You are on page 1of 6

Acta Pædiatrica ISSN 0803-5253

REGULAR ARTICLE

Children aged 6–24 months like to watch YouTube videos but could not
learn anything from them
Savita Yadav (savitaydv@yahoo.com)1, Pinaki Chakraborty1, Prabhat Mittal2, Udit Arora1
1.Division of Computer Engineering, Netaji Subhas Institute of Technology, New Delhi, India
2.Department of Commerce, Satyawati College (Evening), University of Delhi, Delhi, India

Keywords ABSTRACT
Infant entertainment, Infant learning, Smartphone, Aim: Parents sometimes show young children YouTube videos on their smartphones. We
Video, YouTube
studied the interaction of 55 Indian children born between December 2014 and May
Correspondence 2015 who watched YouTube videos when they were 6–24 months old.
S Yadav, Division of Computer Engineering, Netaji
Subhas Institute of Technology, New Delhi 110078, Methods: The children were recruited by the researchers using professional and personal
India. contacts and visited by the same two observers at four ages, for at least 10 minutes. The
Tel: 00 91 99680 77734 |
observers recorded the children’s abilities to interact with touch screens and identify people
Fax: 00 91 11250 00151 |
Email: savitaydv@yahoo.com in videos and noted what videos attracted them the most.
Received
Results: The children were attracted to music at six months of age and were interested in
9 October 2017; revised 18 December 2017; watching the videos at 12 months. They could identify their parents in videos at
accepted 13 February 2018. 12 months and themselves by 24 months. They started touching the screen at 18 months
DOI:10.1111/apa.14291 and could press the buttons that appeared on the screen, but did not understand their use.
The children preferred watching dance performances by multiple artists with melodical
music, advertisements for products they used and videos showing toys and balloons.
Conclusion: Children up to two years of age could be entertained and kept busy by
showing them YouTube clips on smartphones, but did not learn anything from the videos.

INTRODUCTION children have been reported to be attracted to salient


Parents and other adults often hand their smartphones to auditory and visual features like applause and visual
young children for short periods of time (1), perhaps to surprises. The attentional shift from such salient features
pacify them or to divert their attention from something. This to general content starts between 18 and 30 months of age.
can allow parents to do some important work while Barr et al. (5) reported that children aged 12–18 months
remaining close to their children. We conducted a longitu- paid more attention to media if they had prior exposure to
dinal study where we observed the interaction of children such media and watching with their parents helped young
from six months to two years of age with smartphones. children to concentrate on media content. The same
Children of this age cannot use apps, but they are attracted observations were also made by Fidler et al. (6) when they
to videos played on smartphones and can be kept busy with studied children aged 6–18 months.
them for some minutes. As a result, parents and other adults Krcmar et al. (7) found that children aged 15–24 months
often play YouTube videos on their smartphones and lend were able to learn novel words from watching television,
them to children, typically for up to five minutes. This often with some support from parents. However, the extent of this
becomes the first interaction children have with digital learning and its long-term effects is debatable (8,9). Several
media.
Diener et al. (2) observed that children aged nine to ten
months were able to distinguish between real-world events Key notes
and videos and that videos conveyed meaning and emo-  Observers studied the interaction of 55 children who
tional impact to children of this age. However, children watched YouTube videos on smartphones when they
were more interested in, and could be better engaged by, were 6–24 months of age.
real-world presentations. Demers et al. (3) observed that  At six months, they were attracted to music, at
children aged 12–21 months assigned greater value to 12 months, they watched videos and identified their
media content that their parents were looking at and were parents and at 18 months, they touched buttons, but
more likely to look towards a screen and for a longer time if did not know what they did.
their parents were also looking at it. Valkenburg and  Children up to 24 months were entertained by watch-
Vroone (4) found that children paid attention to media ing YouTube clips, but did not learn anything from
content that was only moderately different from their them.
existing knowledge and capabilities. Moreover, young

©2018 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1
Impact of YouTube videos on infants Yadav et al.

other studies have actually arrived at a different conclusion. common household gadgets since birth. Their parents often
Robb et al. (10) did not find any evidence of videos helping lent them their smartphones with YouTube videos playing.
children aged 12–15 months to learn words, and Christakis On most occasions, the videos were of song and dance
(11) also made similar observations. Richert et al. (12) performances in the families’ native language. Most of the
found that children aged 12–24 months could hardly learn videos featured multiple dancers. The songs were generally
words from videos without parental intervention. sung by multiple singers, with elaborate orchestra and
DeLoache et al. (13) found that children aged 12– chorus support. We performed five experiments on these
18 months could not learn words from videos and felt that children.
parents who were sympathetic towards media use some-
times overestimated how much their children were learning Experiment one
from such media. We tried to understand how the behaviour of the children
Richards and Cronise (14) found that children aged 6– towards YouTube videos changed over time. We visited the
12 months could not follow the sequential audio and video children when they were 6, 12, 18 and 24 months old at
of a programme. They were unable to differentiate between their homes in the presence of their parents. The same two
a genuine video from a sequence of random screenshots. observers visited all the children every time, and they
Nevertheless, Kabali et al. (15) observed that slightly older observed each child for at least 10 minutes. They observed
children liked to watch videos on YouTube and other video the children watching videos on YouTube without any
sharing websites. Dayanim and Namy (16) found that intervention.
children aged 15 months could learn signs for expressive
communication from videos. However, young children Experiment two
preferred to interact more with the real world than We tried to determine when the children learned to identify
watching videos. This was verified by Barr and Hayne themselves and people they have met in real life in videos.
(17), who found that children aged 12–18 months were able Videos of the children, their relatives and, in some cases,
to imitate actions better if they had seen them in real world family pets were recorded. These videos were uploaded to
than in videos. Courage et al. (18) also found that children YouTube and shown to the children on a smartphone. Two
aged 6–18 months paid more attention to toys than to types of videos were shown to the children. Some of the
videos. videos were recent, while the others were recorded six
Smartphones are highly portable, children seem to like months previously. The children were asked to identify the
them (19), and they can be used to show videos to children people and the animals in the videos. The experiment was
anywhere and at any time. The aim of this study was to conducted when the children were 12, 18 and 24 months
examine the interactions of children when they viewed old.
YouTube videos played on smartphones at four ages
between 6 and 24 months. Experiment three
We tried to determine the ability of the children to identify
people who featured in the videos shown to them but were
METHODS otherwise not known to them. The children were shown a
This was a study of children born between December video of a dance performance in which one dancer
2014 and May 2015 and living in New Delhi, India. The appeared prominently. The video was shown to the children
researchers recruited children using their professional and repeatedly, and then they were asked to identify the same
personal contacts, and they included the children of dancer in other videos. The experiment was conducted
relatives, colleagues, neighbours and friends. We met the when the children were 18 and 24 months old.
parents of the children when they were six months old
and asked them whether they had used music on their Experiment four
smartphones to quieten their children down at that age. If We studied how easily the children interacted with smart-
they said yes then we explained the objectives of our phones using a custom designed video player app that
study to the parents and included those children with the looked similar to the YouTube app, but had five modes of
parents’ verbal consent. There were no exclusion criteria. operation. In mode one, the control buttons were displayed
We visited the children four times over the next on the screen for the entire duration of the video. In mode
18 months at their homes in the presence of at least two, the buttons appeared when the screen was touched
one of their parents. We approached 88 parents, started and vanished if the screen was not touched for three
the study with 62 children and completed it with 55 seconds. Mode three was a variation of mode two, where
children (31 girls). We had to discontinue visiting seven the buttons reappeared at random positions every time the
children when it became difficult to get appointments screen was touched. The buttons were half the size in mode
with their parents. We did not encourage the parents to four and the buttons appeared in random colours in mode
play music or videos to the children in any way at any five. A video was played using the app, and the children
stage of the study. were allowed to interact with the app without any inter-
The children belonged to upper-class Indian families and vention. The experiment was conducted when the children
had been exposed to smartphones along with other were 18 and 24 months old.

2 ©2018 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Yadav et al. Impact of YouTube videos on infants

Experiment five less frequent, and most of the children (67%) were now
We tried to find out what videos attracted the children more interested in watching the videos. The children maintained
when they were 24 months old. We classified the YouTube a steady head position and eye gaze while watching the
videos that were typically shown to them into 10 categories: videos. The children listened to the music and watched the
a dance by multiple dancers with melodical music, a dance videos passively. The children did not respond even if the
by a single dancer with melodical music, singing without display was turned off suddenly with the music continuing
dancing, playing musical instruments without singing and to play.
dancing, movies and soap operas, cartoons, news and talk At the age of 18 months, the children continued to listen
shows, commercial advertisements, videos showing toys to music and watch videos played on YouTube. More
and balloons and videos of animals, birds, butterflies and children (65%) now asked for videos to be played to them
fishes. The children were shown five videos from each and were adamant about it sometimes. The children were
category. The number of times the children watched a video more active now and wanted to participate in the activity.
for one minute or more was noted. The children observed how adults played the videos by
touching the buttons that appeared on the smartphone
Statistics screen. Most of the children (76%) kept touching the screen
The behaviour of the children who participated in the study and when the buttons appeared they could press them with
was analysed with frequency and descriptive statistics with ease. However, the children did not understand the use of
95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) across different age the different buttons. They expressed displeasure if the
categories. We used SPSS version 19 (IBM Corp, Armonk, video stopped playing because they pressed the buttons
NY, USA) to report the results. Possible interactions randomly. The children generally (56%) got confused if the
between age categories were evaluated, and differences display was turned off suddenly. Some of them picked up
were tested using the k-proportion chi-square test. Because the smartphone and turned it over expecting to find the
experiment three and experiment four were only performed dancers there. A few of the children (16%) sometimes
when the children were 18 and 24 months old, we used the confused a dancer with their relatives and uttered their
Z-test to find significant differences between the propor- names.
tions at those ages. At the age of 24 months, the children were more
confident users of smartphone. They could hold a smart-
phone in their hands firmly and were used to watching
RESULTS videos on YouTube. The children asked for YouTube videos
In experiment one, we observed that the children were to be played to them more frequently (80%) than before.
attracted towards the music in YouTube videos at the age of The children (80%) tried to interact more with the smart-
six months (Table 1). Although the children listened curi- phone and touched the screen more frequently. However,
ously to sounds from different sources at this age, they were the children could only make simple gestures with their
attracted more towards YouTube videos especially if they fingers on the screen and could not control a smartphone.
played melodical music. Some children (31%) danced when The children now rarely thought a dancer was one of their
the music was played and the nature of the dancing varied relatives.
widely among the children. Some shook their limbs slightly The chi-square k-proportion test (k = 4) was conducted
while others jumped up and down if someone held their to test the differences in proportions across the age
torsos and enabled them to stay in a standing position. At categories. It was observed that all treatments were signif-
this age, the children showed no interest in watching the icantly different at a 0.1% level across the age categories.
videos. The children did not ask for videos to be played at In experiment two, we observed that the children were
this age either. mostly (89%) able to identify their relatives and pets in the
At the age of 12 months, some children (38%) asked for videos at the age of 12 months (Table 2). However, the
YouTube videos to be played to them. The dancing became children were not able to identify themselves in the videos

Table 1 Results of experiment one (n = 55)


Number of children who . . . Six months 12 months 18 months 24 months Chi-square statistic p Value

Were attracted to, and listened to, music from videos played on YouTube 55 55 55 55 – –
Danced when music was played 17 1 2 4 31.054 <0.001
Showed interest in watching the videos Nil 37 52 48 130.382 <0.001
Asked for videos to be played Nil 21 36 44 82.204 <0.01
Got confused when the display was turned off suddenly Nil Nil 31 2 98.787 <0.01
Misidentified a dancer as a parent or other relative Nil Nil 9 1 23.885 <0.01
Were keen in touching the screen and pressed the buttons when they appeared Nil Nil 42 44 141.346 <0.01
Could hold the smartphone firmly Nil Nil Nil 55 220.000 <0.01

©2018 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 3
Impact of YouTube videos on infants Yadav et al.

at this age. At the age of 18 months, some children (44%) indicated that proportion of children aged 18 and
were able to identify themselves in recent videos, but were 24 months of age were significantly different in identifying
unable to identify themselves in videos recorded six months the dancers when they appeared in another video in a
earlier. In some cases, the children thought the children in similar dress.
the videos were other children they knew. At the age of In experiment four, we observed that the children
24 months, most children (98%) were able to identify typically (64%) learnt to press the control buttons displayed
themselves in recent videos, and some (25%) were also on the screen by 18 months of age (Table 4). The children
able to identify themselves in older videos. The chi-square could press the buttons irrespective of whether they then
k-proportion test (k = 3) was conducted to test the differ- vanished, their random positions on the screen, smaller size
ences in proportions across the age categories. It was and random colours by 24 months of age (69%). However,
observed that all treatments were significantly different at a the children did not understand the use of the different
1% level across the age categories. buttons and kept pressing them randomly. The children
In experiment three, we observed that the children were could not use the sliding seek bar at this age. Table 4 also
not able to identify a prominently featured dancer when reports the Z statistics and the significance values of
they appeared in other videos when they were 18 months differences in the treatments for children aged 18 and
old (Table 3). At the age of 24 months, some children 24 months old. Differences in the proportion of children
(49%) were able to identify the dancer when they appeared belonging to the two age categories were found to be
in another video in a similar dress. However, the children insignificant with respect to pressing the buttons that
were not able to identify the dancer in other videos where appeared permanently on the screen and the buttons that
they appeared in other outfits. To find the significant appeared on touching the screen. On the contrary, the
differences in the proportion of two independent samples, ability to press the buttons whose position and colour
we used the Z-test. The significance value (p < 0.01) changed and the buttons of smaller size was significantly
different at 1 and 5%, respectively, for children aged 18 and
24 months old.
Table 5 reports the mean, standard error and 95% CI of
Table 2 Results of experiment two (n = 55) the number of videos a child watched for one minute or
Number of children 12 18 24 Chi-square more in experiment five. It was observed that children aged
who were . . . months months months statistic p Value 24 months preferred to watch dances by multiple dancers
with melodical music most (mean score 3.76, 95% CI: 0.50–
Able to identify 49 55 55 12.452 <0.01
1.03). Commercial advertisements (mean score 2.82) and
relatives and pets
videos showing toys and balloons (mean score 2.24) also
Able to identify self 1 24 54 102.924 <0.01
Able to identify self Nil Nil 14 30.596 <0.01
attracted children at this age. Furthermore, when we tested
in an older video the average scores at three, we found that the average scores
of most of the types of videos were significantly different
than three at the 1% level.

Table 3 Results of experiment three (n = 55) DISCUSSION


Number of children who were . . . 18 months 24 months Z value p Value
This study of children at four ages observed some interest-
ing trends regarding the use of smartphones. How often and
Able to identify a dancer Nil 27 5.982 <0.01 for how long children watched YouTube videos largely
in similar dresses depended on their parents. Lauricella et al. (20) made
Able to identify a dancer Nil Nil – – similar observations. We found that the total amount of
in different dresses
time the children spent watching YouTube videos typically
varied from 0 to 15 minutes per week. The children liked to

Table 4 Results of experiment four (n = 55)


Number of children who could . . . 18 months 24 months Z value p Value

Press buttons that appeared permanently on the screen 35 41 1.238 >0.05


Press buttons that appeared on touching the screen 35 41 1.238 >0.05
Press buttons whose positions on the screen changed Nil 38 7.619 <0.01
Press buttons of smaller size 19 41 4.213 <0.01
Press buttons of different colours 28 40 2.355 <0.05
Understand the use of the buttons Nil Nil – –
Control the seek bar Nil Nil – –

4 ©2018 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Yadav et al. Impact of YouTube videos on infants

Table 5 The number of videos a child watched for one minute or more in experiment five
Type of videos Mean and SE 95% CI

Dance by multiple dancers with melodical music 3.76* (0.13) 0.50 1.03
Dance by a single dancer with melodical music 1.91* (0.17) 1.43 0.75
Singing without dancing 0.64* (0.08) 2.53 2.20
Playing of musical instruments without singing and dancing 0.65* (0.09) 2.54 2.15
Movies and soap operas 1.82* (0.15) 1.49 0.88
Cartoon films 1.98* (0.14) 1.30 0.74
News and talk shows 0.71* (0.11) 2.52 2.07
Commercial advertisements 2.82 (0.18) 0.55 0.19
Videos showing toys and balloons 2.24* (0.18) 1.12 0.40
Videos of animals, birds, butterflies and fishes 1.02* (0.12) 2.21 1.75

*p < 0.01.

hold a smartphone in their hands and touch its screen and smartphones when they were 6–24 months of age. At six
that the size and portability of a smartphone made it more months, they were attracted to music, at 12 months, they
attractive for children than a personal computer. However, watched videos and identified their parents, and at
the children were not able to control a smartphone at the 18 months, they touched buttons, but did not know what
age of two years. they did. Our study concluded that children up to
By the age of two years, children can identify characters 24 months of age were entertained by watching YouTube
that appear in the videos regularly shown to them. The clips, but did not learn anything from them.
characters may be historic, mythological, cartoon or fairy-
tale characters or relatives they have never met. Children
become more interested in characters if their photographs FINANCE
or stickers are displayed in their homes. However, children This study did not receive any specific funding.
cannot remember the story of a fairytale, a cartoon or any
other play even after seeing it several times. Two-year-old
children are quite keen on watching advertisements for CONFLICTS OF INTEREST
products they consume, such as baby food, chocolates, The authors have no conflict of interests to declare.
biscuits, toys, balloons and diapers. They are also more
interested in real videos than in animated ones at this age.
Children typically want to see a few favourite videos every References
time they get a smartphone. 1. Chau CL. Positive technological development for young
Two-year-old children cannot learn any new word from children in the context of children’s mobile apps. PhD
videos by their own. However, children can learn words dissertation. Medford, MA: Tufts University, 2014.
that are used in videos if parents and other adults teach 2. Diener ML, Pierroutsakos SL, Troseth GL, Roberts A. Video
them those words later. Children can only learn a few new versus reality: infants’ attention and affective responses to video
and live presentations. Media Psychol 2008; 11: 418–41.
words at the age of two years and tend to forget them after a
3. Demers LB, Hanson KG, Kirkorian HL, Pempek TA, Anderson
few hours unless they are regularly used by adults in their DR. Infant gaze following during parent-infant co-viewing of
homes. baby videos. Child Dev 2013; 84: 591–603.
Children who are used to watching YouTube videos may 4. Valkenburg PM, Vroone M. Developmental changes in infants’
at time ask for videos to be played to them. However, that and toddlers’ attention to television entertainment. Commun
behaviour is hardly compulsive. Courage et al. (18) Res 2004; 31: 288–311.
5. Barr R, Zack E, Garcia A, Muentener P. Infants’ attention and
reported that children preferred to play with toys than
responsiveness to television increases with prior exposure and
watch videos. We also observed that children who watched parental interaction. Infancy 2008; 13: 30–56.
YouTube videos also played with their toys and interacted 6. Fidler AE, Zack E, Barr R. Television viewing patterns in 6- to
normally in the physical world. In our opinion, watching 18-month-olds: the role of caregiver-infant interactional
YouTube videos does not provide any developmental quality. Infancy 2010; 15: 1–21.
benefits for children up to two years of age, but they are 7. Krcmar M, Grela B, Lin K. Can toddlers learn vocabulary from
not harmful either. television? An experimental approach. Media Psychol 2007;
10: 41–63.
8. AAP. Media use by children younger than 2 years. Pediatrics
2011; 128: 1040–5.
CONCLUSION 9. Reid Chassiakos Y, Radesky J, Christakis D, Moreno MA, Cross
This was an observational study that examined the interac- C. Children and adolescents and digital media. Pediatrics 2016;
tion of 55 children who watched YouTube videos on 138: e1–18.

©2018 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 5
Impact of YouTube videos on infants Yadav et al.

10. Robb MB, Richert RA, Wartella E. Just a talking book? word media devices by young children. Pediatrics 2015; 136:
learning from watching baby videos. Brit J Dev Psychol 2009; 1044–50.
27: 27–45. 16. Dayanim S, Namy LL. Infants learn baby signs from video.
11. Christakis DA. The effects of infant media usage: what do we Child Dev 2015; 86: 800–11.
know and what should we learn? Acta Paediatr 2009; 98: 17. Barr R, Hayne H. Developmental changes in imitation from
8–16. television during infancy. Child Dev 1999; 70: 1067–81.
12. Richert RA, Robb MB, Fender JG, Wartella E. Word learning 18. Courage ML, Murphy AN, Goulding S, Setliff AE. When the
from baby videos. Arch Pediat Adol Med 2010; 164: 432–7. television is on: the impact of infant-directed video on 6- and
13. DeLoache JS, Chiong C, Sherman K, Islam N, Vanderborght 18-month-olds’ attention during toy play and on parent-infant
M, Troseth GL, et al. Do babies learn from baby media? interaction. Infant Behav Dev 2010; 33: 176–88.
Psychol Sci 2010; 21: 1570–4. 19. Yadav S, Chakraborty P. Using smartphones with suitable apps
14. Richards JE, Cronise K. Extended visual fixation in the early can be safe and even useful if they are not misused or overused.
preschool years: look duration, heart rate changes, and Acta Paediatr 2018; 107: 384–7.
attentional inertia. Child Dev 2000; 71: 602–20. 20. Lauricella AR, Wartella E, Rideout VJ. Young children’s screen
15. Kabali HK, Irigoyen MM, Nunez-Davis R, Budacki JG, time: the complex role of parent and child factors. J Appl Dev
Mohanty SH, Leister KP, et al. Exposure and use of mobile Psychol 2015; 36: 11–7.

6 ©2018 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd

You might also like