of previous
studies conducted in Western countries suggested that con- Abbey, K. (1979). “Does Life-style Profiling Work?” Journal of Travel
sumers with various cultural backgrounds, Chinese in particu- Research, 18 (1): 8-14.
lar, may be different, and that existing travel motivation and Ajzen, I. (1985). “From Intention to Action: A Theory of Planned Behaviors.”
In Action Control: From Cognition to Behaviors, edited by J. Kuhl
behavior theories may be insufficient in explaining their behav- and J. Beckman. New York: Springer.
ior and decision making. Unlike previous consumer marketing ——— (1991). “The Theory of Planned Behavior.” Organizational
literature concluding that people in Asian societies tend to be Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50: 179-211.
Alba, J., and W. Hutchinson (1987). “Dimensions of Consumer Expertise.”
less individualistic and adhere more to social norms, findings Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (4): 411-54.
in this study showed that different levels of reference group Assael, H. (1987). Consumer Behavior and Marketing Action. 3rd ed. Boston:
influence existed within a single nation. Therefore, generalized Kent.
Bansal, H. S., and P. A. Voyer (2000). “Word-of-Mouth Processes within
implications from consumer marketing literature should be a Services Purchase Decision Context.” Journal of Service Research,
cited with caution especially when they are applied to Chinese, 3 (2): 166-77.
Batra, R., P. M. Homer, and L. R. Kahle (2001). “Values, Susceptibility to
whose economic, social, and political changes have been sig- Normative Influence, and Attribute Importance Weights: A Nomological
nificantly noted in recent years. Analysis.” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11 (2): 115-28.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014


484 MAY 2006

Bayus, B. L. (1985). “Word of Mouth: The Indirect Effects of Marketing Leigh, J. H., and T. G. Gabel (1992). “Symbolic Interactionism: Its Effects
Efforts.” Journal of Advertising Research, 25 (3): 31-39. on Consumer Behavior and Implications for Marketing Strategy.”
Bearden, W. O., and M. J. Etzel (1982). “Reference Group Influence on Journal of Services Marketing, 6 (3): 5-16.
Product and Brand-Purchase Decisions.” Journal of Consumer Lessig, P. V., and C. W. Park (1978). “Promotional Perspectives of Reference
Research, 9 (2): 198-211. Group Influence: Advertising Implications.” Journal of Advertising,
Bettman, J. R., and C. W. Park (1980). “Effects of Prior Knowledge and 7 (2): 41-7.
Experience and Phase of the Choice Process on Consumer Decision Lovelock, C. H. (1991). Services Marketing. 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Process: A Protocol Analysis.” Journal of Consumer Research, 7 (4): Prentice Hall.
234-48. McClelland, D. C. (1975). Power: The Inner Experience. New York: Irving.
Bieger, T., and C. Laesser (2004). “Information Sources for Travel Decisions: Mehta, S. C., A. K. Lalwani, and L. Ping (2001). “Reference Group Influence
Toward a Source Process Model.” Journal of Travel Research, 42 (4): and Perceived Risk in Services among Working Women in Singapore:
357-71. A Replication and Extension.” Journal of International Consumer
Brislin, R. W. (1976). “Comparative Research Methodology: Cross-Cultural Marketing, 14 (1): 43-65.
Studies.” International Journal of Psychology, 11 (3): 215-29. Middleton, V. T. C. (2002). Marketing in Travel and Tourism. 3rd ed. New
Burnkrant, R. E., and A. Cousineau (1975). “Informational and Normative York: Butterworth Heinemann.
Social Influence in Buyer Behavior.” Journal of Consumer Research, Mitchell, V-W., and M. Greatorex (1993). “Risk Perception and Reduction in
2 (3): 206-15. the Purchase of Consumer Services.” The Services Industries Journal,
Childers, T. L., and A. G. Rao (1992). “The Influence of Familial and Peer- 13 (4): 179-200.
Based Reference Groups on Consumer Decisions.” Journal of Consumer Moutinho, L. (1987). “Consumer Behavior in Tourism.” European Journal
Research, 19 (2): 198-211. of Marketing, 21 (10): 5-9.
Chiu, T., D-P. Fang, J. Chen, Y. Wang, and C. Jeris (2001). “A Robust and National Bureau of Statistics of China (2003). National Bureau of Statistics
Scalable Clustering Algorithm for Mixed Type Attributes in Large of China. Beijing: Statistical Publishing House.
Database Environment.” In Proceedings of the 7th ACM SIGKDDD Nicholas, C. M., and D. J. Snepenger (1988). “Family Decision Making and
International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining. Tourism Behavior and Attitudes.” Journal of Travel Research, 26 (4): 2-6.
San Francisco: ACM SIGKDDD, pp. 263-68. Park, C. W., and P. V. Lessig (1977). “Students and Housewives: Differences
Cochran, J. K., M. B. Chamlin, L. Beeghley, and M. Fenwick (2004). in Susceptibility to Reference Group Influence.” Journal of Consumer
“Religion, Religiosity, and Nonmarital Sexual Conduct: An Application Research, 4 (2): 102-9.
of Reference Group Theory.” Sociological Inquiry, 74 (1): 102-27. Perry, M., and B. C. Hamm (1969). “Canonical Analysis of Relations between
Dodd, T. H. (1998). “Influences on Search Behavior of Industrial Tourists.” Socioeconomic Risk and Personal Influence in Purchase Decisions.”
Journal of Hospitality and Leisure Marketing, 5 (2/3): 77-94. Journal of Marketing Research, 6 (3): 351-54.
Escalas, J. E., and J. R. Bettman (2003). “You Are What They Eat: The Quellett, J. A., and R. Wood (1998). “Habit and Intention in Everyday Life:
Influence of Reference Groups on Consumers’ Connections to Brands.” The Multiple processes by Which Past Behavior Predicts Future
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13 (3): 339-48. Behavior.” Psychological Bulletin, 124 (1): 54-74.
Fisher, R. C. (1996). “Black, Hip, and Primed.” American Demographics, Sarigollu, E., and R. Huang (2005). “Benefits Segmentation of Visitors to
18 (12): 26. Latin America.” Journal of Travel Research, 43 (3): 277-93.
Fodness, D., and B. Murray (1997). “Tourist Information Search.” Annals of Shim, S., M. A. Eastlick, S. L. Lotz, and P. Warrington (2001). “An Online
Tourism Research, 24 (3): 503-23. Prepurchase Intentions Model: The Role of Intention to Search.”
——— (1999). “A Model of Tourist Information Search Behavior.” Journal Journal of Retailing, 77: 397-416.
of Travel Research, 37 (3): 220-30. Shoemaker, S. (1984). “Marketing to Older Travelers.” Cornell Hotel and
Gitelson, R. J., and J. L. Crompton (1983). “The Planning Horizons and Restaurant Administration Quarterly, 25 (August): 84-91.
Sources of Information Used by Pleasure Vacationers.” Journal of Travel Snepenger, D. J., K. Meged, M. Snelling, and K. Worrall (1990). “Information
Research, 21 (2): 2-7. Search Strategies by Destination Naive Tourists.” Journal of Travel
Guseman, D. S. (1981). “Risk Perception and Risk Reduction in Consumer Research, 29 (1): 13-16.
Services.” In Marketing of Services, edited by J. H. Donnelly and SPSS (2003). Statistical Package for Social Sciences for Windows, 12.0.
W. R. George. Chicago: American Marketing Association, pp. 200-4. Chicago: SPSS.
Hair, J. F. Jr., R. E. Anderson, R. L. Tatham, and W. C. Black (2002). Stafford, J. E., and B. A. Cocanougher (1977). “Reference Group Theory.” In
Multivariate Data Analysis. 6th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Selected Aspects of Consumer Behavior. Washington, DC: Superin-
Hall. tendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office, pp. 361-80.
Hatcher, L., and E. J. Stepansk (1994). A Step-by-Step Approach to Using Tabachnick, B., and L. Fidel (1989). Using Multivariate Statistics. 2nd ed.
the SAS System for Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling. New York: HarperCollins.
Cary, NC: SAS Institute. Venkatesan, M. (1966). “Experimental Study of Consumer Behavior
Hayes-Roth, B. (1977). “Evolution of Cognitive Structures and Processes.” Conformity and Independence.” Journal of Marketing Research, 3 (4):
Psychological Review, 840: 260-78. 384-87.
Hoyer, W. D., and R. Deshpande (1982). “Cross-Cultural Influences on Buyer Voss, P. Jr. (1984). “Status Shifts to Peer Influence.” Advertising Age, 17
Behavior: The Impact of Hispanic Ethnicity.” In Educators’ Conference (May): 1-10.
Proceedings, edited by B. Walker. Chicago: American Marketing Wernick, A. (1994). Promotional Culture: Advertising, Ideology and Symbolic
Association. Expression. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Johar, G. V., and J. M. Sirgy (1991). “Value Expressive versus Utilitarian Woodside, A. G., and R. E. Pitts (1976). “Effects of Consumer Lifestyle,
Advertising Appeals: When and Why to Use Which Appeal.” Journal Demographics, and Travel Activities on Foreign and Domestic Travel
of Advertising, 20 (3): 23-33. Behavior.” Journal of Travel Research, 14 (4): 13-15.
Kelly, H. H. (1947). Two Functions of Reference Groups in Readings in Woodside, A. G., and I. A. Ronkainen (1980). “Vacation Travel Planning
Social Psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Segments.” Annals of Tourism Research, 7: 385-94.
Kemper, T. P. (1968). “Reference Group, Socialization and Achievement.” Zhang, H. Q. (2004). “Accession to the World Trade Organization: Challenges
American Sociological Review, 33 (1): 31-45. for China’s Travel Service Industry.” International Journal of Contem-
Kim, Y-K., and J. Kang (2001). “The Effects of Ethnicity and Product on porary Hospitality Management, 16 (6): 369-72.
Purchase Decision-Making.” Journal of Advertising Research, 41 (2): Zhang, H Q., and T. Lam (1999). “An Analysis of Mainland Chinese Visitors’
39-48. Motivations to Visit Hong Kong.” Tourism Management, 20 (5): 587-94.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on October 7, 2014

You might also like