You are on page 1of 2

Phoneme errors on a nonsense syllable test

S. B. Resnick, J. R. Dubno, S. Hoffnung, and H. Levitt

Citation: 58, S114 (1975); doi: 10.1121/1.2001878


View online: http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.2001878
View Table of Contents: http://asa.scitation.org/toc/jas/58/S1
Published by the Acoustical Society of America
S114 90th Meeting:AcousticalSocietyof America S114

11:30 11:50

BBB18. Analysis of recognition scores of hearing-impaired


BBB16. Intelligibility of speechwith simulated single reflec- subjectsusing Pairbanks Rhyme Test. B. Franklin (Depart-
tion for normal and hearing-impaired subjects. Anna K.
ment of Special Education, San Francisco State University,
Nabelek, Elizabeth Slawinska, and Larry N. Robinette San Francisco, CA 94132)
(Audiology and Speech Pathology Department, University of
Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37916) The five lists of the Fairbanks Rhyme Test were presented
to the better ear of 12 teeffagers with bilateral, congenital,
Simulated reflection was produced by a computer as a de- moderate to profound sensorineural hearing losses at 20 dB
layed form of the speech material (Modified Rhyme Test). above theft speech reception thresholds. Analysis of the recog-
The delays were 0, 5, 10, 20, 40, 80, and 160 msec. The nition scores for each consonant for all subjects revealed that
signals were fed separately into two loudspeakers, positioned the/s/score of 69% was the highest. The average score for
in a sound-insulated booth 6 ft in front of a subject and form- alveolars /d/ and /t/ of 49% was almost double the average
ing a 60ø angle. The intelligibility was tested with five to nine score for labials/b/and/p/ of 25%. The relatively high
normal and hearing-impaired subjects in binaural and monaural scores for/s/, /d/, and/t/, and low scores for/b/and/p/
conditions with masking noise (babble of eight voices) reducing are different from what one would expect on basis of subjects'
intelligibility to about 75%. The intelligibility of normals did audiograms in relation to acoustic energy in these consonants.
not change up to 20-msec delay and decreased for longer de- There was an unexpected variation in the number of times
lays for both conditions. Scores were lower for monaural the same consonant was correctly identified on different lists,
listening. Such an advantage for binaural listening was not for example, /b/--eight times for list V and 21 times for
observed for the impaired listeners. In both conditions, their list I. There were also unexpected discrepancies in the number
intelligibility decreased gradually as the duration increased. of correct responsesfor certain words, for example, "fame,"
The lack of plateau in the impaired subjects' results might "vain," "fed," and "buck"were missedby all 12 subjects;
indicate that any reflected energy in reverberant rooms is whereas "look" and "saw" were correctly identifiedby 11
harmful for their intelligibility. Normals, however, might subjects.
benefit from early reflections. [Supportedby NIH NISND
12:00
grant 1 R01 NS 10962-01. ]
BBB19. Phoneme errors on a nonsense syllable test. S.B.
Resnick, J.R. Dubno, S. Hoffnung, and H. Levitt (Communi-
11:40 cation Sciences Laboratory, City University of New York
Graduate School and University Center, 33 West 42nd Street,
BBB17. Speech reception threshold testing materials. R.A. New York, NY 10036)
Campbell (Speech Communication Department, State University In an effort to construct a test which permits detailed analy-
of New York at Buffalo, Buffalo, NY 14226) sis of the phonemic errors made by naive hearing-impaired
individuals under conditions of hearing aid use, we have dev-
This study was carried out to increase the number of sponda- eloped a closed response nonsense syllable test consisting of
ic words available for speech reception threshold testing. Al- CV, VC, and CVC syllables organized into modules. The
though recent trends in clinical speech reception testing has modules differ with respect to the class of consonants repre-
been toward use of small lists of spondaic words, with which sented, the consonant position within the syllable, and the
the testee has been familiarized, it was felt that the general vowel context (/i/, /a/, or/u/). Responsesto syllables within
utility of such testing would be improved by having available a given module are limited to syllables within the same module.
a larger number of words which could be used for extensive The response foils (which correspond to all the syllables with-
testing without having to repeat. Particular attention was in a module) include the most frequent perceptual confusions
paid to the intelligibility of these additional words as com- reported for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired individuals.
pared to CID Auditory Tests W-1 and W-2. Thirty words The results of tests administered to hearing-impaired individ-
from an experimental list of 250 spondaic words were admin- uals under difficult listening conditions indicate a pattern of
istered to a total of 84 clinical patients at their previously errors consistent with those reported by previous investigators,
established speech reception threshold. From the data, a total including (1) greater errors on final than on initial conson-
of 150 new spondaic words, arranged in five 30 word lists, ants; (2) greater errors on unvoiced than on voiced consonants;
were obtained which appear to be more homogeneous in intelli- and (3) a dependence of consonant identification on vowel
gibility than the CID lists. context. [Research supportedby NINCDS. ]

J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 58, Suppl. No. 1, Fall 1975

You might also like