You are on page 1of 12

Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Teaching and Teacher Education


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tate

Biology instruction using a generic framework of scientific reasoning


and argumentation
Tobias Dorfner*, Christian Fo
€rtsch, Michael Germ, Birgit J. Neuhaus
Biology Education, Department of Biology I, LMU Munich, Germany

h i g h l i g h t s

 Fostering students' learning requires further and new implications.


 SRA offers the possibility to foster students' learning.
 We observe biology lessons using a framework of SRA.
 The use of SRA and its influence on students' achievement are demonstrated.
 The study offers a complementary perspective on how to foster students' learning.

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: We investigated biology instructiondusing a generic framework of scientific reasoning and argumen-
Received 29 June 2017 tation (SRA) with eight epistemic activitiesdon how to foster student learning in biological literacy
Received in revised form which had not been clarified in previous studies. Our analysis of videotaped biology lessons and student
28 May 2018
achievement showed varying frequencies in using these activities and their effects on achievement.
Accepted 4 July 2018
Those students taught with more epistemic activities had higher achievement. We believe that the SRA
framework can be a worthwhile methodical tool for teaching biology to foster student learning.
Therefore, we draw practically orientated implications for educational research, practitioners, teacher
Keywords:
Scientific literacy
educators, and curriculum developers.
Scientific reasoning and argumentation © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Biology instruction
Video study

1. Introduction knowledge, scientific inquiry, communication, and scientific judge-


ment. These four competencies could be seen as subdimensions of
More than ten years ago, low results of German students in scientific literacy and could be divided into one content sub-
scientific literacy from international comparative studies, like the dimension: content knowledge and three process subdimensions:
Third International Mathematics and Science Study 1995 (Baumert, scientific inquiry, communication, and scientific judgment (Kampa &
Bos, & Lehmann, 2000) or the Programme for International Stu- Ko€ ller, 2016).
dents Assessment 2000 (Baumert et al., 2001) led to the intro- Despite the time that has passed since the introduction of the
duction of the German National Education Standards (NES) for German NES, the question of how to foster students' learning
three subjects, biology, chemistry, and physics in 2005 (The within these four subdimensions still plays a major role in the
Standing Conference of the Ministers of Education and Cultural agendas for researchers as well as instructional approaches for
Affairs of The La€nder in the Federal Republic of Germany [KMK], teachers. In different publications, scientific reasoning is discussed
2005a; 2005b; 2005c). These standards are the framework for a as one crucial element of the subdimension scientific inquiry
national evaluation of these subjects. To clearly mention students' (Hartmann, Upmeier zu Belzen, Krüger, & Pant, 2015; Mayer, 2007;
performance, four competencies were established: content Nowak, Nehring, Tiemann, & Upmeier zu Belzen, 2013;
Zimmerman, 2005). By comparing different approaches which aim
that students become deeply involved with authentic processes of
* Corresponding author. Biology Education, Department of Biology I, Ludwig- scientific reasoning and argumentation (SRA), Fischer et al. (2014)
Maximilians University Munich, Winzererstraße 45/II, 80797, Munich, Germany. described that “it has been found crucial to provide students with
E-mail address: tobias.dorfner@bio.lmu.de (T. Dorfner).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2018.07.003
0742-051X/© 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243 233

structural support” (p. 31). So, scaffolding students with prompts suggested by Fischer et al. (2014). Thereby, their theoretical
and hints on SRAdin individual learning or whole classroom assumption refers to the following two constructs: scientific
learningdis an effective method to foster students' scientific reasoning and argumentation.
reasoning skills (Fischer et al., 2014). On the basis of current
research findings and deficits from studies on SRA (for details of the 2.2. Conceptualizations of scientific reasoning and argumentation
studies, see Fischer et al., 2014, pp. 29e31), Fischer et al. argued to
expand future research on SRA by the range of epistemic activities, Scientific reasoning relates to proof of a theory by collecting data
and therefore, developed a framework of SRA with eight epistemic about the natural world and to recognize patterns in the data. It also
activities usable across domains. So, using this generic framework relates to formulation of theories and hypotheses referring to the
as a structural element for biology instruction seems to be a natural world and to verify or reject those hypotheses with ex-
worthwhile and promising approach to foster students' scientific periments (Furtak, Hardy, Beinbrech, Shavelson, & Shemwell, 2010;
reasoning skills. Thereby, we try to create our awareness that it is Lawson, 2005). In brief, reasoning means to draw conclusions from
interesting and rewarding to take elements from the research on principles and from evidence (Wason & Johnson-Laird, 1972). A
SRA and to use them in biology instruction. It is obvious that some prominent and strong conceptualization of scientific reasoning is
elements are already established in biology instruction, but we the model of scientific discovery as dual search (SDDS) from Klahr
demonstrate further important aspects of the research on SRA that and Dunbar (1988). They assume that “scientific reasoning requires
are useful and interesting for teaching biology. By doing so, we search in two related problem spaces: the hypothesis space, con-
want to connect research disciplines, namely, the research on SRA sisting of the hypotheses generated during the discovery process,
and the research in biology education. We are convinced that and the experiment space, consisting of all possible experiments
research in both disciplines could benefit by mutual consideration, that could be conducted” (p. 32). In this model, scientific reasoning
but there is a need to demonstrate this mutual benefit. Important to is an interaction of nonlinear processes consisting of formulating
mention is that teaching biology should definitively not be rein- hypotheses, designing experiments for testing the hypotheses, and
vented with our approach. But, some aspects are new or differ in evaluating the hypotheses.
comparison to the already known elements of scientific inquiry. Argumentation is part of scientific reasoning and arguments
Therefore, in our study, we were interested in how often the given by students and teachers in science classrooms are collected
eight epistemic activities of SRA from the framework of Fischer to provide support for previously made claims. This process also
et al. (2014) had already been used in videotaped biology lessons includes the creation of artefacts for proofing claims (Furtak et al.,
from grade nine (N ¼ 85 videos), and which of these epistemic 2010; Sampson & Clark, 2008). In the research field of argumen-
activities had been used in the lessons. Furthermore, we focused on tation, various conceptualizations of argumentation exist (for an
whether or not the used epistemic activities have an effect on overview, see Bricker & Bell, 2008). In general, Bricker and Bell
students' achievement in biology. More precisely, we had the (2012) defined argumentation as “the process of constructing ar-
following two main goals in our study: (1) to show the current use guments” (p. 119) and described that it is field-dependent and
of the eight epistemic activities in regular biology instruction, and situated. Rationally, we briefly review here some theories of argu-
(2) to show the effects of the epistemic activities on students' mentation that fit with Bricker and Bell's conceptualization and
achievement in biology. which could influence classroom practice. Osborne, Erduran, and
Simon (2004) named the contribution of Kuhn (1991) as signifi-
2. Theoretical background cant to the research field of argumentation because Kuhn showed
the “basic capacity of individuals to use reasoned argument” (p.
To frame our study, we start with a short general introduction to 996). Kuhn (1991) concluded that children and adults have poor
scientific reasoning and argumentation (SRA). Then, we illustrate capacity to coordinate and to establish a connection between data
theoretical conceptualizations of SRA and subsequently describe and theory. However, Kosloswski (as cited in Osborne et al., 2004)
the generic framework of SRA from Fischer et al. (2014). Finally, we criticized “Kuhn's emphasis on covariation” (p. 996) and stressed
demonstrate the interconnection of SRA and educational standards, the interdependence of data and theory (as cited in Bricker & Bell,
which give reasons for using SRA in our study to foster students' 2008). Therefore, Kuhn (as cited in Osborne et al., 2004) “was less
learning within the correlated subdimensions content knowledge doubtful of young people's ability to reason” (p. 996), because
and scientific inquiry. missing theoretical or conceptual knowledge could crucially reduce
people's reasoning ability (Osborne et al., 2004). Furthermore,
2.1. General introduction to scientific reasoning and argumentation Andriessen (as cited in Bricker & Bell, 2008) “proposes the notion of
collaborative argumentation, which he says is the type of argu-
Over the previous years, research in cognitive and develop- mentation in which scientists engage” (p. 490). According to
mental psychology has been implemented to study constructs such Andriessen (as cited in Bricker & Bell, 2008), this type of argu-
as scientific reasoning and argumentation (Chinn & Clark, 2013; mentation should be taught to students so that they could “arguing
Zimmerman, 2000). Research in this area has the basic intention to to learn” (p. 490). By conducting a systematic review of argument
foster knowledge about the natural world and to support processes interventions in K-12 science contexts, Cavagnetto (2010) identified
to explore, evaluate, revise, and communicate that knowledge three orientations that are used as approaches for learning of
(Klahr, Zimmerman, & Jirout, 2011). This scientific knowledge could argument. In short, these are named as (1) immersion, (2) structure,
be generated with a combination of scientific process skills like and (3) socioscientific. The interventions with an orientation of (1)
observing or questioning (Klahr & Dunbar, 1988; Kremer, Specht, immersion had their focus on emphasizing the “relationship be-
Urhahne, & Mayer, 2013; Lederman, Abd-El-Khalick, Bell, & tween investigation and explanation” (Cavagnetto, 2010, p. 347).
Schwartz, 2002; Roberts & Gott, 1999). However, Fischer et al. Therefore, “scaffolds were used to inform both argument con-
(2014) remarked that there is need for a more interdisciplinary struction and student investigative decisions” (Cavagnetto, 2010, p.
research to condense scattered knowledge on this topic over 347). The interventions using an orientation of (2) structure
different research disciplines. Therefore, a generic framework of focused on the construction of explanations by “explicitly teaching
SRAdto foster the learning of scientific reasoning skills domain- a structure for argument and asking students to apply the structure
independently and interdisciplinarilydwas developed and in scientific and sometimes socioscientific contexts” (Cavagnetto,
234 T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243

2010, p. 348). The last orientation of (3) socioscientific focused on crucial necessity to identify domain-specific elements of this
the interaction of society and science. This means, “moral, ethical, framework. This aims to clearly mention similar and different skills
and political influences on decision making in scientific contexts” of SRA across domains.
(Cavagnetto, p. 349) were used. In fact, Cavagnetto (2010) consti-
tuted that “the immersion orientation appears to fully capture the 2.4. The interconnection of SRA and educational standards
culture, including the epistemic nature, of science that is embedded
in scientific practice” (p. 352). “Science educators, scientists, and policy makers agree that
development of students' scientific literacy is an important aim of
2.3. Fostering interdisciplinary research e a generic framework of science education” (Gormally, Brickman, & Lutz, 2012). Content
SRA (Fischer et al., 2014) knowledge and scientific inquiry are crucial subdimensions of sci-
entific literacy (Bybee, 2002; Kampa & Ko €ller, 2016; Shavelson
In general, studies within SRA can be divided into two main et al., 2008). Kampa and Ko € ller (2016) described that content
types of investigation: (1) studies on types of knowledge, which are knowledge contains knowledge about facts and concepts. So, this
further divided into (a) domain-specific knowledge, including subdimension relates to the “declarative (factual, conceptual)
topics from physics, chemistry, biology, Earth sciences, and so on, knowledge …. and being able to reason with this knowledge”
and (b) domain-general knowledge, containing aspects such as (Shavelson et al., 2008, p. 302). Important to mention is that con-
equilibrium, time, feedback, and causality, and (2) studies on types tent knowledge is not simply a recall of knowledge; it describes
of scientific processes, which includes formulation of hypotheses, rather active dealing and working with scientific content for solv-
design of experiments and observations, and evaluation of evi- ing problems (Kampa & Ko €ller, 2016; Pant et al., 2012). Further-
dence (Klahr et al., 2011; Zimmerman, 2000). Fischer et al. (2014) more, Kampa and Ko €ller (as cited in Shavelson et al., 2008)
described that research on SRA “has substantially progressed over described that scientific inquiry refers to the “procedural (step-by-
the last two decades” (p. 32), but there are still important gaps step or condition-action) knowledge and reasoning with this
within this research field. For example, “while research on SRA knowledge” (p. 302). So, ways to answer scientific questions can be
focused on commonalities across domains, investigations on the defined as scientific inquiry (Nowak et al., 2013), but there is not yet
differences of SRA between disciplines have been rare (e.g., a homogeneous definition of this construct (Lederman &
Herrenkohl & Cornelius, 2013)” (p. 35). Therefore, they argued for Lederman, 2012; Nowak et al., 2013). The whole process of scien-
“more systematic and interdisciplinary research on SRA” (p. 32). tific inquiry can be specified as a problem-solving task (Abd-El-
Consequently, they developed a generic framework of SRA with Khalik et al., 2004; Klahr, 2000; Mayer, 2007; Nowak et al., 2013)
scientific processes as eight epistemic activities. The crucial aim of and scientific reasoning is discussed as one crucial element of sci-
Fischer et al.’s (2014) framework is to synthesize different research entific inquiry (Hartmann et al., 2015; Mayer, 2007; Nowak et al.,
disciplines. Furthermore, Bricker and Bell (2008) discussed that 2013; Zimmerman, 2005). Due to their importance, these two
science education and the learning sciences could probably benefit subdimensions have been considered in normative education
from each other by considering “both science education and standards in different countries. In the United States, the National
learning sciences theories” (Bricker & Bell, 2008, p. 493). Therefore, Research Council (NRC) formulated standards for science education
Fischer et al. postulated eight epistemic activities as interdiscipli- (National Research Council [NRC], 1996, 2012). Based on guiding
narily valid scientific processes. By setting up this framework of principles, a framework with three dimensions (Dimension 1:
SRA, they asked researchers to identify (a) domain-specific and (b) Practices; Dimension 2: Crosscutting Concepts; Dimension 3:
domain-general aspects of SRA in future research. In the following, Disciplinary Core Ideas) (NRC, 2012, p. 29) was created. By intro-
these eight epistemic activities are briefly described: ducing these three dimensions, the NRC intended, among other
things, to ensure “that students will themselves engage in the
(1) Problem identification: Scientific processes often result from practices and not merely learn about them secondhand” (NRC,
a concrete problem. These problems could be practical, for 2012, p. 30), and “to prepare students with sufficient core knowl-
example, real-world problems. So, the beginning of SRA is a edge so that they can later acquire additional information on their
missing explanation of a particular phenomenon. own” (NRC, 2012, p. 31). Consequently, it is important, among other
(2) Questioning: Based on the problem, at least one answerable things, to develop “students' knowledge of how science and engi-
question is formulated. neering achieve their ends while also strengthening their compe-
(3) Hypothesis generation: As the upcoming process, a hypoth- tency with related practices” (NRC, 2012, p. 41), for example “asking
esis relating to the question is generated. questions and defining problems, developing and using models,
(4) Construction and redesign of artefacts: Physical artefacts are planning and carrying out investigations, …” (Alshamali & Daher,
constructed according to theoretical concepts. 2016, p. 1004; see also; NRC, 2012, p. 42). In Germany, within the
(5) Evidence generation: In this activity, evidence gets generated German NES for the subjects biology, chemistry, and physics, con-
in different ways. Fischer et al. (2014) described the tent knowledge and scientific inquiry are formulated as two sub-
hypothetico-deductive or inductive way as possibilities to dimensions (Kampa & Ko € ller, 2016). For each subject, content
generate evidence. knowledge and scientific inquiry are described, in addition to some
(6) Evidence evaluation: Evidence evaluation means the given examples, in the NES. In biology instruction, content
assessment of evidence with a claim or theory. Therefore, a knowledge contains knowing living things, biological phenomena,
critical appraisal of the generated evidence is needed. concepts, principles, facts and assigning them to the core concepts.
(7) Drawing conclusions: This activity means to draw at least Scientific inquiry includes observing, comparing, conducting experi-
one inference based on the previous reasoning process. ments, using models, and using scientific working methods (KMK,
(8) Communicating and scrutinizing: The reasoning process it- 2005a). In the context of research on scientific inquiry in Ger-
self and its results have to be shared and critically many, central skills within the process of scientific inquiry have
reconsidered. been defined. Based on the results of several studies (e.g.
Hammann, 2004; Klahr, 2000; Koslowski, 1996), Mayer (2007)
However, although this framework was developed to describe identified four central skills of scientific inquiry: formulating ques-
SRA in different research disciplines, Fischer et al. outlined the tions, generating hypotheses, planning investigations, and interpreting
T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243 235

data. Klos, Henke, Kieren, Walpuski, and Sumfleth (2008) differ- epistemic activities are already used in regular biology lessons? We
entiated between generating hypotheses, implementing experiments, expected that the eight epistemic activities had been used with a
and drawing conclusions. In general, scientific inquiry skills are varying frequency in biology instruction (Hypothesis 1) (based on
differently taken into account during science instruction. For Bao et al., 2009; Hammann, 2004; Nehring et al., 2016; Stiller,
example, asking questions and generating hypotheses are rarely 2016). (b) Is there an effect of the epistemic activities of SRA on stu-
used compared to implementing experiments (e.g. Bao et al., 2009; dents' achievement in biology instruction? We expected positive ef-
Hammann, 2004; Nehring, Stiller, Nowak, Upmeier zu Belzen, & fects of the eight epistemic activities on students' achievement
Tiemann, 2016; Stiller, 2016). For biology instruction, empirical (Hypothesis 2) (based on Fischer et al., 2014; Kampa & Ko € ller, 2016;
results showed that less time during lessons is spent on using sci- Klahr & Dunbar, 1988; Klahr et al., 2011; Kremer et al., 2013;
entific working methods (e.g. experiments) (Berck & Graf, 2010; Lederman et al., 2002; Roberts & Gott, 1999).
Füller, 1992; Gropengieber, 2013; Jatzwauk, 2007). For example,
Jatzwauk (2007, p. 133) analyzed 45 videotaped biology lessons 4. Methods
with the topic “blood and blood circulation” and found that only in
two of these 45 lessons were questions and hypotheses generated. The data and videos we used in this study were selected from
Experiments and scientific investigations were not planned at all, the cooperative German Federal Ministry of Education and
but at least in four of these 45 lessons, experiments were con- Research (BMBF)-funded1 project Professional Knowledge of Sci-
ducted. Additionally, recent studies on biology instruction showed ence Teachers (ProwiN) (Tepner et al., 2012). Besides the video-
that methodological and epistemological aspects during the con- taping of teachers' regular biology lessons in the project ProwiN,
duction of scientific working methods are rarely taken into account different data from the participating students were also collected
(Ehmer, 2008; Eschenhagen, Kattmann, & Rodi, 2006). before and after the videotaping.
Besides the German NES, there are also several other frame-
works in Germany for describing scientific and biology literacy. An 4.1. Design and sample
overview of the different frameworks was described by Kampa and
Ko€ller (2016). In compiling different frameworks, Kampa and Ko € ller In the biological part of the project ProwiN, two lessons of 9th
showed that the majority of these frameworks contain at least a grade biology instruction within the topic neurobiology of each of
content part and a process part. Many frameworks have additional the 43 participating teacher were videotaped (N ¼ 85 videos2). The
dimensions like communicative aspects or epistemological beliefs. randomly selected schools were located in the federal state of
Until now, there has still not been a consensus about these addi- Bavaria in Germany. The teachers participated voluntarily, and both
tional dimensions. As there is a missing agreement on the addi- videotaped lessons per teacher were about the teaching of the topic
tional dimensions, for example, communicative aspects, within the neurobiology. For the first lesson, the teachers were asked to teach a
compared frameworks, Kampa and Ko €ller only referred to the two lesson on the topic reflex arc. All participating teachers fulfilled the
consensual subdimensions content knowledge and scientific inquiry requirements to teach biology in German secondary schools
in their study. By measuring students' biology literacy, Kampa and (Gymnasium) (Fo €rtsch, Werner, von Kotzebue, & Neuhaus, 2016).
Ko€ller (2016) showed that these two subdimensions highly corre- Details about the teacher education in Germany were described in
lated in their study and concluded that “students could activate Cortina and Thames (2013). For the multilevel analyses, we used
both subdimensions while solving scientific problems” (p. 918). data from a subsample of 39 teachers (53% female; teaching
However, it could not be derived how to promote students' learning experience after traineeship in years: M ¼ 6.1, SD ¼ 5.7; age in
within both subdimensions, therefore, Kampa and Ko € ller suggested years: M ¼ 35.6, SD ¼ 8.3) and their 78 videos. We used this sub-
in their discussion that that students may benefit from transferring sample because we could not collect students' post-test achieve-
knowledge from one to the other subdimension. Furthermore, ment data of four teachers who were, therefore, not included in our
Nowak et al. (2013) confirmed the reasonable separation of these multilevel analyses. The student subsample consisted of 788 stu-
two subdimensions, and based on their measurement of students' dents (M ¼ 21.2 students per class; 49.7% female; age in years:
abilities in scientific inquiry, they concluded that it is important in M ¼ 14.3; SD ¼ .60).
designing learning environments to foster students' abilities within
this subdimension. 4.2. Students' data

3. Aims and hypotheses Students' achievement was assessed in a pre-post-test design


with a paper-and-pencil knowledge test (with 18 or 22 items) to
Three key findings of previous studies led to the intention of our measure the competencies content knowledge and scientific inquiry
study. First, students may benefit from transferring knowledge of the German NES integrated with the Bavarian curriculum on the
between the two subdimensions of scientific literacy: content topic neurobiology. The topic neurobiology is taught in about 18
knowledge and scientific inquiry (Kampa & Ko €ller, 2016). Second, it lessons as suggested by the Bavarian biology curriculum (Bavarian
was not yet fully clarified in those studies regarding how to foster State Ministry for Education and Culture [StMUK], 2004). Therefore,
students' learning within these two subdimensions (Kampa & multiple-choice items, as well as open partial credit items, were
Ko€ller, 2016; Nowak et al., 2013). Third, scientific reasoning is one used in the knowledge test. The items were constructed in accor-
key element of scientific inquiry (Hartmann et al., 2015; Mayer, dance with the competency content knowledge of the German NES.
2007; Nowak et al., 2013; Zimmerman, 2005). Based on these Content knowledge deals with students' “active engagement with
initial considerations, it seems worthwhile in our study to observe scientific content in order to solve specific scientific problems”
biology instruction in terms of the generic framework of SRA with (Pant et al., 2012, p. 4). To solve these items, students needed
eight epistemic activities (Fischer et al., 2014) for gathering more cognitive skills and conceptual understanding. An example item
information about fostering students' learning. Our main goals was: Explain how the human nervous system is adapted to humans'
were: (1) to show the current use of the eight epistemic activities in
regular biology instruction, and (2) to show the effects of SRA on
students' achievement in biology lessons. Therefore, we formulated 1
Grant No.: BMBF 01JH0904.
2
the following two research questions: (a) How often and which One teacher was sick on the day of the second videotaping.
236 T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243

way of living. Give two examples. Furthermore, items were designed identify if the theoretically developed descriptions illustrate ac-
to cover the competency scientific inquiry of the German NES and its tivities that could be observed in biology lessons and therefore
“sub-areas scientific investigation, scientific modelling, and scientific would be rated appropriately. After the piloting, the coding manual
theorizing” (Pant et al., 2012, p. 5). In an example item, students was revised and this new version was used for the coding. To verify
were shown a drawing of an experiment and they had to solve the the quality of the coding, two independent raters coded 10% of all
following task: Formulate a hypothesis which could be investigated 85 videotaped lessons and a Cohen's kappa value was found to be
with this experiment. The students completed the pre-test before .66 indicating a substantial inter-rater agreement (Landis & Koch,
the topic neurobiology was taught and then completed the post- 1977).
test one lesson after the teaching of the topic neurobiology was
completed. To ensure the objectivity of the measurement, two in-
dependent test markers scored 10% of each of the pre- and post-test
items (Fo €rtsch et al., 2016). The two-way random ICCs, ICC(abso- 4.4. Multilevel analyses
lute), showed a high agreement between the scores awarded by the
two markers, pre-test: ICC(absolute) ¼ .99, F(1277,1277) ¼ 77.92, Our data had a hierarchical structure. The epistemic activities of
p < .001, N ¼ 1278; post-test: ICC(absolute) ¼ .98, SRA were measured on the class level. Students' achievement in the
F(2477,2477) ¼ 56.50, p < .001, N ¼ 2478 (Fo €rtsch et al., 2016; pre- and post-test and their willingness to make an effort were
Werner, 2016; Wirtz & Caspar, 2002). The Partial Credit Model measured on the individual student level. Students' achievement in
(PCM) (Bond & Fox, 2007) was used for analyzing the results of the the post-test was used as an outcome variable on the student and
tests with the software Winsteps 3.81 (Linacre, 2015) and the item the class levels. We conducted the multilevel analysis with the
thresholds of the post-test were anchored to those of the pre-test program MPlus 7.3 (Muthe n & Muthe n, 1998e2012). Since not all
(Bond & Fox, 2007; Fo €rtsch et al., 2016). Good fit values of all the variables were distributed normally, we conducted the multi-
items were shown: all Outfit-MNSQ < 1.3. Also the reliabilities were level analysis using the weighted least squares means and variance-
satisfactory, person reliability (pre-test) ¼ .63; item reliability (pre- adjusted estimator. The results of the used estimator were quite
test) ¼ 1.0; person reliability (post-test) ¼ .78; item reliability (post- similar to the maximum likelihood estimator (Beauducel &
test) ¼ 1.0 (Bond & Fox, 2007; Boone, Staver, & Yale, 2014; Fo €rtsch Herzberg, 2006). To ensure the model fit, the comparative fit in-
et al., 2016; Werner, 2016). In addition, the students answered a dex (CFI), the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA),
questionnaire on motivational aspects (Wild, Gerber, Exeler, & and the standardized root-mean-square residuals for the between-
Remy, 2001). This questionnaire included items on their willing- class (SRMRbetween) and the within-class (SRMRwithin) covariance
ness to make an effort (3 items; a ¼ .72) (Fo €rtsch et al., 2016). This matrices were used. Hu and Bentler (1998) described a good model
motivational aspect means the readiness of students to deal inde- fit by the following values of measures: CFI above .90, RMSEA below
pendently with the biological content of the lessons. One example .05, and SRMR below .08. The calculated results of the multilevel
item was: When I will be examined in biology lessons (e.g. oral ex- analysis were shown as standardized values. Thus, one unit change
amination), I make a big effort to be successful. means a standard deviation change in the original values (z-stan-
dardized). Additionally, the epistemic activities of SRA were grand-
4.3. Coding of the eight epistemic activities mean centered. In our analysis, we modelled the data on both levels
(class and student levels) simultaneously. Concerning our second
All videotaped lessons (N ¼ 85 videos) were analyzed in terms of hypothesis, we had interest in the effects of the eight epistemic
the eight epistemic activities. For the coding of the eight epistemic activities of SRA on the class level. To construe the differences in
activities, we used a self-developed coding manual (see Table 1). students' achievement in the post-test on the eight epistemic ac-
With this manual, we coded whether or not each epistemic activity tivities of SRA (class-level variable), we used students' achievement
could be observed at least once per lesson (0 ¼ “not observed”, in the pre-test and their willingness to make an effort as control
1 ¼ “observed”). In this coding manual, each epistemic activity has variables on the student level. Both control variables were found to
a “description”, which illustrates how each epistemic activity could have an effect on achievement in Schmidt-Atzert's (2006) study.
be observed in biology lessons. This description was developed To evaluate the amount of between-class variance of students'
using the theoretical description of the eight epistemic activities achievement in the post-test, we first calculated the ICC of the null
and using a pilot sample of videos (N ¼ 10 videos) from the German model. In this calculation, no predictor was used for modelling
Research Foundation (DFG)-funded project Competence orientation students' achievement in the post-test on both the class and stu-
and Task Culture in Nature and Science Instruction (LerNT) with the dent levels. Afterwards, we conducted the multilevel analysis, in
€rtsch, Werner, Dorfner, von Kotzebue, & Neuhaus,
topic “botany” (Fo which the eight epistemic activities were found to have effects on
2017; von Kotzebue et al., 2015). This pilot sample was used to students' achievement in the post-test.

Table 1
Coding manual of the eight epistemic activities.

Epistemic activity Description

Problem identification A biological phenomenon is presented problem-orientedly or a key problem is posed.


Questioning The present problem is articulated or formulated (e.g. using questions, leading discussions).
Hypothesis generation A presumption, a hypothesis or a main research question is posed.
Construction and redesign of artefacts Biological methods (experiments, investigations, observations, heuristic procedures, comparison with known
phenomena) are planned.
Evidence generation Biological methods are performed or retraced.
Evidence evaluation The biological methods are interpreted.
Drawing conclusions General conclusions are drawn based on the biological methods.
Communicating and scrutinizing The biological phenomenon or the key problem from the beginning is picked up again after conducting the
biological methods. The presumptions are falsified or confirmed.
T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243 237

5. Results 5.2. Effects of the epistemic activities of SRA on students'


achievement
5.1. Current use of the different epistemic activities in regular
biology lessons Considering our second research question about the effects of
the eight epistemic activities of SRA on students' achievement, we
According to our first research question about how often the conducted multilevel analysis of the data. First, the ICC (null model)
epistemic activities had already been used in regular biology les- was found to be .192, that is, 19.2% of variance in students'
sons, we found in this study the number of different epistemic achievement in the post-test could be explained by between-class
activities in the lessons (N ¼ 85 videos) but in nine lessons, no differences. To test our second hypothesis, we calculated a two-
epistemic activity was used at all (see Fig. 1). In ten lessons, at least level path model (see Fig. 3). In this model, the eight epistemic
one epistemic activity was implemented. In 25 lessons, which activities of SRA predicted students' achievement in the post-test
means in around 30% of all the lessons, two epistemic activities on the class level. Students' achievement in the pre-test and their
were applied. In 15 lessons, three epistemic activities were used, willingness to make an effort were used as control variables. Ac-
whereas in five lessons, four epistemic activities were used. Five cording to Hu and Bentler (1998), the model fit indices showed
epistemic activities were applied in 14 lessons. Six epistemic ac- suitable values: c2 (3) ¼ 154.18, p < .001; CFI ¼ 1.000,
tivities were used in six lessons, and seven epistemic activities were RMSEA < .001; SRMR within < .001, SRMR between < .001.
used in only one lesson. None of all the lessons implemented all the On the class level, the results of the multilevel analysis showed a
eight epistemic activities. On average, 2.8 different epistemic ac- positive significant effect of the eight epistemic activities of SRA on
tivities (SD ¼ .93) were used in the lessons. students' achievement in the post-test, ß ¼ .30, SE ¼ .15, p < .05. In
Since we were also interested in which epistemic activities had other words, the use of epistemic activities in the lessons had
already used in regular biology lessons, we found in our study the positive effects on students' achievement. Furthermore, the eight
number of videotaped lessons which used each of the eight epistemic activities explained 9% of variance in students' achieve-
epistemic activities (see Fig. 2). The graph in Fig. 2 shows that some ment in the post-test on the class level, R2 ¼ .09 (see Fig. 3).
epistemic activities were more often used than others, for example, On the student level, the two control variables each showed a
problems were identified in 37 lessons, but only in 32 lessons did positive significant effect on students' achievement in the post-
the teachers ask a question related to the problems. Hypotheses test: ß ¼ .37, SE ¼ .03, p < .001 (students' achievement in the pre-
were even only generated in 16 lessons. A very conspicuous aspect test); ß ¼ .18, SE ¼ .03, p < .001 (willingness to make an effort) and
is that the epistemic activity construction and redesign of artefacts explained 18% of the variance in students' achievement in the post-
was almost never used in the lessons. Just in one lesson was this test on student level, R2 ¼ .18 (see Fig. 3).
epistemic activity conducted. Nonetheless, evidence was generated
in 56 lessons and evaluated in 47 lessons. This means that the 6. Discussion
teachers took less time in planning the evidence generation phase
with the students and instead they just did the evidence generation We had two goals in this study: to show the current use of
and evaluation. In 26 lessons, conclusions were drawn, whereas in epistemic activities and to show the effects of the epistemic ac-
23 lessons, epistemic activity communicating and scrutinizing was tivities on students' achievement in biology instruction. We first
used. Finally, we found examples for each epistemic activity from discuss our results according to our first goal to show the current
the videotaped lessons. These examples were intended to illustrate use of the eight epistemic activities in biology lessons. Then, we
practically the current use of the epistemic activities in biology discuss the results according to our second goal to show the effects
instruction (see Table 2). of the epistemic activities on students' achievement. Finally, we
address several limitations of our study.

6.1. Current use of the epistemic activities in regular biology lessons

We examined how often the epistemic activities were used in


videotaped biology lessons and showed that the epistemic activ-
ities were not used at all in some of our analyzed lessons (see Fig. 1).
In more than half of the lessons, three or less epistemic activities
were used. Not all the eight epistemic activities were observed in
any one single lesson. In view of the fact that “an understanding of
how scientists work is necessary to be scientifically literate”
(Nowak et al., 2013, p. 182), it seems necessary to give students an
understanding of the whole scientific process by using more
epistemic activities in one lesson. So, the most consistent practice
would be to use all epistemic activities during one lesson. It is
obvious that focusing extensively on specific epistemic activities in
some lessons could help students to understand the scientific
process that should be embedded in these activities. Here, we see
more need of using all or at least more epistemic activities in a
continuous way during biology instruction. By using only one single
epistemic activity, only fragments and parts of the scientific process
could be shown.
We also showed in this study which epistemic activities were
used in the lessons (see Fig. 2). Some epistemic activities could be
observed more often than others in the 85 videotaped lessons. It
Fig. 1. Number of different epistemic activities in the lessons (N ¼ 85 videos). was remarkable that we could obtain evidence and evaluated these
238 T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243

Fig. 2. Use of the eight epistemic activities of SRA in the lessons (N ¼ 85 videos).

Table 2
Examples from the video study ProwiN for each epistemic activity.

Epistemic activity Example (Important: Read each example separately, no coherent storyline)

Problem identification {Part of a movie is shown (“Dinner for One” or “The 90th Birthday”)}
Teacher: “You know this, it [stumbling over objects] increases in the course of this movie, that the waiter always has these problems with
the head of the tiger [an object on the carpet which he stumbles over]. You may also know such situations that you sometimes
underestimate the height of a step or the height of a curb or a cobblestone pavement. You stumble. Normally you do not fall.”
Questioning In relation to the problem presented above (see “Problem identification”)
Teacher: “Why do you not stumble? How does the body react?”
Hypothesis generation {Teacher and students talk about the patellar reflex}
Teacher: “Let's make a presumption, a hypothesis. What happened in your body […] when you hit on the knee area?”
S3: “When you hit this area, then a muscle become tense.”
Construction and redesign of Teacher: “So, suppose you are scientists and you want to find out what meaning the spinal cord has in human beings. How could one clarify
artefacts this? Student1.”
Student1: “So that would not be a good idea now, but you could take a spinal cord from a human being and then look, which functions this
person does not have, in contrast to a person who has the spinal cord.”
[…]
Teacher: “Yes, this may not be so easy now. The approach is not bad, modify a little bit of your approach.”
Student2: “So, it is possible to look at a human being who is somehow paralyzed and therefore, cannot trigger [the functions of] the spinal
cord. Then you can see what he cannot do.”
Teacher: “Okay. So, that is, one possibility, when it comes to the meanings of some organs or structures in the human body. One could look,
what happens when this organ is somehow injured. For example, if the spinal cord is injured this is what we name cross-paralysis. You
could actually do that.”
Evidence generation Teacher: “You should measure your reaction time. That means, how it is written here. {Points to a slide} Form teams of two or three. […]
One is the experimenter, who holds the ruler somewhere at the top. The other, the guinea pig, has index finger and thumb on the zero mark
[of the ruler]. But please do not hold the ruler, the fingers should be at a distance of one centimeter left and right of the ruler. […] The
experimenter releases the ruler at some point and the guinea pig should catch the ruler [with both fingers]. […] You must conduct this
experiment three times and record the three distances of the catches [from zero to the point of the catch on the ruler], calculate their mean
value and find the reaction time using the given table {Shows the table}.”
Evidence evaluation Teacher: “Think again of this experiment here {holding glasses with bellows}. Student closed his eyes. What is probably known about the
commonality between the lid-closing reflexa and the reduction of the pupils? What is the meaning and purpose of these reflex reactions?”
Drawing conclusions {Discussion about the patellar reflex, which was carried out before}
Teacher: “[…] Principally, this is a kind of “stumbling reflex” that prevents you from falling in a normal movement process, yes? If the leg
somehow does not remain in the normal movement process. This is a defensive reflex. This would be the so-called reflex arc. A reflex arc is,
so to say, a description of this interconnection. It always has similar components. What components does each reflex arc have?”
Communicating and Teacher: “This is the way a normal transmission of signals at the synapse works. Let's go back to the beginning of the lesson with the dance
scrutinizing or recreational drug “ecstasy.” Why is there the persistent state of intoxication?”
a
“Lidschlussreflex” in German, that is, “lid reflex” or “corneal reflex” in English biology textbooks.
T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243 239

Fig. 3. Multilevel analysis of the eight epistemic activities of SRA and students' achievement data.

epistemic activities in most lessons using this SRA framework, from the content and the process subdimensions. As Kampa and
without which few activities like problem identification, ques- Ko€ller argued, students may benefit from transferring knowledge
tioning or hypothesis generation could be identified. These results from one subdimension of scientific literacy to the other, that is,
were in line with those from other studies. For example, asking from content knowledge to scientific inquiry. So, our findings
questions or generating hypothesis is rarely taken into account provided further evidence for this relation. Furthermore, Kampa
during the teaching of the scientific inquiry process (Hammann, and Ko € ller (2016) concluded that using elements from one sub-
2004; Nehring et al., 2016; Sodian, Zaitchik, & Carey, 1991; Stiller, dimension might foster students' learning in both of these sub-
2016). Consequently, students could hardly understand the link dimensions. Before our study, the subdimension scientific inquiry
between a problem and its scientific investigation (Di Fuccia, 2007). of the German NES had been evaluated with different developed
There was a missing link between these epistemic activities competence models and investigations (cf. Kampa & Ko €ller, 2016;
(questioning and hypothesis generation)din which teachers intend Mayer, 2007; Nehring et al., 2016; Nowak et al., 2013; Wellnitz &
to show to their students why evidence has to be generateddand Mayer, 2013). For example, Nowak et al. (2013) pointed out that
the activities, in which evidence is generated and evaluated. There according to their “diagnosis of students' abilities, specific learning
was also a missing link between the epistemic activities construc- environments can be created to promote students' inquiry skills”
tion and redesign of artefactsdwhich is meant as an activity for (p. 187). Practical elaborations on how to foster students learning
planning to generate evidencedand evidence generation (Fischer within this subdimension are missing so far (Kampa & Ko € ller, 2016).
et al., 2014). The former activity, which was used only once in all Therefore, the eight epistemic activities of SRA we used in this
the observed lessons, was very conspicuous (see Fig. 2). This result study are one possible and feasible methodical tool to foster stu-
means that students were very marginally involved in developing dents' learning in both subdimensions.
scientific methods to investigate the scientific question and hy-
pothesis. Nehring et al. (2016) described similar results and they
observed that a lot of time was spent on planning and conducting 6.3. The application of epistemic activities in regular biology
experiments in videotaped chemistry lessons. Unfortunately, instruction
Nehring et al. did not distinguish between activities in planning and
conducting experiments. In our investigation, we could show that We derive further discussion points of our study by additionally
more activities were used in generating and evaluating evidence in using an application for practitioners to accentuate these aspects.
biology lessons than those in planning beforehand. In conclusion, Although we showed in our study that some epistemic activities of
our analysis of the observed biology lessons showed that students SRA had already often been used in biology instruction whereas
might not have an idea of how to plan the generation of evidence others were used rarely, for example, construction and redesign of
but they just conducted scientific methods. The last two activities artefacts, we also showed their positive effects on students'
drawing conclusions and communicating and scrutinizing were achievement. In view of this fact, it seems quite necessary to
observed in the videotaped lessons, but not so often. This result was demonstrate how all eight epistemic activities could be applied in
approximately in line with the results from Nehring et al. (2016) biology instruction. Furthermore, there are many instructional
who reported that these activities were used in their analyzed frameworks, but examples of applying these frameworks in regular
chemistry lessons, but not so often like the activity for generating instruction for practitioners rarely exist. Therefore, we decided to
evidence. briefly plan three biology lessons, which show exemplarily how all
eight epistemic activities of SRA could be considered in regular
biology instruction (see Table 3). The biological topics of the lessons
6.2. Effects of the epistemic activities on students' achievement and the grades are based on the curriculum of Bavaria, a federal
state of Germany. We describe the epistemic activities for the in-
Unless there were not enough epistemic activities used in a struction in each of the lessons which are planned for 45 min.
continuous way during the lessons, we showed their positive sig- Before the instructional activities are described, the needed prior
nificant effects on students' achievement in terms of the compe- knowledge for the students is also explained. In general, students
tencies content knowledge and scientific inquiry (see Fig. 3). We also need certain prior knowledge to understand the biological content
found that students achieved higher when they used more of these of a lesson, and to identify specific outlined problems. Additionally,
eight epistemic activities of SRA during instruction. As a conse- there is a relationship between prior knowledge and further steps
quence, our results could be interpreted in the way Kampa and of scientific investigation (Zimmerman, 2005). The first lesson
Ko€ller (2016) discussed their results which showed that during considers the topic “human respiration” in grade 5, the second
the problem-solving process, students could activate elements lesson deals with the topic “germination of plant seeds” in grade 6
240 T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243

Table 3
Planned biology lessons in three different grades referring to the Bavarian curriculum.

Human respiration (5th grade) Germination of plant seeds (6th grade) Reflexes (9th grade)

Students' prior knowledge


Students know the respiratory organs of the Students know the structures of plant seeds and Students know reflexes (definition of reflex arc
human body. can describe their respective function. In addition, and two examples: knee-jerk reflex, eyelid
they can establish a link between the energy reflex).
sources of the seeds and the nutrition of animals.
Epistemic Instructional activity
activity
Problem Students inhale and exhale normally. During Students find various plant seeds in the dry food of Picture collage is shown to the students (e.g. car
identification breathing, they measure their own chest a hamster. These seeds do not germinate. driver steps on brake, goalkeeper catches ball, …)
abdominal girth with a measuring tape. Students should justify if one of these shown
Their girth increases by inhaling and decreases by actions is a reflex. Students conclude that all of
exhaling. these actions are fast reactions.
Questioning Why does the abdominal girth increase, even Why do the plant seeds not germinate in the food How do a reflex and an arbitrary reaction differ in
though the lungs are located in the chest cavity? of the hamster? How could these seeds their speed? How could the different speeds be
germinate? explained?
Hypothesis Inhalation and exhalation rely on movements of Various external factors (e.g. water, temperature Reflexes are probably faster than arbitrary
generation the midriff. …) are necessary for the germination of plant reactions because the stimulus-response neural
seeds. pathway of reflexes is possibly shorter.
Construction For comparison with the natural structures, the The students plan a control experiment (with the Two reflexes should be investigated: (1) patellar
and redesign lung model (Donders' model) is made available to variable for testing, e.g. water) and an experiment tendon reflex, and (2) eyelid reflex.
of artefacts students. (lacking in variable for testing, e.g. water)*. Students plan comparative experiments (reflex
For assistance, students get the hint that only the vs. arbitrary reaction), and additionally, students
variable for testing has to be manipulated in their search a wide range of models for suitable ones to
experiments. test the hypothesis.
*Mangelexperimente in German
Evidence Students observe what happens if they pull the Each student implements the planned control Students conduct possible comparative
generation “midriff” of the model and then carefully release it. experiment and the planned experiment to experiments and use suitable models (see
investigate a factor (e.g. water) necessary for seed Appendix A for possible experiments/suitable
germination. models)
Evidence Use of a model for prediction of the results of Empirical data generation for predicting the Empirical data generation for predicting the
evaluation testing: By contracting the midriff, the volume of results of testing: The plant seeds do not results of testing: patellar tendon reflex and
the lungs is increased and air is sucked in. By germinate if one or more of the three factors eyelid reflex proceed faster than comparable
dilating the midriff, the volume of the lungs is (water, heat, and oxygen) is/are missing. arbitrary reactions.
reduced and air flows outwards. Use of models for predicting the results of
testing:
The stimulus-response neural pathway of a reflex
is shorter at the designated locations.
Drawing The movements of the midriff affect a substantial For germination of plant seeds, water, heat, and The speed of reflexes is faster because of a shorter
conclusions change in the volume of the lungs, whereby oxygen are necessary. stimulus-response neural pathway. The
therefore, the abdominal girth changes at the interconnection is simpler and short links of the
same time. neural pathway exist.
Communicating The results of the model use, and their conclusions The experimental results and their conclusions are The results of the experiment/model use and
and are noted. Problems and limitations of the model noted. Problems and limitations in the their conclusions are noted. Problems and
scrutinizing are discussed. experimental setup are discussed. limitations in the experimental setup/model are
discussed.

and the third lesson is about the topic “reflexes” for grade 9. For mentioned above, we see much potential in using more epistemic
example, these lessons could be used by teachers for a better un- activities to make students more aware of the whole scientific
derstanding of how all the eight epistemic activities could be process. So, it definitely should be clarified how many and which
considered in their instruction. Important to mention is that these epistemic activities are needed in one single lesson to appreciable
suggested lessons should not imply to explicitly use these eight convey SRA to students in biology instruction. But maybe it is not
epistemic activities as a structure for instruction and ask students just the question how many and which epistemic activities are
to apply these epistemic activities step-by-step for several biolog- needed per lesson, but rather how is the interconnection between
ical contexts, like a recipe in a cookbook. Since Cavagnetto (2010) several epistemic activities in one biology lesson. According to an
provided strong evidence against the claim of explicit teaching orientation of immersion this approach could show students the
approaches, which are not as strong as often illustrated. Therefore, important relationship between their investigations and their
our described instructional practices in class are meant in a way to explanations.
provide enough and necessary opportunities for students to ask
questions about illustrated scientific phenomena and to guide them
6.4. Limitations
with scaffolds such as prompts through the epistemic activities of
SRA. Such prompts could be questions like: “What is the illustrated
There are some limitations of this study. First, the current use
problem?” “What is the main research question?” “How does an
and the effects of the epistemic activities have to be evaluated in
experiment look like to test your hypothesis?” and “What are the
other grades, in lessons with other biological topics and in further
main conclusions you draw from your generated evidence?” Hence,
school types, for example, in primary school. Second, our results
students should clearly understand that the developed scientific
should be reinforced by further studies, especially the effects of
explanations are related to their self-developed investigating ac-
epistemic activities has to be re-evaluated according to other stu-
tivities. Considering the result of the review from Cavagnetto
dents' outcome variables, e.g. students' interest as dependent var-
(2010) also leads us to a further discussion point of our study. As
iable. This may lead to draw more general conclusions on effects
T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243 241

using the epistemic activities in class. Further, in our study stu- to be considered in curricula through scheduling enough time pe-
dents' achievement was measured using a paper-and-pencil riods for biology instruction, which allows conducting epistemic
knowledge test. In general, paper-pencil knowledge tests measure activities during regular instruction. Since of course, some time is
the knowledge of students, but not necessarily how effectively they needed to usefully consider these eight epistemic activities during
use their knowledge in practice. So, an alternative way to measure regular biology instruction. Overall, our study reinforces further call
students' knowledge would be to develop a knowledge test in to provide students with opportunities in biology classes to use
which students' knowledge is measured by solving a specific sci- epistemic activities of SRA.
entific problem in a practical way. Such a test could, inter alia, raise
students' dexterity, their accuracy, and their logic of applying sci- Funding statement
entific investigations or their knowledge. Such a practical knowl-
edge test would definitely complement our results to make further This research was supported by the German Federal Ministry of
statements according to the effects of the eight epistemic activities Education and Research (Bundesministerium für Bildung und For-
of SRA in biology instruction. Third, our analyzed videotaped les- schung e BMBF) [Grant number 01JH0904].
sons were not standardized between the different teachers. Only
the taught topic and the grade were the same. Therefore, it is Appendix A
possible that students' achievement may also be influenced by
other variables, for example, instructional quality features like Possible comparative experiments:
classroom management. This also showed that we could not
explain the whole variance in students' achievement with the (1) acoustic "knock tests"
epistemic activities. Fourth, an open question, which we could not
yet answer, is the question about qualitative aspects of the A student “knocks” his/her leg against the edge of a table. The
epistemic activities. We just coded whether or not each epistemic time of stimulus (“hitting” with a reflex hammer on the knee
activity could be observed at least once per lesson. This means, we tendon or striking with a reflex hammer on the thigh) to the re-
could not judge how good the epistemic activities were qualita- action (the leg kicks forward) is measured (with the aid of simple
tively addressed during the videotaped lessons. For example, how audio recording).
strong were the generated hypotheses or how clear were the evi-
dences generated? Here, further and more research is definitively (2) eyelid reflex
needed.
Nevertheless, the epistemic activities played an important role Investigation in groups of two students:
in affecting students' achievement in our study. However, we need
more research according to the epistemic activities with specific (a) One student blows another, who has opened his eyes, with a
settings. One possibility could be an experimental setup, in which rubber tube about 10 cm away from one of his eyes (reflex
the instruction is standardized and the use of different epistemic glasses)
activities is varied systematically. / Observation?
(b) A short text is read to a student, who always closes an eyelid
7. Conclusions and implications at the word "and".
/ Observation?
In conclusion, we have showed and discussed in this article the
current use of epistemic activities of SRA in biology instruction we Possible models:
found in our study and their positive effects on students' achieve- Images of the human nervous system and stimulus-response
ment. Additionally, we discussed the meaningful application of the neural pathway (receptor, afferent nervous system, CNS, efferent
eight epistemic activities in regular biology instruction. According nervous system, reaction).
to our findings, the framework of SRA could be stated as one There are speed delays which are unavoidable due to anatomical
effective possibility to improve students' achievement in biology. conditions. Are there anatomical possibilities to make a reflex faster
Finally, we derive several implications of our study. First for than an arbitrary reaction?
educational research, our study demonstrated that a psychological
framework, the generic framework of SRA by Fischer et al. (2014), a) short neural pathways (e.g. knee-length reflex: interconnection
which was developed by considering results from psychological in the spinal cord, eyelid reflex: interconnection in the brain or
research, is suitable to describe the use of scientific processes in brain stem)
biology lessons. This means an interdisciplinary exchange between b) as few synapses as possible
different research disciplines about ideas and further collaborations
should be fostered, which is also required by different authors (cf.
Bricker & Bell, 2008; Fischer et al., 2014). In consequence, we as-
Appendix B. Supplementary data
sume that psychological research disciplines may also benefit from
considering results from biology education. Second, creating op-
Supplementary data related to this article can be found at
portunities for students to get aware of interrelated epistemic ac-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2018.07.003.
tivities during class is a major task for practitioners, but especially
teacher educators are further required to develop assistances for
practitioners, which show how epistemic activities could be References
imparted during biology instruction and how single epistemic ac-
Abd-El-Khalick, F., BouJaoude, S., Duschl, R., Lederman, N. G., Mamlok-Naaman, R.,
tivities could be interrelated in the classroom discourse, e.g. using Hofstein, A., et al. (2004). Inquiry in science education: International perspec-
appropriate questions. Finally, of course, several frame conditions tives. Science Education, 88(3), 397e419. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.10118.
must be given to implement epistemic activities in regular biology Alshamali, M. A., & Daher, W. M. (2016). Scientific reasoning and its relationship
with problem solving: The case of upper primary science teachers. International
lessons. Although, educational standards, e.g. the German NES, Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 14(6), 1003e1019. https://doi.org/
require fostering scientific inquiry in biology, this requirement has 10.1007/s10763-015-9646-1.
242 T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243

Bao, L., Cai, T., Koenig, K., Fang, K., Han, J., Wang, J., et al. (2009). Physics. Learning U. Harms, & U. Kattmann (Eds.), Fachdidaktik biologie. Die biologiedidaktik (pp.
and scientific reasoning. Science, 323(5914), 586e587. https://doi.org/10.1126/ 284e293). Hallbergmoos: Aulis Verlag.
science.1167740. Hammann, M. (2004). Kompetenzentwicklungsmodelle. Merkmale und ihre
Baumert, J., Bos, W., & Lehmann, R. (2000). TIMSS/III. Dritte Internationale Mathe- Bedeutung - dargestellt anhand von Kompetenzen beim Experimentieren
matik- und Naturwissenschaftsstudie. Mathematische und naturwissenschaftliche [[Competence development models. Characteristics and their meaning - rep-
Bildung am Ende der Schullaufbahn [[TIMSS/III. Third international mathematics resented by competencies in experimentation]]. Mathematische und Natur-
and science study. Mathematical and science education at the end of school]]. wissenschaftliche Unterricht, 57(4), 196e203.
Opladen: Leske þ Budrich. Hartmann, S., Upmeier zu Belzen, A., Krüger, D., & Pant, H. A. (2015). Scientific
Baumert, J., Klieme, E., Neubrand, M., Prenzel, M., Schiefele, U., Schneider, W., et al. reasoning in higher education. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 223(1), 47e53. https://
(2001). PISA 2000: Basiskompetenzen von Schülerinnen und Schülern im inter- doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000199.
nationalen Vergleich [[PISA 2000: Basic competencies of students in international Herrenkohl, L. R., & Cornelius, L. (2013). Investigating elementary students' scien-
comparison]]. Opladen: Leske þ Budrich. tific and historical argumentation. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 22(3),
Bavarian State Ministry for Education and Culture [StMUK] (Ed.). (2004). Lehrplan 413e461. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508406.2013.799475.
für das Gymnasium in Bayern [Curriculum for the Bavarian Gymnasium]. Woln- Hu, L.-t., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling:
zach: Kastner. Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological
Beauducel, A., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2006). On the performance of maximum likelihood Methods, 3(4), 424e453. https://doi.org/10.1037//1082-989X.3.4.424.
versus means and variance adjusted weighted least squares estimation in CFA. Jatzwauk, P. (2007). Aufgaben im biologieunterricht: Eine analyse der merkmale und
Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 13(2), 186e203. des didaktisch-methodischen einsatzes von aufgaben im biologieunterricht [[Tasks
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1302_2. in biology lessons: An analysis of characteristics and the use of didactic method-
Berck, K.-H., & Graf, D. (2010). Biologiedidaktik: Grundlagen und Methoden [[Biology ological tasks in biology lessons]]. Berlin: Logos.
education: Basics and methods]]. Wiebelsheim: Quelle und Meyer. Kampa, N., & Ko € ller, O. (2016). German national proficiency scales in biology: In-
Bond, T. G., & Fox, C. M. (2007). Applying the Rasch model: Fundamental measurement ternal structure, relations to general cognitive abilities and verbal skills. Science
in the human sciences. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Education, 100(5), 903e922. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.21227.
Boone, W. J., Staver, J. R., & Yale, M. S. (2014). Rasch analysis in the human sciences. Klahr, D. (2000). Exploring science: The cognition and development of discovery pro-
Dordrecht: Springer. cesses. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bricker, L. A., & Bell, P. (2008). Conceptualizations of argumentation from science Klahr, D., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Dual space search during scientific reasoning.
studies and the learning sciences and their implications for the practices of Cognitive Science, 12(1), 1e48. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1201_1.
science education. Science Education, 92(3), 473e498. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Klahr, D., Zimmerman, C., & Jirout, J. (2011). Educational interventions to advance
sce.20278. children's scientific thinking. Science, 333(6045), 971e975. https://doi.org/
Bricker, L. A., & Bell, P. (2012). Argumentation and reasoning in life and in school: 10.1126/science.1204528.
Implications for the design of school science learning environments. In Klos, S., Henke, C., Kieren, C., Walpuski, M., & Sumfleth, E. (2008). Natur-
M. S. Khine (Ed.), Perspectives on scientific argumentation. Theory, practice and wissenschaftliches Experimentieren und chemisches Fachwissen - zwei ver-
research (pp. 117e133). Dordrecht: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94- schiedene Kompetenzen [[Science experimentation and chemical expertise -
007-2470-9_7. two different competencies]]. Zeitschrift für Padagogik, 54(3), 304e321.
Bybee, R. W. (2002). Scientific literacy - mythos oder realit€ at? [[Scientific literacy - Koslowski, B. (1996). Theory and evidence: The development of scientific reasoning.
myth or reality?]] In W. Gr€ aber, P. Nentwig, T. R. Koballa, & R. H. Evans (Eds.), Learning, development, and conceptual change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Scientific Literacy. Der Beitrag der Naturwissenschaften zur Allgemeinen Bildung von Kotzebue, L., Fo €rtsch, C., Reinold, P., Werner, S., Sczudlek, M., & Neuhaus, B. J.
[Scientific literacy. The contribution of science to education] (pp. 21e43). Wies- (2015). Quantitative Videostudien zum gymnasialen Biologieunterricht in
baden: Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften. Deutschland e aktuelle Tendenzen und Entwicklungen [[Quantitative video
Cavagnetto, A. R. (2010). Argument to foster scientific literacy: A review of argu- studies on German biology instruction in the Gymnasium e actual trends and
ment interventions in K-12 science contexts. Review of Educational Research, 80, developments]]. Zeitschrift für Didaktik der Naturwissenschaften, 21(1), 231e237.
336e371. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654310376953. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40573-015-0033-9.
Chinn, C., & Clark, D. B. (2013). Learning through collaborative argumentation. In Kremer, K., Specht, C., Urhahne, D., & Mayer, J. (2013). The relationship in biology
C. E. Hmelo-Silver, C. A. Chinn, C. Chan, & A. M. O'Donnell (Eds.), The interna- between the nature of science and scientific inquiry. Journal of Biological Edu-
tional handbook of collaborative learning (pp. 314e332). Hoboken: Taylor and cation, 48(1), 1e8. https://doi.org/10.1080/00219266.2013.788541.
Francis. Kuhn, D. (1991). The skills of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cortina, K. S., & Thames, M. H. (2013). Teacher education in Germany. In M. Kunter, Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for
J. Baumert, W. Blum, U. Klusmann, S. Krauss, & M. Neubrand (Eds.), Cognitive categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159e174. https://doi.org/10.2307/2529310.
activation in the mathematics classroom and professional competence of teachers. Lawson, A. E. (2005). What is the role of induction and deduction in reasoning and
Results from the COACTIV project (pp. 49e62). New York: Springer. scientific inquiry? Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 42(6), 716e740.
Di Fuccia, D.-S. (2007). Schülerexperimente als Instrument der Leistungsbeurteilung https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.20067.
[[Students' experiments as instrument for performance assessment]]. Berlin: Uni- Lederman, N. G., Abd-El-Khalick, F., Bell, R. L., & Schwartz, R. S. (2002). Views of
Ed. nature of science questionnaire: Toward valid and meaningful assessment of
Ehmer, M. (2008). Fo €rderung von kognitiven Fa €higkeiten beim Experimentieren im learners' conceptions of nature of science. Journal of Research in Science
Biologieunterricht der 6. Klasse: Eine Untersuchung zur Wirksamkeit von meth- Teaching, 39(6), 497e521. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.10034.
odischem, epistemologischem und negativem Wissen [[Promoting cognitive abili- Lederman, N. G., & Lederman, J. S. (2012). Nature of scientific knowledge and sci-
ties during experimentation in biology instruction in grade 6: An investigation entific inquiry: Building instructional capacity through professional develop-
about the effectiveness of methodological, epistemological and negative knowl- ment. In B. J. Fraser (Ed.), Second international handbook of science education (pp.
edge]]. Doctoral thesis. Germany: Christian-Albrechts-Universita €t zu Kiel. 335e359). Berlin: Springer.
Eschenhagen, D., Kattmann, U., & Rodi, D. (Eds.). (2006). Fachdidaktik biologie Linacre, J. M. (2015). A user's guide to Winsteps/Ministep: Rasch-model computer
[biology education]. Ko €ln: Aulis. programs. Retrieved from http://www.winsteps.com/manuals.htm. (Accessed
Fischer, F., Kollar, I., Ufer, S., Sodian, B., Hussmann, H., Pekrun, R., et al. (2014). 29 June 2017).
Scientific reasoning and argumentation: Advancing an interdisciplinary Mayer, J. (2007). Erkenntnisgewinnung als wissenschaftliches Problemlo €sen [[Sci-
research agenda in education. Frontline Learning Research, 5, 28e45. https:// entific inquiry as scientific problem solving]]. In D. Krüger, & H. Vogt (Eds.),
doi.org/10.14786/flr.v2i3.96. Theorien in der biologiedidaktischen Forschung. Ein Handbuch für Lehramts-
€rtsch, C., Werner, S., Dorfner, T., von Kotzebue, L., & Neuhaus, B. J. (2017). Effects of
Fo studenten und Doktoranden [Theories in biology education research. A handbook
cognitive activation in biology lessons on students' situational interest and for pre-service teachers and doctoral students] (pp. 177e186). Berlin: Springer.
achievement. Research in Science Education, 47(3), 559e578. https://doi.org/ Muthe n, L. K., & Muthe n, B. O. (1998-2012). Mplus user's guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles:
10.1007/s11165-016-9517-y. Muthe n & Muthe n.
€rtsch, C., Werner, S., von Kotzebue, L., & Neuhaus, B. J. (2016). Effects of biology
Fo National Research Council [NRC]. (1996). The national science education standards.
teachers' professional knowledge and cognitive activation on students' Washington: National Academies Press.
achievement. International Journal of Science Education, 38, 2642e2666. https:// National Research Council [NRC]. (2012). A framework for K-12 science education:
doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2016.1257170. Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. Washington: National Academies
Füller, F. (1992). Biologische Unterrichtsexperimente: Bedeutung und Effektivita €t: Press.
[Biological experiments: Importance and effectiveness] (Vol. 8). München: Nehring, A., Stiller, J., Nowak, K. H., Upmeier zu Belzen, A., & Tiemann, R. (2016).
Münchner Schriften zur Didaktik der Biologie. Naturwissenschaftliche Denk- und Arbeitsweisen im Chemieunterricht e eine
Furtak, E. M., Hardy, I., Beinbrech, C., Shavelson, R. J., & Shemwell, J. T. (2010). modellbasierte Videostudie zu Lerngelegenheiten für den Kompetenzbereich
A framework for analyzing evidence-based reasoning in science classroom der Erkenntnisgewinnung [[Inquiry methods and scientific reasoning in
discourse. Educational Assessment, 15(3e4), 175e196. https://doi.org/10.1080/ chemistry education e a model-based video study on learning opportunities in
10627197.2010.530553. the field of scientific inquiry]]. Zeitschrift für Didaktik der Naturwissenschaften,
Gormally, C., Brickman, P., & Lutz, M. (2012). Developing a test of scientific literacy 22(1), 77e96. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40573-016-0043-2.
skills (TOSLS): Measuring undergraduates' evaluation of scientific information Nowak, K. H., Nehring, A., Tiemann, R., & Upmeier zu Belzen, A. (2013). Assessing
and arguments. CBE-Life Sciences Education, 11(4), 364e377. https://doi.org/ students' abilities in processes of scientific inquiry in biology using a paper-
10.1187/cbe.12-03-0026. and-pencil test. Journal of Biological Education, 47(3), 182e188. https://doi.org/
Gropengießer, H. (2013). Experimentieren [[Experimentation]]. In H. Gropengießer, 10.1080/00219266.2013.822747.
T. Dorfner et al. / Teaching and Teacher Education 75 (2018) 232e243 243

Osborne, J., Erduran, S., & Simon, S. (2004). Enhancing the quality of argumentation Schulabschluss [Educational standards in biology for the intermediate school-
in school science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41(10), 994e1020. leaving qualification]. München: Wolters Kluwer.
https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.20035. The Standing Conference of the Ministers of Education and Cultural Affairs of The
€hlmann, C. (2012).
Pant, H. A., Stanat, P., Schroeders, U., Roppelt, A., Siegle, T., & Po L€ander in the Federal Republic of Germany [Kultusministerkonferenz or KMK]
The IQB national assessment study. Competencies in mathematics and the sciences (Ed.). (2005b). Bildungsstandards im Fach Chemie für den Mittleren Schulabschluss
at the end of secondary level I. Summary. Münster: Waxmann. [Educational standards in chemistry for the intermediate school-leaving qualifi-
Roberts, R., & Gott, R. (1999). Procedural understanding: Its place in the biology cation]. München: Wolters Kluwer.
curriculum. School Science Review, 81(294), 19e25. The Standing Conference of the Ministers of Education and Cultural Affairs of The
Sampson, V., & Clark, D. B. (2008). Assessment of the ways students generate ar- L€ander in the Federal Republic of Germany [Kultusministerkonferenz or KMK]
guments in science education: Current perspectives and recommendations for (Ed.). (2005c). Bildungsstandards im Fach Physik für den Mittleren Schulabschluss
future directions. Science Education, 92(3), 447e472. https://doi.org/10.1002/ [Educational standards in physics for the intermediate school-leaving qualifica-
sce.20276. tion]. München: Wolters Kluwer.
Schmidt-Atzert, L. (2006). Leistungsrelevante rahmenbedingungen/leistungsmoti- Wason, P. C., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972). Psychology of reasoning: Structure and
vation. Einleitung [[Achievement-related framework conditions/achievement content. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
motivation. Introduction]]. In K. Schweizer (Ed.), Leistung und Leis- Wellnitz, N., & Mayer, J. (2013). Erkenntnismethoden in der Biologie - entwicklung
tungsdiagnostik [Achievement and achievement diagnostics] (pp. 224e241). Ber- und Evaluation eines Kompetenzmodells [[Scientific methods in biology e
lin: Springer. development and evaluation of a competence model]]. Zeitschrift für Didaktik
Shavelson, R. J., Young, D. B., Ayala, C. C., Brandon, P. R., Furtak, E. M., Ruiz- der Naturwissenschaften, 19, 315e345.
Primo, M. A., et al. (2008). On the impact of curriculum-embedded formative Werner, S. (2016). Zusammenha €nge zwischen dem fachspezifischen Professionswissen
assessment on learning: A collaboration between curriculum and assessment einer Lehrkraft, dessen Unterrichtsgestaltung und Schülervariablen am Beispiel
developers. Applied Measurement in Education, 21(4), 295e314. https://doi.org/ eines elaborierten Modelleinsatzes [[Teachers' domain-specific professional
10.1080/08957340802347647. knowledge, instructional quality using the example of elaborated model use, and
Sodian, B., Zaitchik, D., & Carey, S. (1991). Young children's differentiation of hy- students' variables]]. Doctoral thesis. Germany: Ludwig-Maximilians-Universit€ at
pothetical beliefs from evidence. Child Development, 62(4), 753e766. https:// München.
doi.org/10.2307/1131175. Wild, E., Gerber, J., Exeler, J., & Remy, K. (2001). Dokumentation der Skalen- und Item-
Stiller, J. (2016). Scientific Inquiry im Chemieunterricht - eine Videoanalyse zur Auswahl für den Kinderfragebogen zur Lernmotivation und zum emotionalen
Umsetzung von Erkenntnisgewinnungsprozessen im internationalen und schul- Erleben [[Documentation of the scales and items of the questionnaire on motiva-
stufenübergreifenden Vergleich [[Scientific inquiry in chemistry teaching - A video tion and emotional experience]]. Bielefeld: Universita €t Bielefeld.
analysis for the implementation of scientific inquiry processes in international and Wirtz, M., & Caspar, F. (2002). Beurteilerübereinstimmung und Beurteilerreliabilita €t
interschool comparison]]. Doctoral thesis. Germany: Humboldt-Universita €t zu [[Interrateragreement and interraterreliability]]. Go € ttingen: Hogrefe.
Berlin. Zimmerman, C. (2000). The development of scientific reasoning skills. Develop-
Tepner, O., Borowski, A., Dollny, S., Fischer, H. E., Jüttner, M., Kirschner, S., et al. mental Review, 20(1), 99e149. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1999.0497.
(2012). Modell zur Entwicklung von Testitems zur Erfassung des Pro- Zimmerman, C. (2005). The development of scientific reasoning skills: What psy-
fessionswissens von Lehrkr€ aften in den Naturwissenschaften [[Model for the chologists contribute to an understanding of elementary science learning. In
development of test items measuring science teachers' professional knowl- Final draft of the national research council committee on science learning
edge]]. Zeitschrift für Didaktik der Naturwissenschaften, 18, 7e28. kindergarten through eighth grade. Retrieved on 4th July, 2018 from: https://sites.
The Standing Conference of the Ministers of Education and Cultural Affairs of The nationalacademies.org/cs/groups/dbassesite/documents/webpage/dbasse_
La€nder in the Federal Republic of Germany [Kultusministerkonferenz or KMK] 080105.pdf.
(Ed.). (2005a). Bildungsstandards im Fach Biologie für den Mittleren

You might also like