You are on page 1of 11

pebo

Belief beyond the evidence: using the proposed effect of breakfast


on obesity to show 2 practices that distort scientific evidence1–4
Andrew W Brown, Michelle M Bohan Brown, and David B Allison

ABSTRACT there are not insurmountable barriers to moving beyond obser-


Background: Various intentional and unintentional factors influ- vational evidence, and 2) it seems like a less politically charged
ence beliefs beyond what scientific evidence justifies. Two such topic than the topics of some previous publications on biased
factors are research lacking probative value (RLPV) and biased reporting [eg, sugar-sweetened beverages (2, 3) and breastfeeding
research reporting (BRR). (4)] to better facilitate the discussion of extrascientific factors that
Objective: We investigated the prevalence of RLPV and BRR in affect the fidelity of research reporting.
research about the proposition that skipping breakfast causes weight In this article, we first establish that the PEBO is only a pre-
gain, which is called the proposed effect of breakfast on obesity sumption so that we can examine 2 factors that may influence the

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


(PEBO) in this article. propensity to believe in a presumption beyond the available evi-
Design: Studies related to the PEBO were synthesized by using dence. The first factor we identified is research lacking probative
a cumulative meta-analysis. Abstracts from these studies were also value (RLPV), which we defined as experiments or analyses that
rated for the improper use of causal language and biased interpre- are 1) about questions that have already been sufficiently an-
tations. In separate analyses, articles that cited an observational swered or 2) designed in such a way that they cannot advance the
study about the PEBO were rated for the inappropriate use of causal scientific knowledge about the question. As an example, when no
language, and articles that cited a randomized controlled trial (RCT) association studies existed about the PEBO, the first few studies
about the PEBO were rated for misleadingly citing the RCT. were certainly probative. Similarly, the first few studies to show
Results: The current body of scientific knowledge indicates that the the association in children compared with adults may be con-
PEBO is only presumed true. The observational literature on the PEBO sidered probative. However, at some point, additional similar
has gratuitously established the association, but not the causal
observational analyses will only trivially add to our knowledge
relation, between skipping breakfast and obesity (final cumulative
regarding the PEBO by studying nonmeaningfully different sub-
meta-analysis P value ,10242), which is evidence of RLPV. Four
groups or by gratuitously replicating existing associations. We
examples of BRR are evident in the PEBO literature as follows: 1)
quantified RLPV by conducting a cumulative meta-analysis to
biased interpretation of one’s own results, 2) improper use of causal
show that the association, but not the causal relation, between
language in describing one’s own results, 3) misleadingly citing
breakfast and obesity has been more than sufficiently established.
others’ results, and 4) improper use of causal language in citing others’
work.
The second factor was biased research reporting (BRR). We
Conclusions: The belief in the PEBO exceeds the strength of
looked at 4 indications of BRR: 1) biased interpretation of one’s
scientific evidence. The scientific record is distorted by RLPV own results, 2) improper use of causal language in describing
and BRR. RLPV is a suboptimal use of collective scientific re- one’s own results, 3) misleadingly citing others’ results, and 4)
sources. Am J Clin Nutr 2013;98:1298–308. improper use of causal language in citing others’ work. By
discussing RLPV and BRR, we hope that 1) future efforts will be
spent on elucidating novel associations and causal relations by
INTRODUCTION
1
From the Office of Energetics (AWB and DBA), the Nutrition Obesity
Some beliefs about scientific topics are held true despite evi-
Research Center (AWB, MMBB, and DBA), the School of Public Health
dence refuting them, whereas other science-related beliefs are (AWB and DBA), and the Department of Nutrition Sciences (MMBB and
presumed true even though insufficient evidence exists to support DBA), University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL.
or refute them (1). One such presumption is that regularly con- 2
Views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not nec-
suming compared with skipping breakfast protects against obesity essarily represent those of the NIH or any other organization.
3
or causes weight loss, which we refer to as the proposed effect of Supported by the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kid-
breakfast on obesity (PEBO)5 in this article. More precisely, the ney Diseases (NIDDK P30DK056336 and NIDDK T32DK62710).
4
PEBO goes beyond indicating an association between skipping Address correspondence to DB Allison, RPHB 140J, 1530 Third Avenue
South, Birmingham, AL 35294-0022. E-mail: dallison@uab.edu.
breakfast and obesity (in which breakfast consumption and lower 5
Abbreviations used: BRR, biased research reporting; NWCR, National
weight are observed together) by indicating that the introduction Weight Control Registry; PEBO, proposed effect of breakfast on obesity;
of breakfast causes a decrease in body weight. We have chosen RCT, randomized controlled trial; RLPV, research lacking probative value.
the PEBO as an example because 1) it is a topic that is amenable Received April 15, 2013. Accepted for publication August 20, 2013.
to conducting randomized controlled trials (RCTs), and thus, First published online September 4, 2013; doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.064410.

1298 Am J Clin Nutr 2013;98:1298–308. Printed in USA. Ó 2013 American Society for Nutrition
BREAKFAST, OBESITY, AND BIAS 1299
conducting probative research, 2) future research results will be from the meta-analysis by publication year starting from 1994
reported with greater fidelity by minimizing BRR, and 3) the and ending in 2011. All studies published within a given year were
belief in scientific topics will be grounded in scientific evidence. added concurrently.

METHODS Assessing BRR


Establishing that the PEBO is a widely believed For all ratings, AWB and MMBB independently rated the
presumption articles with input from DBA on the rating scales and criteria. All
rating disagreements were settled by consensus between AWB
Lay-media, scientific, and government sources were searched
and MMBB.
for statements about breakfast and obesity to establish that the
PEBO is widely believed. To evaluate our perception that the 1) Biased interpretation of one’s own results. Of the 92
PEBO is only a presumption rather than an empirically supported articles identified for the cumulative meta-analysis, 88
scientific conclusion, scientific databases were searched for articles contained abstracts indexed in PubMed in En-
empirical human research about the PEBO. In addition, studies glish. The results and conclusions stated in these ab-
were reviewed from one published meta-analysis and 3 sys- stracts were rated as follows: either not mentioning
tematic reviews (5–8). breakfast in relation to obesity (none) or indicating that
breakfast was beneficial (positive), there was no relation
between breakfast and obesity (no relation), breakfast
Cumulative meta-analysis to assess RLPV was detrimental (negative), or the relation between
We identified 92 unique articles about the PEBO that were breakfast and obesity was mixed (mixed). Although

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


cited in one published meta-analysis and 3 published systematic some studies implied mixed results (eg, breakfast was
reviews (5–8). Briefly, Horikawa et al (5) meta-analyzed 19 only stated as significant in one group and not mentioned
studies of the association between breakfast consumption and in the other group), results were rated only on explicit
odds of being overweight or obese in Asian and Pacific coun- mentioning of breakfast and obesity to allow reasonable
tries. Szajewska and Ruszczy nski (8) systematically reviewed tolerance for word limitations of abstracts.
studies of children and adolescents in Europe and identified 16
2) Improper use of causal language in describing one’s own
studies. Mesas et al (6) systematically reviewed a number of
results. The 88 rated abstracts were from observational
eating behaviors related to obesity and reported 69 articles that
studies; 76 studies were cross-sectional, 11 studies were
looked at breakfast and obesity in English, Spanish, or Portu-
longitudinal, and one study was a case-control study.
guese. Rampersaud et al (7) identified 16 articles in children and
Therefore, language concluding a finding of cause and
adolescents in a systematic manner, although the review was not
effect was not appropriate within these 88 abstracts. Con-
declared a systematic review. All identified studies were ob-
clusions that included breakfast and obesity were subse-
servational. Studies were synthesized in a manner similar to the
quently rated causal if causal language was used or
breakfast-obesity meta-analysis by Horikawa et al (5) as fol-
associative if the inference was limited to associations.
lows: breakfast consumption in each article had to be defined as
In addition, conclusions were rated qualified causal if
a frequency (as opposed to investigating only the type or amount
qualifiers such as “may” or “suggests” were included in
of breakfast consumed); the breakfast skipping group “was de-
the causal language because distinguishing between state-
fined as the lowest category of breakfast frequency in an in-
ments that used qualifiers to introduce a hypothesis com-
dividual study” (5); body weight needed to be classified into
pared with veiling a causal statement was subjective.
overweight and/or obese; and analyses that adjusted for potential
confounders were selected when available and appropriate. In 3) Misleadingly citing others’ results. We examined the man-
addition, studies that considered both overweight and obese ner in which articles cited an RCT reported by Schlundt
were included; mutually exclusive groups (eg, male and female et al (10). Schlundt et al (10) assigned women to a weight-
subjects) were included as separate groups for analysis where loss plan that included or excluded breakfast and stratified
possible and appropriate; and we limited the analysis only to full them by prestudy habitual breakfast intake for a total of 4
articles in the English language. SEs and ORs were calculated study groups. They noted a P value ,0.06 for the interac-
for each independent study group. In total, 58 of 92 studies fit tion of breakfast habit by breakfast assignment, with no
these criteria with a total of 88 independent OR estimates. With significant main effect of breakfast consumption, which
the use of a random-effects model in Review Manager 5.1 indicated that subjects who were assigned to change from
software (The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collab- their baseline breakfast frequency lost more weight than
oration), we first synthesized all data by weighting each study did subjects assigned to continue their baseline breakfast
group by the inverse variance of its point estimate. As a check, frequency (Table 1). We identified a total of 91 English-
this method successfully reproduced the results of Horikawa language articles that cited Schlundt et al (10) by searching
et al (5) when limited only to data in their analysis (OR: 1.75; the Web of Science (http://apps.webofknowledge.com) and
CI: 1.57, 1.95). Evidence of funnel-plot asymmetry was assessed Scopus (http://www.scopus.com) on 14 May 2012. Scopus
graphically and by using Egger’s linear regression (9). Funnel-plot contains citation records back to 1996; the Web of Science
asymmetry can be indicative of a publication bias or unaccounted subscription for the University of Alabama at Birmingham
heterogeneity in the analysis. Subsequently, we conducted a cu- is current from 1990 to year end 2011. We selected this
mulative meta-analysis by sequentially synthesizing point estimates article because 1) it was fairly well cited, and 2) the study
1300 BROWN ET AL
TABLE 1 ference in reported energy intake between breakfast
Weight-loss results from Schlundt et al (10)1 eaters and non-eaters, but breakfast eaters reported slightly
Baseline breakfast habits more physical activity than non-breakfast eaters (p=0.05).”
(11)
Eaters Skippers Total
This study was chosen because 1) it was fairly well cited
Assignment (kg) [we identified a total of 91 articles that cited Wyatt et al
Breakfast 6.2 6 3.3 [15]2 7.7 6 3.3 [8] 6.73 (11) in English as described previously for Schlundt et al
No breakfast 8.9 6 4.2 [14] 6.0 6 3.9 [8] 7.8
(10), of which 72 articles cited Wyatt et al (11) regarding
Total 7.5 6.9 —
the PEBO]; and 2) the NWCR describes a case series,
1
No main effects between the 2 factors were noted, but an interaction which meant common behaviors observed within this
effect was noted at P , 0.06. Adapted with permission from reference population simply co-occurred with weight-loss mainte-
10.
nance, and therefore showed neither an association nor
2
Mean 6 SD; n in brackets (all such values).
3
Mean (all such values).
causation. Citations were rated as stating that the NWCR
article concluded the relation between breakfast and obesity
was causal or associative or correctly stated that the NWCR
had mixed results, which allowed us to potentially observe simply observed co-occurrence. The rest of the citations
misleading citations both for and against breakfast. Arti- were rated as qualified associative or causal as previously
cles were categorized on the basis of the way they cited described.
Schlundt et al (10) as accurate, mildly misleading positive,
explicitly misleading positive, mildly misleading negative,

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


RESULTS
explicitly misleading negative, neutral, inaccurate unre-
lated, and otherwise unrelated. Positive was defined as The PEBO is widely stated as true
misleadingly citing the results to make breakfast seem
We identified numerous statements and recommendations
more beneficial, and negative was defined as misleadingly
encouraging individuals to eat breakfast with the express purpose
citing the results to make breakfast seem detrimental. Neu-
of influencing obesity. These recommendations came from
tral meant that results were cited as indicating that break-
popular health icons (12) and respected web-based health in-
fast and weight had no relation and did not mention the
formation outlets (13) and even the United States Surgeon
interaction trend, and the 2 unrelated categories indicated
General (14) (Table 2). Some statements included uncertainty
that breakfast and weight results were not cited (eg, the
(14), whereas other statements were hyperbolic and attributed
citation was related to other study aspects).
unfounded behaviors to skipping breakfast [eg, “raiding the
4) Improper use of causal language in citing others’ work. vending machine” (15)], and still other statements created spe-
We examined to what extent authors extrapolated beyond cific temporal constraints on when breakfast must be consumed
the limitations of the study design when citing a National (16). A satellite symposium at Nutrition Society’s 2012 summer
Weight Control Registry (NWCR) study by Wyatt et al meeting was even titled “How does breakfast help manage
(11). Wyatt et al (11) observed that “A large proportion bodyweight?” (22) Although we cannot conclude that there is
of NWCR subjects (2313 or 78%) reported regularly eat- universal agreement in lay-media, government, and scientific-
ing breakfast every day of the week. Only 114 subjects opinion leaders from these exemplars, we concluded that the
(4%) reported never eating breakfast. There was no dif- PEBO is widely presumed true, as previously suggested (1).

TABLE 2
Quotations that show breakfast is widely believed to protect against obesity
Quotation Source

The fact is, when you’re trying to lose body fat, you can’t skip breakfast. Dr Oz (12)
.there’s ample evidence that the simple act of eating breakfast–every day–is a big WebMD (13)
part of losing weight, lots of weight.
Eating a healthy breakfast is a good way to start the day and may be important in US Surgeon General 14)
achieving and maintaining a healthy weight.
Skipping breakfast to lose weight makes you fatter - and far more likely to raid the Daily Mail (15)
vending machine.
Make sure to eat a healthy breakfast no later than 9 a.m., even if it means placing a Discovery Health (16)
bowl of oatmeal on your dressing table to eat while you put on your makeup.
Studies show that breakfast can be important in maintaining a healthy body weight. Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health (17)
With benefits of decreased body mass index, reduced fatigue, and improved NYU Langone Internal Medicine Blog (18)
cognition, we should all eat our Wheaties to start the day!
In fact, skipping breakfast actually increases your risk of obesity. Mayo Clinic (19)
Research provides evidence that the simple act of eating breakfast every day is a Scale Back Alabama (20)
big part of losing weight.
Want to trim your waist? Try eating breakfast! Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics (21)
BREAKFAST, OBESITY, AND BIAS 1301
TABLE 3
Randomized controlled trials that investigated breakfast interventions and obesity
First author, year of
publication (reference) Study length Treatment description Relevant results Notes

Schlundt, 1992 (10) 12 wk Skipping or eating breakfast Habitual breakfast eaters Interaction effect was
stratified by baseline assigned to skip breakfast P , 0.06
breakfast habits and habitual breakfast
skippers assigned to eat
breakfast lost more
weight than subjects who
maintained their
breakfast habits; no main
effect of breakfast on
weight loss.
Geliebter, 2000 (23) 4 wk Oatmeal, corn flakes, or Water group lost 1.2 kg; Specific breakfast foods
water for breakfast breakfast groups did not
change weight. No
changes in lean and fat
mass in groups.
Farshchi, 2005 (24) 2 wk Controlled chronobiological No significant differences in Controlled calories at
study in which breakfast weight, anthropometric subsequent eating
calories were shifted to measures, or body occasions

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


lunch composition.
Hirsch, 1975 (25) 1 wk/arm All food consumed at dinner Dinner-only resulted in Not specifically breakfast
or at breakfast; crossover a relative increase in compared with no
weight compared with breakfast
breakfast-only.
Crepinsek, 2006 (26) 3 school years Universal-free school No difference in BMI or Not specifically breakfast
breakfast compared with percentage of students compared with no
standard School overweight. breakfast
Breakfast Program
Powell, 1998 (27) 1 school year Breakfast or a quarter of an Breakfast intervention One-half of children were
orange resulted in increased less than 21 SD weight-
weight. for-age; not specifically
breakfast compared with
no breakfast
Ask, 2006 (28) 4 mo Free breakfast and Weight increased in control Unclear if control and
supplements compared group and M subjects of intervention groups were
with control intervention; BMI compared; one class
increased in control room per treatment; not
group but not in specifically breakfast
intervention. compared with no
breakfast
Rosado, 2008 (29) 12 wk Ready-to-eat-cereal or No significant difference in Included unrelated
habitual intake weight between groups. additional treatment
groups; not specifically
breakfast compared with
no breakfast

Research does not clearly confirm or refute the PEBO the rest of the day. Although some studies showed a decreased
RCTs energy intake at a subsequent eating occasion after eating
compared with skipping breakfast, none of the studies showed
There have been very few RCTs that investigated breakfast and a decrease in the sum of calories consumed across the study
weight change. RCTs related to the PEBO, with no consistent period when breakfast was eaten rather than skipped (Table 4).
evidence that breakfast affects weight, are summarized in Table Other potential mechanisms have been proposed for the PEBO,
3. One study even suggests that whether eating or skipping such as chronobiology-dependent changes in metabolism;
breakfast is more effective for weight loss depends on the in- however, there is currently no clearly and consistently shown
dividual’s typical breakfast habits (10). physiologic mechanism to explain the PEBO.

Intermediate endpoint studies Observational evidence


Physiologic and behavioral mechanisms have been proposed Of the 88 abstracts used to investigate BRR, 86% of studies
to link breakfast and obesity, with the primary one being that were cross-sectional. Longitudinal analyses used various
breakfast consumption will result in a decreased cumulative measures of breakfast exposure, despite the relation between
energy intake by causing a compensatory hypophagia throughout breakfast and obesity that potentially depends on how an individual
1302 BROWN ET AL
TABLE 4
Studies that evaluated breakfast and energy intake
First author, year of
publication (reference) Study length Treatment description Relevant results Notes

Astbury, 2011 (30) 1-d crossover Breakfast or no breakfast; No significant differences in Significant increased ad libitum
plus liquid preload total energy intake (breakfast, test meal intake in no-breakfast
before lunch preload, and ad libitum lunch) group
Geliebter, 1999 (31) 1-d crossover 350 kcal oatmeal, sugared Liquid test meal intake was lower Overweight M and F subjects
cornflakes, or water control in the oatmeal but not cornflake
conditions compared with water
control
Geliebter, 1999 (32) 1-d crossover 350 kcal oatmeal, sugared Liquid test meal intake was lower Lean and overweight M and F
cornflakes, or water control in the oatmeal but not cornflake subjects; overweight subjects
conditions compared with water ingested even less after
control consumption of oatmeal
Kral, 2011 (33) 1-d crossover Breakfast or no breakfast Significantly higher energy intake Similar intakes at lunch and rest
in breakfast group of day
Leidy, 2011 (34) 1-d crossover No breakfast, normal breakfast, No significant differences in total Protein-rich breakfast lowered
or high-protein breakfast energy intake lunch intake compared with
that with normal and no breakfast
1) Levitsky, 2013 (35) 1-d crossover High carbohydrate, high fiber, Significantly higher energy intake Similar intakes at lunch
or no breakfast; 1.4-MJ in breakfast groups (breakfast

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


breakfasts plus lunch)
2) Levitsky, 2013 (35) 1-d crossover Ad libitum or no breakfast Significantly higher daily intake in Significant increase in lunch intake
breakfast group for no-breakfast group
Taylor, 2001(36) 2-d crossover 6 meals/d compared with No significant differences in total Wide range of BMI above
4 or 2 meals/d energy expenditure or intake overweight; F only

defines breakfast eating (eg, number of days per week; including RLPV
only certain foods) and obesity (eg, categorical obesity, con- Studies included in the cumulative meta-analysis were con-
tinuous BMI, including or excluding overweight) (37) and were ducted in 30 countries on 5 continents (Figure 1; countries in
generally weak because of dataset limitations. There is a clear dark gray) with a variety of subpopulations (eg, rural and urban
association between breakfast omission and excess weight as and men and women) and, thus, provide a widely generalizable
summarized in our cumulative meta-analysis (see RLPV be- association. Consistent with the general recommendations
low), and this association supports the PEBO but does not shown in Table 2 to eat breakfast with no regard for sub-
show causation. populations, all OR estimates were pooled together regardless

FIGURE 1. Map of countries where the proposed effect of breakfast on obesity has been studied. Dark gray denotes studies used in the meta-analysis or
abstract analysis (n = 30). Black denotes studies used in the abstract analysis only (n = 6).
BREAKFAST, OBESITY, AND BIAS 1303
that, by 1998, after synthesizing only 5 estimates, the P value
was ,0.001, which Sterne and Smith proposed to be interpreted
as strong evidence of a relation (38). By 2011, the P value was
,10242. Although no universal threshold exists for what con-
stitutes sufficient observational evidence, we posit that 10242 is
excessive, and therefore, it was gratuitous to conduct additional
association studies regarding the PEBO well before 2011.

Evidence of BRR in abstracts and citing behaviors


Of our 4 examples of BRR, the first 2 examples represented
infidelity in the reporting of one’s own work, whereas the second
2 examples represented the distortion of others’ research.

Biased interpretation of one’s own results


If all 88 of the rated abstracts accurately reported results and
FIGURE 2. Funnel plot of the meta-analysis. Dots represent each of 88 conclusions, and all of these articles only investigated breakfast and
independent study group OR estimates. The vertical dashed line represents obesity, all counts should appear on the diagonal in Figure 4, which
the meta-analyzed log OR. The solid line is an unweighted transformation of was not the case (ie, all diagonal cells should be black). In par-
the Egger’s linear regression test for funnel-plot asymmetry (9). The weighted
intercept (0.5345) shows modest evidence of funnel-plot asymmetry (P =
ticular, 65% of studies that reported positive results for breakfast

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


0.0856), which may represent a publication bias, unaccounted heteroge- also mentioned the results in the abstract’s conclusion compared
neity in the meta-analysis, or asymmetry by chance. with only 39% of studies with nonpositive results (2-proportion
test: P = 0.0492). Thus, a disproportionate number of abstracts
of the subpopulation. The meta-analysis of the 88 study groups with nonpositive results failed to include a nonpositive conclusion.
resulted in a pooled OR of 1.55 (95% CI: 1.46, 1.65) (see Note that none of the studies that reported positive results had
Supplemental Figure S1 under “Supplemental data” in the on- conclusions categorized as no relation, negative, or mixed,
line issue), with modest evidence of funnel-plot asymmetry as whereas one abstract that did not mention breakfast and obesity in
assessed graphically and by using Egger’s linear regression (P = the results still had a positive conclusion toward the PEBO.
0.0856) (9) (Figure 2). Such asymmetry can be indicative of
a publication bias, unaccounted heterogeneity in the analysis, or Improper use of causal language to describe one’s own results
asymmetry by chance. Language that concluded a finding of cause and effect was not
The cumulative association between breakfast and obesity was appropriate for this set of 88 abstracts from observational studies.
significant at a = 0.05 (2-tailed test) after the first year, which However, of the 42 abstracts that included conclusions about
included only 3 study groups, and remained so throughout the breakfast and obesity, 11 abstracts made unqualified causal
entire cumulative time course (Figure 3). Of particular note was claims about breakfast consumption and obesity, whereas an

FIGURE 3. Cumulative meta-analysis of studies analyzed in the meta-analysis. Each data point represents the synthesis of all studies included year on year
(n = 58 studies with 88 independent groups). Diamonds represent the ORs of skipping breakfast and being overweight or obese bounded by a 95% CI. The
black horizontal line is set at an OR of 1. The final OR is 1.55 (95% CI: 1.46, 1.65). Squares represent P values of ORs for each year of the analysis. Two
horizontal dotted lines represent the traditional P = 0.05 significance and P = 0.001, which was suggested to represent strong evidence of an association (38).
The final P value is 10242.
1304 BROWN ET AL

FIGURE 4. Concordance between results and conclusions from 88 identified abstracts. Abstracts were rated on the basis of whether the results or
conclusions stated that eating breakfast was associated with lower obesity (Positive), showed no relation between breakfast and obesity (No Relation),
showed that eating breakfast was associated with higher obesity (Negative), showed mixed relations (Mixed), or did not mention an analysis between breakfast
and obesity (None). Cell shading represents the percentage of conclusion ratings within a result-rating column, with black representing 100% and white
representing 0%. Marginal counts are not shaded.

additional 10 abstracts used qualifiers to introduce uncertainty citations), and they were almost exclusively biased in favor of
into the causal statements; 21 articles limited the abstract con- breakfast.
clusions to noncausal language (Figure 5). Thus, 26–50% of
studies were ascribed greater inferential strength than the study Improper use of causal language in citing others’ work
designs warranted.
Of the 91 articles that cited Wyatt et al (11), 72 articles cited the
article with respect to the PEBO. Of these articles, 29% correctly
Misleadingly citing others’ results described the relation between breakfast and weight-loss main-

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


Of the 91 articles that cited Schlundt et al (10) , 46 articles cited tenance as simply co-occurring, whereas 26% stated the 2 were
the article with respect to the PEBO, whereas the other citations associated, and 22% made statements that causally linked
were related to other endpoints (eg, snacking frequency) or were breakfast and obesity (Figure 7). The rest of the articles were
otherwise unrelated (Figure 6). Of the PEBO-relevant citations, rated qualified associative or causal (4% and 18% of relevant
only 17% of articles cited the results accurately. Of the 29 ar- abstracts, respectively). Thus, 48% of the PEBO-relevant citations
ticles that misleadingly cited Schlundt et al (10), 28 articles of Wyatt et al (11) explicitly ascribed a stronger inferential
mildly or explicitly cited Schlundt et al (10) positively toward meaning to the article than was warranted, with an additional 22%
breakfast with only 1 article that cited Schlundt et al (10) neg- of articles that potentially did so depending on the interpretation.
atively toward breakfast (P = 1.12 3 10–7; 2-sided binomial test
against H0: P-positive|misleading citation = 0.5). The only ar-
ticle (2%) that cited the results misleadingly negatively toward DISCUSSION
breakfast also cited the results accurately elsewhere in the article We have shown that 1) the PEBO is presumed and stated as
(44). These results show that a sizeable number of citations of true despite equivocal evidence; 2) the gratuitous replication
Schlundt et al (10) were misleading (62% of the PEBO-relevant of associations between breakfast and obesity showed that

FIGURE 5. Authors’ use of causative language in their own observational studies. The left pie chart shows that 48% (n = 42) of 88 abstracts made
conclusions about breakfast and weight, which is broken down by the use of causal language in the right pie chart.
BREAKFAST, OBESITY, AND BIAS 1305

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


FIGURE 6. Categorization of 91 articles that cited Schlundt et al (10). Articles that cited Schlundt et al (10) were categorized as shown in the table inset in
the figure. All articles were included in the left pie chart, with only the relevant citations presented in the right pie chart (n = 42). 1The one study that was
explicitly misleadingly negative also cited Schlundt et al (10) accurately elsewhere in the article.

numerous nonprobative studies exist in the PEBO literature; and We reiterate that we used breakfast and obesity as an example
3) there is evidence of a bias with respect to the reporting of for RLPV and BRR and did not consider other important asso-
one’s own and others’ research. ciations with breakfast, such as cognitive function, or other

FIGURE 7. The use of causal language in 91 articles that cited Wyatt et al (11). Articles that cited Wyatt et al (11) were categorized as shown in the table inset
in the figure. The left pie chart represents all articles that cited Wyatt et al (11), whereas the right pie chart was limited to relevant citations (n = 72). 1Unratable
citations include accurate citations unrelated to breakfast and weight and citations for which it was unclear what was being attributed to the Wyatt et al (11) article.
1306 BROWN ET AL

important associations with obesity, such as fruit and vegetable sources if a manuscript is being written. Unfortunately, there is an
consumption. We also acknowledge that our analysis may not obligation to make nonprobative replications publicly available,
have been fully comprehensive because we selected studies on regardless of the outcome, lest we encourage publication bias.
the basis of previous research syntheses from which we could Perhaps a more useful exercise would be for researchers to better
calculate ORs. However, additional studies that might have been reflect on the body of evidence before engaging in research. This
identified from a de novo systematic review were unlikely to exercise could be facilitated by research registration that is
meaningfully affect the final P value of the cumulative meta- subjected to peer review (56).
analysis because of its magnitude and only weak evidence of Another means of increasing the marginal probative value of
a publication bias. Therefore, additional unidentified studies a study relative to an established body of research evidence is to
would have likely contributed to our conclusion that the asso- conduct experiments that directly test the relation of interest
ciation has been excessively established. rather than creating causal extrapolations from observational or
Our cumulative meta-analysis that showed the presence of intermediate-endpoint studies in obesity (57). These experiments
RLPV had a final P value ,10242, which was almost forty can often be conducted with established designs, are generally
degrees of magnitude less than the proposed significance level of cost effective, and often have relatively low ethical concern,
Sterne and Smith (38). Despite this gratuitous replication of particularly when studying nutrition. However, the United States
effort, we are still uncertain of the role that breakfast plays in and WHO clinical trial registries have only 1 study registered to
obesity because these observational studies informed association test the PEBO (as of 9 October 2012; www.clinicaltrials.gov;
and not necessarily causation. Each study and analysis that is NCT01781780). Researchers with the Nutrition Obesity Re-
conducted costs time and resources that could be dedicated to search Center at the University of Alabama at Birmingham, in
unveiling novel associations or engaging in randomized trials collaboration with other research centers, are conducting an
that can better define causal relations. Therefore, the evidence of effectiveness study that will test recommendations to eat

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


RLPV is also evidence of an unproductive use of scientific re- breakfast or not to eat breakfast, as well as having a control group,
sources. over 16 wk at 5 sites, with groups stratified by typical breakfast-
We do not suggest that replication be suppressed (55); rather consumption habits. Not including personnel costs, the study is
we want to discourage gratuitous replication. Some journals, eg, estimated to cost in the neighborhood of $40,000. This study
now require bullet points that identify what is already known shows the feasibility of conducting randomized controlled trials
compared with what is needed. Although this may help stifle the on obesity-targeted population health recommendations rather
publication of RLPV, the research has already consumed re- than relying on observational extrapolations.

FIGURE 8. Hypothetical model of how BRR and RLPV may be involved in perpetuating presumptions. A: Exposure to such phrases as “breakfast is the
most important meal of the day” may predispose individuals to believe positive things about breakfast because of the “halo effect” (58). B: The RLPV related
to the PEBO may augment this predisposition to believe the PEBO through the “mere exposure effect,” (59) particularly when the research is presented in
a biased manner (eg, in the presence of BRR). C: Individuals tend to seek out information confirming their point of view and reject information to the contrary
(confirmation bias) to prevent or reduce cognitive dissonance (60), thereby retaining a biased sample of information. D. A hypothetical illustrative graph of the
comparison of strength of conviction compared with the strength of existing evidence supporting the PEBO. E: Together, these cognitive biases may
predispose researchers to bias their research reporting, which, thereby, would feed the cycle. BRR, biased research reporting; PEBO, proposed effect of
breakfast on obesity; RLPV, research lacking probative value.
BREAKFAST, OBESITY, AND BIAS 1307
It is unclear why BRR and RLPV are present in the PEBO university from Kraft Foods, Kellogg Company, PepsiCo, Jason Pharmaceu-
literature. A hypothetical framework to tie these factors together ticals, and McNeil Nutritionals. MMBB had no conflict of interest.
is shown in Figure 8. Frequent assertions of the general benefits
of consuming breakfast, most easily summarized as “breakfast is
the most important meal of the day,” could potentially pre- REFERENCES
dispose individuals to ascribe other positive attributes to 1. Casazza K, Fontaine KR, Astrup A, Birch LL, Brown AW, Bohan
breakfast because of the “halo-effect,” in which some foods are Brown MM, Durant N, Dutton G, Foster EM, Heymsfield SB, et al.
Myths, presumptions, and facts about obesity. N Engl J Med 2013;368:
considered to be intrinsically good and wholesome (58). In 446–54.
addition, the repeated exposure to RLPV may further predispose 2. Cope MB, Allison DB. White hat bias: examples of its presence in
researchers to specifically believe in the PEBO because of the obesity research and a call for renewed commitment to faithfulness in
“mere exposure effect”: individuals tend to believe something to research reporting. Int J Obes (Lond) 2010;34:84–8.
3. Weed DL, Althuis MD, Mink PJ. Quality of reviews on sugar-sweetened
which they have been repeatedly exposed (59). This pre- beverages and health outcomes: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr
disposition may be exacerbated if the exposure consists of bi- 2011;94:1340–7.
ased information that communicates stronger conclusions than 4. Cope MB, Allison DB. Critical review of the World Health Organi-
the evidence warrants, as shown in BRR. In turn, individuals zation’s (WHO) 2007 report on ’evidence of the long-term effects of
breastfeeding: systematic reviews and meta-analysis’ with respect to
may be less likely to seek out or accept information contrary to obesity. Obes Rev 2008;9:594–605.
their preconceptions (confirmation bias) as a way to decrease the 5. Horikawa C, Kodama S, Yachi Y, Heianza Y, Hirasawa R, Ibe Y, Saito
mental discomfort associated with conflicting information K, Shimano H, Yamada N, Sone H. Skipping breakfast and prevalence
(cognitive dissonance) (60). A resultant biased mental repository of overweight and obesity in Asian and Pacific regions: a meta-anal-
ysis. Prev Med 2011;53:260–7.
of information would be consistent with why individuals believe 6. Mesas AE, Munoz-Pareja M, Lopez-Garcia E, Rodriguez-Artalejo F.

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


more strongly in a presumption than the existing evidence ob- Selected eating behaviours and excess body weight: a systematic re-
jectively supports (a hypothetical representation is shown in view. Obes Rev 2012;13:106–35.
Figure 8D). Conversely, “white hat bias” (2) suggests the in- 7. Rampersaud GC, Pereira MA, Girard BL, Adams J, Metzl JD.
Breakfast habits, nutritional status, body weight, and academic per-
fluence of bias on research distortion may be more direct formance in children and adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 2005;105:743–
whereby individuals (consciously or unconsciously) might ig- 60; quiz 61–2.
nore the weakness of the scientific evidence regarding a pre- 8. Szajewska H, Ruszczynski M. Systematic review demonstrating that
sumption in favor of believing in the wholesome nature of the breakfast consumption influences body weight outcomes in children
and adolescents in Europe. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 2010;50:113–9.
exposure (ie, breakfast) in hopes of addressing a formidable 9. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-
consequence (ie, obesity). Although we could not directly test analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997;315:629–34.
either framework in the current study, we hypothesize that these 10. Schlundt DG, Hill JO, Sbrocco T, Pope-Cordle J, Sharp T. The role of
factors may influence the propensity of authors to bias research breakfast in the treatment of obesity: a randomized clinical trial. Am J
Clin Nutr 1992;55:645–51.
reporting. 11. Wyatt HR, Grunwald GK, Mosca CL, Klem ML, Wing RR, Hill JO.
Beyond the investigator-centric framework proposed in Figure Long-term weight loss and breakfast in subjects in the National Weight
8, the implications of BRR extend to other evidence consumers. Control Registry. Obes Res 2002;10:78–82.
When research reporting is biased, the integrity of scientific 12. Lynn L. Breakfast shakes: drink yourself skinny. 26 October 2011.
Available from: http://www.doctoroz.com/blog/lisa-lynn/breakfast-
information deteriorates because it drifts from the original
shakes-drink-yourself-skinny (cited 17 January 2013).
source, much like the childhood game of “telephone.” If the 13. Davis JL. Lose weight: eat breakfast. 31 August 2010. Available from:
fidelity of the scientific message cannot be replicated from the http://www.webmd.com/diet/features/lose-weight-eat-breakfast (cited
results to conclusions within a study [(as shown in the first and 20 November 2012).
14. SurgeonGeneral.gov. The Surgeon General’s call to action to prevent
second examples of BRR in the current article and “spin” by
and decrease overweight and obesity: overweight in children and
Boutron et al (61)], the reporting of the scientific finding is adolescents. Available from: http://www.surgeongeneral.gov/library/
distorted before it even reaches press releases (62). Therefore, calls/obesity/fact_adolescents.html (cited 20 November 2012).
evidence consumers are receiving biased information. 15. Macrae F. Skipping breakfast to lose weight makes you fatter - and far
more likely to raid the vending machine. 16 October 2012. Available
Ironically, raising awareness of a bias often helps individuals
from: http://www.dailymail.co.uk/health/article-2218649/Skipping-
identify biases in others rather than oneself, which is a so-called breakfast-cause-weight-GAIN-weight-loss.html (cited 20 November
“bias blind spot” (63). Although we hope that the results pre- 2012).
sented in the current article will help individuals identify biased 16. Peeke P. Dr. Pamela Peeke’s 10 weight-prevention tips. Available from:
reporting in their own work (ourselves included), it is the peer- http://health.howstuffworks.com/wellness/diet-fitness/weight-loss/10-
toxic-weight-prevention-tips1.htm (cited 20 November 2012).
review process that may be best improved by raising awareness 17. Health JHBSoP. Breakfast. Available from: http://www.jhsph.edu/
of bias. As writers and reviewers, we need to be vigilant for offices-and-services/student-affairs/Breakfast (cited 17 January 2013).
lapses in objectivity and for studies of nonprobative value to 18. Che C. Myths and realities: is breakfast the most important meal of the
hopefully keep scientific beliefs grounded in scientific evidence. day? 18 June 2009. Available from: http://www.clinicalcorrelations.
org/?p=1525 (cited 20 November 2012).
The authors’ responsibilities were as follows—AWB and MMBB: con- 19. Zeratsky K. Why does eating a healthy breakfast help control weight?
ducted the research; AWB: analyzed data; DBA: had primary responsibility 23 July 2011. Available from: http://www.mayoclinic.com/health/food-
and-nutrition/AN01119/ (cited 20 November 2012).
for the final content of the manuscript; and all authors: designed the research,
20. Alabama SB. Choosing breakfast as a lifestyle habit. 2011. Available
wrote the article, and approved the final manuscript. AWB has received from: http://www.scalebackalabama.com/toolkit/2011/lessonplans/
consulting fees from Farmland Foods and lecture fees from Danisco. DBA Lesson6_ChoosingBreakfast.pdf (cited 20 November 2012).
has served as an unpaid board member for the International Life Sciences 21. Blake JS. Want to trim your waist? Try eating breakfast! 15 Novemeber
Institute of North America and has received payment for board membership 2010. Available from: http://www.eatright.org/Media/Blog.aspx?id=
from Kraft Foods and consulting fees, grants, and/or unrestricted gifts to his 4294969065&blogid=269 (cited 20 November 2012).
1308 BROWN ET AL
22. Ashwell M, De la Hunty A. How does breakfast help manage body- 43. Booth CK, Reilly C, Farmakalidis E. Mineral composition of Austra-
weight? Nutr Bull 2012;37:395–7. lian ready-to-eat breakfast cereals. J Food Compost Anal 1996;9:135–47.
23. Geliebter A, Yahav E, Haq S, Hashim SA. Cholesterol and weight 44. Berkey CS, Rockett HR, Gillman MW, Field AE, Colditz GA. Lon-
change following daily high or low fiber breakfast cereals. Obes Res gitudinal study of skipping breakfast and weight change in adolescents.
2000;8:25S. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2003;27:1258–66.
24. Farshchi HR, Taylor MA, Macdonald IA. Deleterious effects of 45. Masheb RM, Grilo CM, White MA. An examination of eating patterns
omitting breakfast on insulin sensitivity and fasting lipid profiles in in community women with bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder.
healthy lean women. Am J Clin Nutr 2005;81:388–96. Int J Eat Disord 2011;44:618–24.
25. Hirsch EHE, Halberg F, Goetz FC, Cressey D, Wendt H, Sothern R, 46. Ozdogan AGY, Ozcelik AO, Surucuoglu MS. The breakfast habits of
Haus E, Stoney P, Minors D, Rosen G, et al. Body weight change female university students. Pakistan J Nutr 2010;9:882–6.
during 1 week on a single daily 2000-calorie meal consumed as 47. Turnin MC, Bourgeois O, Cathelineau G, Leguerrier AM, Halimi S,
breakfast (B) or dinner (D). Chronobiologia 1975;2(suppl 1):31–2. Sandre-Banon D, Coliche V, Breux M, Verlet E, Labrousse F, et al.
26. Crepinsek MK, Singh A, Bernstein LS, McLaughlin JE. Dietary effects Multicenter randomized evaluation of a nutritional education software
of universal-free school breakfast: findings from the evaluation of the in obese patients. Diabetes Metab 2001;27:139–47.
school breakfast program pilot project. J Am Diet Assoc 2006;106: 48. Fiore H, Travis S, Whalen A, Auinger P, Ryan S. Potentially protective
1796–803. factors associated with healthful body mass index in adolescents with
27. Powell CA, Walker SP, Chang SM, Grantham-McGregor SM. Nutrition obese and nonobese parents: a secondary data analysis of the Third
and education: a randomized trial of the effects of breakfast in rural National Health And Nutrition Examination Survey, 1988-1994. J Am
primary school children. Am J Clin Nutr 1998;68:873–9. Diet Assoc 2006;106:55–64; quiz 76–9.
28. Ask AS, Hernes S, Aarek I, Johannessen G, Haugen M. Changes in 49. Carels RA, Young KM, Coit C, Clayton AM, Spencer A, Wagner M.
dietary pattern in 15 year old adolescents following a 4 month dietary Skipping meals and alcohol consumption. The regulation of energy
intervention with school breakfast–a pilot study. Nutr J 2006;5:33. intake and expenditure among weight loss participants. Appetite 2008;
29. Rosado JL, del R Arellano M, Montemayor K, Garcı́a OP, Caamaño 51:538–45.
Mdel C. An increase of cereal intake as an approach to weight re- 50. Albertson AM, Anderson GH, Crockett SJ, Goebel MT. Ready-to-eat
duction in children is effective only when accompanied by nutrition cereal consumption: its relationship with BMI and nutrient intake of
education: a randomized controlled trial. Nutr J 2008;7:28. children aged 4 to 12 years. J Am Diet Assoc 2003;103:1613–9.

Downloaded from ajcn.nutrition.org by guest on April 30, 2016


30. Astbury NM, Taylor MA, Macdonald IA. Breakfast consumption af- 51. di Giuseppe R, Di Castelnuovo A, Melegari C, De Lucia F, Santimone
fects appetite, energy intake, and the metabolic and endocrine re- I, Sciarretta A, Barisciano P, Persichillo M, De Curtis A, Zito F, et al.
sponses to foods consumed later in the day in male habitual breakfast Typical breakfast food consumption and risk factors for cardiovascular
eaters. J Nutr 2011;141:1381–9. disease in a large sample of Italian adults. Nutrition, metabolism, and
31. Geliebter A, Yahav E, Forbes G, Hashim SA. Lunch meal intake fol- cardiovascular diseases. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis 2012;22:347–54.
lowing high and low glycemic breakfast cereals. FASEB J 1999;13: 52. Schaffner M. Always. Gastroenterol Nurs 2007;30:437–8.
A871–A. 53. Raynor HA, Jeffery RW, Ruggiero AM, Clark JM, Delahanty LM,
32. Geliebter A, Yahav E, Biase B, Hashim SA. Lunch meal intake fol- Look ARG. Weight loss strategies associated with BMI in overweight
lowing high and low fiber breakfast cereals. Obes Res 1999;7:44S. adults with type 2 diabetes at entry into the Look AHEAD (Action for
33. Kral TV, Whiteford LM, Heo M, Faith MS. Effects of eating breakfast Health in Diabetes) trial. Diabetes Care 2008;31:1299–304.
compared with skipping breakfast on ratings of appetite and intake at 54. Cho S, Dietrich M, Brown CJ, Clark CA, Block G. The effect of
subsequent meals in 8- to 10-y-old children. Am J Clin Nutr 2011;93: breakfast type on total daily energy intake and body mass index: results
284–91. from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey
34. Leidy HJ, Racki EM. The addition of a protein-rich breakfast and its (NHANES III). J Am Coll Nutr 2003;22:296–302.
effects on acute appetite control and food intake in ’breakfast-skipping’ 55. Laws KR. Negativland - a home for all findings in psychology. BMC
adolescents. Int J Obes 2010;34:1125–33. Psychology 2013;1.
35. Levitsky DA, Pacanowski CR. Effect of skipping breakfast on sub- 56. Neuroskeptic. Fixing science - systems and politics. 14 April 2012.
sequent energy intake. Physiol Behav 2013;119:9–16. Available from: http://neuroskeptic.blogspot.co.uk/2012/04/fixing-science-
36. Taylor MA, Garrow JS. Compared with nibbling, neither gorging nor systems-and-politics.html (cited 03 April 2013).
a morning fast affect short-term energy balance in obese patients in 57. Casazza K, Allison D. Stagnation in the clinical, community and public
a chamber calorimeter. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2001;25:519–28. health domain of obesity: the need for probative research. Clin Obes
37. Dialektakou KD, Vranas PB. Breakfast skipping and body mass index 2012;2:83–5.
among adolescents in Greece: whether an association exists depends on 58. Chandon P, Wansink B. The biasing health halos of fast-food restaurant
how breakfast skipping is defined. J Am Diet Assoc 2008;108:1517–25. health claims: lower calorie estimates and higher side-dish consump-
38. Sterne JA, Davey Smith G. Sifting the evidence-what’s wrong with tion intentions. J Consum Res 2007;34:301–14.
significance tests? BMJ 2001;322:226–31. 59. Kahneman D. Thinking fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus
39. Croezen S, Visscher TL, Ter Bogt NC, Veling ML, Haveman-Nies A. and Giroux, 2011.
Skipping breakfast, alcohol consumption and physical inactivity as 60. Festinger L. A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Standford
risk factors for overweight and obesity in adolescents: results of the University Press, 1957.
E-MOVO project. Eur J Clin Nutr 2009;63:405–12. 61. Boutron I, Dutton S, Ravaud P, Altman DG. Reporting and inter-
40. Kent LM, Worsley A. Breakfast size is related to body mass index for pretation of randomized controlled trials with statistically nonsig-
men, but not women. Nutr Res 2010;30:240–5. nificant results for primary outcomes. JAMA 2010;303:2058–64.
41. Fabritius K, Rasmussen M. [Breakfast habits and overweight in Danish 62. Yavchitz A, Boutron I, Bafeta A, Marroun I, Charles P, Mantz J,
schoolchildren. The role of socioeconomic positions] Ugeskr Laeger Ravaud P. Misrepresentation of randomized controlled trials in press
2008;170:2559–63 (in Danish). releases and news coverage: a cohort study. PLoS Med 2012;9:
42. Song WO, Chun OK, Obayashi S, Cho S, Chung CE. Is consumption of e1001308.
breakfast associated with body mass index in US adults? J Am Diet 63. Pronin E, Lin DY, Ross L. The bias blind spot: perceptions of bias in
Assoc 2005;105:1373–82. self versus others. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2002;28:369–81.

You might also like