You are on page 1of 9

DRAMA

Drama: narrative pedagogy and socially


challenged children
Melanie Peter

ties. Drama can seem to be a challenging way of teaching,


Melanie Peter, Senior Lecturer in Education and especially with idiosyncratic, unpredictable and seemingly
Early Childhood at Anglia Ruskin University, argues remote children. It can seem beyond their capabilities too, as
that the ability to engage in narrative stems from it all hinges on children’s developing ability to use narrative
early interactive play formats and is essential for form. However, rather than wait for an elusive ‘magic point’
perceiving patterns and sequences in life – the
at which children are deemed ‘ready’ to engage, drama can
fabric of social understanding. Developing themes
provide the actual contexts in which narrative ability can be
set out in her earlier work, this article presents a
rationale and developmental model for drama as learned. My professional experiences over many years indi-
narrative pedagogy. Whether narrative impairment cate that even the most hard-to-reach children may gain
is a cause or consequence of their difficulties, hard- significant benefits from drama. Latterly, my research with
to-reach or socially challenged children – exempli- profoundly socially challenged children on the autistic spec-
fied by those with autism – characteristically show a trum has illuminated what makes drama unique – and, since
limited spontaneous play repertoire, which neces- it has been successful for these children, analysis of the
sarily hampers their development. However, generative conditions that underpin this success may inform
through a play-tutoring approach in drama, espe- drama as pedagogy for everyone. This may provide a ratio-
cially by the teacher-in-role, significant gains may nale for using drama as a learning medium: to achieve a
be noted. Melanie Peter uses case studies to illus-
more integrated, coherent approach to planning and teaching
trate an emerging triad of competencies that con-
across the curriculum for all.
tribute to enhancing social understanding: more
effective communication, greater sensitivity in
interaction and creative problem-solving. The gen- Narrative development and play tutoring
erative pedagogical dimensions support a develop- The term ‘narrative’ is commonly associated with story: an
mental framework for using drama to explore account of a series of events causally and temporally related,
cultural values and issues embedded in stories, and infused with social meanings. For very young children,
towards greater narrative identity and more effec- cultural stories may be their earliest experiences of morality
tive participation in a social world.
and cultural values; for example, good/bad, frightening/safe
(Gilligan, 1982; Whitehead, 1997). Narrative in the social
Key words: drama, play, pedagogy, autism, case domain is a way of engaging with and evaluating cultural
studies. meanings. Grove (2006) explains how in personal narratives
shared in conversation, we go beyond simply recounting a
familiar narrative routine (for example, going to the shops):
Introduction we highlight perhaps a significant moment in an anecdote
Kierkegaard’s claim that ‘life is lived forwards but under- and invite our audience to share a perspective on this, based
stood backwards’ provides the essential premise on which on our own value system (‘You’ll never guess what hap-
this article is based. Reflecting on experience and learning pened to me today on the way to the shops! . . . What do you
about life ‘one step removed’ is a characteristic of drama- think of that?’). Narrative ability is thus instrumental for the
in-education work, first popularised in the mid-1970s by development of social relationships and for acquiring a
Dorothy Heathcote and Gavin Bolton. Rather than being sense of identity through positioning oneself within shared
associated with performance, drama became known as a experiences and events (Lawler, 2002).
classroom methodology – a learning medium through which
to explore social and moral issues and engage in investiga- Ability to engage in narrative requires sensitivity to pattern-
tive problem-solving through spontaneous role-taking with a ing, sequencing and the ‘framing’ of experience (Bruner &
‘living through’ quality. There is currently a revival of inter- Feldman, 1993). Across cultures, this is learned in early
est in drama-in-education in mainstream education for its interactive play formats, such as peek-a-boo games, that
analytical potential and for the purpose of promoting mean- have a predictable structure and provide opportunities for
ingful, integrated learning contexts across the curriculum. establishing joint meanings and reciprocal patterning
through turn-taking. Bruner and Feldman (1993) contend
However, drama as a learning medium is still under-used in that children with autism fail to engage in this ‘cultural
specialist settings and with children with learning difficul- framing’ in infancy, and then fail to use this template as the

© 2009 The Author(s). Journal compilation © 2009 NASEN. Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford, OX4 2DQ,
UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA, 02148, USA.
basis for recognising patterns and sequences, which consti- sisters. In role play, children collaborate on two levels, the
tutes the fabric of social understanding. This impaired real and the fictional, as they explore social meanings such
narrative ability would help to explain their difficulties as different social roles and perspectives; how responses
in anticipating routines, coping with changes, offering may be construed; possible consequences in terms of social
comment and chatting, and in understanding intentions and behaviour; how to manage themselves and their emotions in
consequences. It is a moot point as to whether this difficulty situations; and cultural conventions and possibilities. In
with narrative is a cause or a consequence of autism; either play, children consciously stretch themselves, and allow
way, it will interfere with early forms of play and all subse- themselves to be extended by a more able player, beyond
quent make-believe activities, and therefore learning in their current abilities. Thus play becomes development itself
which joint narratives are developed (Jordan, 2001). It is (Vygotsky, 1978).
possible, however, to structure drama in a way to recapture
the characteristics of these ritualistic early play formats, to Play tutoring is also a key feature of drama-in-education,
include those participants at early stages of learning (Peter, whereby children’s spontaneous play behaviour is chal-
1994, 2003). lenged by the teacher, extended and reflected upon. It
follows that a developmental apprenticeship approach, using
In typical development, the predictable framework of early drama that initially recaptures the characteristics of ritualis-
social play formats is gradually adjusted by the adult or tic early play formats, could make explicit the narrative
more able play partner, and infants engage in more process. This enables a narrative sense of self to emerge
complex flexible play sequences using toys and shared through children learning about various social roles and the
objects of attention. In time, in role play, children explore likely effect of responses in interactions, signalled by the
human experience, and the realm of different perspectives, teacher-in-role. Children can be supported to make links
motivations, intentions and consequences, to create increas- between the implications of play behaviour and real life, so
ingly complex social narratives. Similarly, drama work may that play contexts may be used as a vehicle for learning.
be organised using a range of conventions derived from Increasingly challenging ways of using the drama form may
theatre, to enable activity to be differentiated according to provide contexts for exploring key moments, possibilities
children’s developing abilities to engage in make-believe and consequences; some conventions are more demanding
and the drama form, and to challenge latent play potential than others in terms of group working and sophistication, the
and social understanding. complexity of the social narrative and the potential use of
drama for developing social understanding.
Bruner (1986) identifies the significance of narrative for
developing holistic thinking, distinguishing between this Play tutoring and drama as pedagogy
creative, flexible, intuitive mode, and a paradigmatic mode There are three aspects to play tutoring and drama work
that is logical, analytical and sequential. Smilansky (1968) (Sherratt & Peter, 2002) which together distil into meaningful
clarifies early sociodramatic play (that is, the conscious experience, and which can inform the use of drama as a
enactment of social routines) from more mature themed powerful learning medium and teaching tool. The first aspect
fantasy play that involves projection into imaginary sce- is the importance of harnessing children’s affective engage-
narios. Singer and Singer (1990) map these onto Bruner’s ment – the pleasurable ‘feel good’ factor from involvement
(1986) paradigmatic and narrative modes of thinking, in with others, and a growing awareness of oneself and others as
order to show the importance of both types of role play for they explore cultural conventions and learn appropriate
all-round development. In the context of developing drama, responses. Findings from neuroscience provide support for
this means that the practitioner needs to work towards the use of drama by highlighting the significance of emotional
extending children’s understanding and use of make- engagement and its links with cognition, with memorable
believe, and to go beyond simple re-enactments based on the learning contexts more likely to be favourably edited and
rehearsal of life skills (a common use of drama with people etched on the brain (Iveson, 1996). It is plausible too that
with learning difficulties). They too may be supported into when children engage in playful activity, the brain slips out of
contexts that project into ‘what if ’ and ‘as if ’, and chal- gear as in other mental states such as daydreaming (Hobson &
lenged to resolve problems in analogous real-life situations Stickgold, 1994); this may prompt neurochemical release and
in the drama. pathways becoming passable that may have been previously
obscured (Sherratt & Peter, 2002). It follows that repeated
Research has shown a correlation between the amount and drama opportunities will strengthen this kind of brain activ-
complexity of role play in very young typically developing ity, supporting the development of more flexible thought
children and their social competence (Fein, 1984; Connolly patterns. Engagement with the drama context will also trigger
& Doyle, 1984; Howes, 1983). A particular influence mirror neurons that are sensitive to human action (Rizzolati,
appears to be their home background and exposure to play- Fogassi & Gallese, 2006) and are known to be fragmented in
tutoring that commonly occurs between peers – younger and autism (Ramachandran & Lindsay, 2006); this is significant,
older, experienced and less experienced players. Dunn’s as its physicality could help to repair a possible bridge to
(1988, 1991) studies indicate the significance of warm learning by imitation.
family relationships and highly emotional interaction
between siblings, with children as young as 18 months Figure 1 provides a basic illustration of the ‘amygdala loop’:
engaging in role play, supported by their older brothers and when we respond to experience, a ‘gut reaction’ is triggered

10 British Journal of Special Education · Volume 36 · Number 1 · 2009 © 2009 The Author(s). Journal compilation © 2009 NASEN
Figure 1: The amygdala loop However, in tutoring contexts such as drama, participating in
role-play sequences based on analogous real-life social nar-
ratives may unlock children’s latent potential, and support
FRONTAL
developing social competence even in those with autism.
LOBES This will happen as they consciously adjust their behaviour
towards their play partner in different scenarios, using ana-
CORTEX – Executive
functioning lytical, logical thinking processes. Engaging in imaginative
– ‘Thinking themed fantasy play within drama may further facilitate
brain’
more creative and flexible thought, and thus deeper social
understanding: their level of engagement increases as they
take on a persona, and are caught up in the emotionality of
the dramatic tension, responding intuitively (not just analyti-
AMYGDALA
cally) in the situation, and relating to others in narrative
– ‘Emotional form and in different social roles. An important proviso,
brain’
however, is that they are then enabled afterwards to make
links between the fictitious drama and real-life situations.

A rationale for drama as narrative pedagogy from autism


Jordan (1999) reminds that there is no such thing as ‘autistic
and in a split second we cognitively rationalise our affective behaviour’: diagnosis is based purely on the coincidence of
feeling response and translate this into an appropriate nar- the observable. The implication is that in certain situations
rative course of action (Damasio, 2003). In people with (such as drama), the autism may appear to lift, with indica-
autism, neuroscience has revealed that there is impairment tors of improved social competencies (Sherratt & Peter,
of the amygdala – that part of the brain responsible for the 2002). This means inverting Wing’s (1996) diagnostic triad
initial evaluation of meaning (Ramachandran, 2003) – and of impairments for autism – difficulties in communication,
so they compensate by using analytical, logical thinking difficulties in social interaction and rigidity of thought.
processes; hence the appeal of rule-based approaches. Instead, evidence may be apparent in the form of more
However, it follows that drama as an enlivening experience flexible thinking (creative problem-solving, choice-making),
targets the under-functioning of the amygdala, which could effective communication and sharing of ideas, and more
address this primary disconnectedness in autism. This would intuitive sensitivity in interaction. This is illustrated in the
then provide a coherent and relevant rather than fragmented close analysis of the teaching and responses of participants
learning context, especially if it incorporates elements that in the case study below, first documented in a paper (Peter &
have personal salience for the individuals concerned (a Sherratt, 2003) presented at an inaugural international con-
‘special interest’, such as a favourite toy or colour, used as a ference on autism, co-ordinated by the Tamana Association
prop or item of costume). in New Delhi.

A second pedagogical consideration is the need to provide The following drama lesson was carried out in a group
an appropriate level of cognitive challenge. In typical containing three boys at Key Stage 2. These were all pro-
development, children will spontaneously seek this in role foundly socially challenged children on the autistic spectrum,
play as they create shared meanings, and explore social compounded by their severe learning difficulties. The lesson
boundaries and conventions. Those at earlier stages of uses a narrative framework termed a Prescribed Drama Struc-
learning may acquire understanding of representation ture (PDS) that harnesses features of early interactive play
through being explicitly tutored in play contexts by an formats (Peter, 1994, 1995), including a predictable narrative
adult or older peer as to how one object may be used to framework (a play sequence with understood key moments,
stand for another. Likewise, drama work with blatant, clearly marked by the adult working in role on the inside of
explicit creation of shared meanings may support even the drama); a shared focus of interest (a regular member of
severely affected children with autism; for example, staff in role and familiar objects as props), turn-taking
explaining that we will draw knobs on a cardboard box and (opportunities to listen, watch and regulate behaviour); inter-
pretend it is a television set. action within make-believe (for the timing of utterances and
movements); imitation and reinforcement of appropriate
A third pedagogical dimension is the complexity of the responses; and reciprocal involvement of players.
teaching context – the play narrative. In role play, there are
pivotal moments that impel further action; this hangs on a Example: PDS based on Where’s My Teddy? by Jez
moment of tension that involves intentions and conse- Alborough (Walker Big Book, 1992)
quences. Again, in play tutoring contexts, this may be explic-
itly revealed and talked through. Without developing this This PDS was designed to explore meanings embedded in
narrative dimension, children’s play becomes stuck: they the final illustration of a popular children’s picture story
remain within the familiar and known, and their repertoire of book. The main protagonist (a little boy called Eddy) is
social understanding will be limited, as exemplified by the portrayed as wide-eyed, tucked up safely in bed, having had
limited spontaneous play behaviour of children with autism. a traumatic encounter with a giant bear in the woods while

© 2009 The Author(s). Journal compilation © 2009 NASEN British Journal of Special Education · Volume 36 · Number 1 · 2009 11
looking for his lost teddy. The PDS explores implications The three boys with autism all concentrated throughout,
inferred from the visual text, and seeks to identify possible even ignoring a member of staff who was using a large
embedded meanings. For the purposes of this session, dra- hand-held camcorder. They showed indicators of sustained
matic licence was taken with the final illustration, whereby positive affective engagement; their disposition seemed to
Eddy was understood to have had a broken night’s sleep and reflect a state of receptiveness for social contact and encoun-
therefore was reluctant to get up for school. Eddy’s mother ter. Carl made sustained eye contact with both teachers-in-
(teacher-in-role as a plausible forgotten character) needs role; he also engaged in social referencing with a member of
help to get Eddy (played by a support member of staff) staff on several occasions, and experimented with this use of
through his personal care routine. The drama hinges on a gaze with a visitor. Joe watched intently for the duration
‘look out behind you’ game (a ritualistic narrative), framed of the lesson, sitting on the beanbag independently; he
by a repeated ditty between each task: responded positively to interaction directed towards him (for
example, by shaking a proffered hand) and, unusually for
Wake up Eddy [wash your face] him, accepted support to relate to the teacher-in-role as
Wake up Eddy [wash your face] Eddy. Norman beamed throughout the session; he verbalised
Wake up Eddy [wash your face] enthusiastically in response to the strategic use of questions
You’ll be late for school and complied willingly with tasks.

Each time, Eddy falls asleep again behind his mother’s back, The drama also supported the cognitive development of the
much to her (exaggerated) exasperation. Each child per- three boys, and there were indications of their emergent
formed two tasks, utilising real objects as props (for representational understanding. Each revealed latent play
example, a hairbrush, a flannel, a cup) with staff supporting capabilities to relate to objects functionally, and used these
as necessary. At the end of this process, Eddy is ready for within the make-believe. Carl put a cup to his lips and
school, resplendent in his red T-shirt. pretended to drink; Joe likewise applied a hairbrush to his
head. Norman pretended to eat cereal from an empty bowl,
The three boys were not known to me before this lesson announcing that he was ‘spooning papa-pops’ [coco-pops].
took place. This in itself may have added to their intrigue Their emerging understanding of symbolic transformations
and subconsciously facilitated a shared focus of joint atten- was also apparent: Joe appeared to understand object sub-
tion. Significantly too, perhaps, it meant that I was not stitution (Eddy’s bed improvised by a table top) and laid his
affected by prior conceptions of their possible limitations head down on the pillow; Carl appeared fascinated by Eddy
in terms of challenging their latent play potential. in bed, returning repeatedly to peer at him, indicating his
However, it was apparent on entering the school hall that grasp of a familiar member of staff as an ‘other’. Norman
these boys were profoundly challenged in their social demonstrated the ability to accept imaginary appearance/
behaviour. Staff outnumbered the boys in a ratio of two to disappearance, calling out ‘all gone’ as Eddy replaced his
one, and they had no peer interaction; the boys were some- spoon in the empty cereal bowl.
what agitated at this change from their regular routine,
rushing to the edges of the room and flapping their hands. Each of the three boys also showed initiative and creative
Their limited communication skills suggested that they responses within the make-believe that represented new nar-
were functioning at a pre-verbal level, with some compre- rative departures in their own terms. Joe imitated brushing
hension of single words and ability to follow basic instruc- Eddy’s hair, and only needed light guidance to apply a towel
tions to sit with the group. to Eddy. At the end of the lesson, Carl spontaneously took
the flannel from the tray and applied it to Eddy as if washing
Level of language is closely aligned with ability to engage in his face. Norman related immediately to the make-believe,
symbolic representation and to sustain elements in a play calling out to Eddy, ‘Hallo – what’s your name?’ He also
sequence. Use of a PDS, therefore, would enable the boys to consistently offered comment, drawing the attention of
extend their understanding of cause and effect in the social nearby staff by saying, ‘look, look, Annie, look, apron,
domain (‘I can carry out this action and this will be the apron, fast asleep!’ when Eddy dozed off again. He showed
result’), with the predictable structure affording opportuni- clear grasp of the ‘game of theatre’ (Bolton, 1992) calling
ties to consolidate their understanding of this basic social out ‘oh no’ as Eddy fell asleep again.
narrative. The turn-taking structure would also support basic
training in groupwork. Following a rousing introductory Despite their severe autism, all three boys seemed to be
name song to bring the group together and provide an oppor- liberated by the play structure: Norman began to verbalise
tunity to focus on me as a new teacher, the boys appeared to contributions to the drama, and Joe and Carl (both at a
settle. Joe relaxed on a beanbag placed within the circle of pre-verbal stage) were captivated by the ‘as if’ context,
chairs, while Carl and Norman sat independently, strategi- which appeared to trigger an impulse to play. Grove and Park
cally flanked by members of staff. Throughout the activity, (2001) regard this urge to share a social experience as the
the spontaneous gasps and applause of staff suggested that basis for all communication. Significantly, their play
the play responses of the boys exceeded previous attain- responses became more fluent the second time each boy was
ments and their expectations (this was confirmed afterwards required to perform a task in relation to the teacher-in-role.
in evaluative conversation). The lesson proceeded without This could not have been imitated learning, as the actual task
disruption for just over half an hour. differed (for example, on the first occasion, Joe was required

12 British Journal of Special Education · Volume 36 · Number 1 · 2009 © 2009 The Author(s). Journal compilation © 2009 NASEN
to brush Eddy’s hair, but on the second occasion to dry his purpose (such as a cup, a spoon, a hairbrush). The PDS also
hands). Carl’s play shifted from being prompted to engage supported the development of their representational under-
perfunctorily in a socio-dramatic script from real life (pre- standing, with the children painstakingly involved in adapt-
tending to drink from a real cup, and then applying this to ing the classroom environment to construct the make-
another in giving Eddy a drink); the security of the ritual believe bedroom and to create fictional characters (helping
drama framework may have enabled him then to engage in teachers into role with items of costume). They were intro-
embryonic role-taking (Fein, 1984; Harris, 2000) when at duced to a new symbolic departure: a familiar item (a table)
the end of the lesson he spontaneously took a flannel and was used flexibly as a prop and acquired a new meaning (a
wiped Eddy’s face. This perspective shift took him beyond bed), with the blanket, pillow and Eddy’s costume (pyjamas)
organising himself in his notional role (in which he himself helping to make this explicit. Importantly, new meanings
was caught up in an ‘other’ experience), to a level where he were developed within the group experience: representa-
was beginning to organise himself in the light of the attitudes tional understanding was shared with others, with everyone
and perspectives of the role responses of another. knowing the same ‘code’.

Following this initial lesson, the teaching staff subse- Affect


quently repeated this activity several times, which helped Secondly, the teachers-in-role responded with an emotional
consolidate the boys’ understanding of the predictable intensity and immediacy which necessarily prompted aware-
social narrative. When I returned a few weeks later, all ness of an ‘other’ in a meeting of minds (Trevarthen, 1979)
three boys made notable new departures. Norman sponta- – one of the foundations of a sense of ‘relatedness’ and of
neously seized props and items of costume and engaged in embryonic theory of mind (Oates, 1994). The teachers-in-
sustained independent socio-dramatic play (acting out role provided security by soaking up and responding contin-
washing up), and drew the attention of others, indicating gently to a range of the boys’ reactions. In the same way that
his latent social orientation. Joe responded with demon- caregivers intuitively exaggerate their responses to amplify
strable confidence and engagement, making eye contact meanings to infants, the attitude and commitment of the
throughout, to the delight of staff as he had been very teachers-in-role seemed to trigger emotional mechanisms in
withdrawn latterly. Carl seized items to extend the play those children with autism that enabled otherwise impotent
scenario (using a cloth to wipe the windows), opting to processes to function. Their attention was riveted rather than
join the group rather than sit remotely as he had at the start distracted by the surroundings, crucially focused on the
of the session. interaction. This was helped by distinct markers (such as
the use of visually attractive items of costume) to make clear
The success of the lessons, as evidenced above in the boys’ the pretence and awareness of ‘another’, but these were not
developing social competencies, can be attributed to the so elaborate that they distracted from the essential meanings
integration of the three pedagogical dimensions indicated being expressed through the role.
earlier. Together these facilitated coherent, meaningful
learning: appropriate cognitive interest, affective engage- Structure
ment and narrative structure (see Figure 2). The turn-taking game had key moments, which the boys
learned to recognise and anticipate: the repeated ditty that
Interest framed the pretence, and the peak of tension/excitement as
The first lesson was pitched at an appropriate cognitive level, Eddy fell asleep again, with the significance of this being
using content perceived as relevant by the boys. Real objects highlighted by the exasperated reaction of his mother. The
within their regular experience were used for their usual PDS was used pivotally to extend the children’s play, by
adjusting the challenges for individuals within the familiar
narrative (for example, the number of personal care items
Figure 2: Drama: coherent learning through imagined presented from which to choose, and the degree of adult
experience support offered). A future departure might open up the pre-
dictable narrative to the group through an unexpected
Exploring social roles and outcome, or by ‘tweaking’ one of the other key moments.
possibilities in play (Perhaps one morning Eddy is not feeling well. What could
narratives
they do? Do they show possible latent empathy to comfort
Eddy? Do they show initiative and awareness of a possible
CREATIVITY social narrative through taking appropriate action?)
structure
SOCIAL Drama as narrative pedagogy for all
UNDERSTANDING
Over time, ownership for the direction of a drama may be
interest affect handed increasingly to the children. The extent to which the
COMMUNICATION SENSITIVITY drama is pre-planned or given shape in process will depend
on the children’s growing understanding and confidence in
Making and sharing Engaging in emotionally using the drama form, and the teacher’s confidence in han-
cultural meanings and resonant interaction and dling, shaping and challenging play responses. Teachers will
representations salient experience
need to extend themselves in negotiating with the children in

© 2009 The Author(s). Journal compilation © 2009 NASEN British Journal of Special Education · Volume 36 · Number 1 · 2009 13
process, and in their familiarity with a range of options porting adults who are present at key moments may be
(conventions derived from theatre) for organising the drama crucial for some children in signalling appropriate meanings
towards shaping the unfolding narrative to focus on an and triggering a response. Whether drama is being devel-
emerging learning area. Working in a fully open-ended way oped from a known literary text or from an imaginary theme,
– that is, negotiating all decisions and directions of the the emerging narrative will necessarily reflect the morality
drama from the outset – is not necessarily best. The teacher of the prevailing culture. Drama in education enables chil-
may have in mind a very clear learning area to explore, and dren to explore this, and to re-examine a story’s idea, with
therefore would need to pre-plan a flexible route by which to topics and issues being revisited over time using increas-
provide a ‘focusing lens’ (Neelands, 1984). ingly sophisticated conventions derived from theatre. These
enable children to ‘play’ with the narrative and come to an
The power of drama is its double edge, which Boal (1979) understanding of the story’s possibilities and the art form
termed ‘metaxis’: doing it and watching oneself doing it at used to create it (Booth, 2000).
the same time. This ‘holding in mind more than one world’
necessarily demands flexibility, and strengthens brain archi- A drama based on a fictional literary text may explore a key
tecture. Drama offers a metaphorical device for exploring a moment in the story where a character (the teacher-in-role)
range of perspectives, and analytical potential for under- may be under tension, facing a problem or dilemma. The
standing why people think and behave as they do. The use of children (as themselves, but caught up in the fictional
role protects participants emotionally from failure, through context) may be impelled to help resolve the situation. This
action happening ‘one step removed’ from them. Drama may may entail deviating subsequently from the storyline and
bring about a change of view either directly through role in creating a new narrative; this is crucial in teaching children
the lesson, or retrospectively, through reflecting afterwards to understand that things can be different and that they can
out of role. Bolton and Heathcote (1999) consider this aspect be instrumental in causing that to be so. Table 1 illustrates
of drama – the self-spectatorship – a major contributory the kind of challenges needed in order to engage children in
factor for captivating children’s cognitive engagement: the drama over time, from earliest encounters to performance
children realise that what appears to be about something else work. These challenges need to address their cognitive
is really about them. engagement in developmentally relevant morals and values,
their associated affective engagement with the issues, and
Bolton and Heathcote (1999) also highlight two other sig- appropriate narrative structures towards resolving a related
nificant dynamics that contribute to the potency of drama problem. This is achieved by organising the drama in dif-
as a learning medium, both of which are linked in particu- ferent ways using conventions derived from theatre. At
lar to the use of the teacher-in-role. The first is the provi- more sophisticated levels, this may demand collaboration
sion of an energising, irresistible shared learning context – and sharing or presenting ideas to others (see DfES, 2004,
the affective engagement feels real in a context that is 2005).
experienced as realistic even though it is fictitious. The
second is the tension between a vested interest in an initial For example, in a drama based on the traditional story of ‘Jack
point of view and a contra-position (often presented by the and the Beanstalk’, the teacher-in-role as Jack’s mother could
teacher-in-role) which propels the narrative forward. This present her perspective on the course of events: she is des-
research with children with autism helps to explain Bolton perately worried about her son who has climbed up the
and Heathcote’s far-reaching claims for drama’s scope. In beanstalk. This would enable children (notionally in role as
particular, the emotional significance of the teacher-in-role ‘Jack’s friends’) to engage at a deeper level with possible
for drawing children in (Hobson, 2002), in the same way embedded issues, rather than being preoccupied simply with
that caregivers in early play interaction infuse the activity acting out the superficial storyline. Teachers-in-role can
with melodrama, humour, suspense, warmth and excite- provide a pretext for the children taking action (could they
ment. This is the foundation for eliciting intuitive help find Jack, and then explain to him appropriate ‘stranger
responses, with the enhanced emotional encounters pro- danger’ strategies?). This can empower children in a drama
moting direct, if embryonic, awareness of another’s mental convention popularised by Dorothy Heathcote (Wagner,
state and ‘theory of mind’ (Oates, 1994). This has pro- 1976), known as the ‘mantle of the expert’ (the children
found implications for developing social imagination, a effectively are elevated to a position of possessing vital
process that requires the ability to take on board a repre- knowledge and information, which they then utilise towards
sentation of others’ mental states, but without necessarily achieving an outcome). Depending on the task, this can be
sharing them (Harris, 1989). extended as a child-led approach to curriculum planning and
delivery, exploiting the natural links between subject areas
The teacher-in-role will need to attune sensitively, modulat- and children’s enthusiasm and motivation over weeks, driven
ing levels of emotional arousal to precipitate engagement. by a sense of urgency and purpose (for example, designing a
This may be achieved through infusing high energy through ‘Missing’poster for Jack, exploring concepts of big and small
an authority figure, or a more oblique, less invasive encoun- or growing their own beanstalk).
ter, perhaps with a low-status character in need of their help.
It is the use of dramatic tension that will enable children to Conclusion
engage with the values embedded in the unfolding narrative Drama is capable of motivating even hard-to-reach children,
– the ‘moral impulse’ (Winston, 1998). The reaction of sup- and developing their essential creativity at the core of their

14 British Journal of Special Education · Volume 36 · Number 1 · 2009 © 2009 The Author(s). Journal compilation © 2009 NASEN
Table 1: Exploring cultural narratives through drama

Drama forms in which to


Social cognition Affective engagement Narrative understanding experience tension and
(morals and values) (emotions, empathy) (structure) explore meanings Examples
Reactive: Courage ‘Feel good’/‘feel bad’ Sense of agency Prescribed drama Carrying out a job to
Attending Empowerment of Happy/sad Social causality: action structures (PDS) help desperate Snow
(experiencing the individual Scared/safe and consequence Rituals White (teacher-in-role)
drama) Teacher-in-role with her housework
Active: Caring for others Worried/relieved Problems can be solved (low status or use of Showing an act of
Responding (imitating Caring for our world Angry/calm and content with physical effort and negative role) friendship to lonely

© 2009 The Author(s). Journal compilation © 2009 NASEN


possibilities) Helping Upset/cheered up co-operation Cinderella
Initiating Saving Bereft/restored Others have needs and Tiptoeing past sleeping
(adult supporting) Respect and regard Bored/occupied rights too (turn-taking) giant (teacher-in-role)
Lonely/together
Interactive: Perseverance Tricked/justice Problems may need Teacher-in-role Remonstrating with Red
Sustaining (short, then Quick-wittedness Deceived/restored negotiation (high status) Riding Hood
longer, play New connections Cheated/exploited A positive solution can Movement (teacher-in-role) for
sequences based on Risk taking Betrayed/supported often be found ‘Living through’ a simple causing anxiety
familiar social Good/bad secrets Irritated/soothed narrative Instructing RRH on how
scripts) Stranger danger Embarrassed/confident Mantle-of-the-expert to stay safe
Advising Snow White on
how to be assertive
Proactive Persuasiveness Mixed feelings Dilemmas Open-ended PDS Guarding Jack’s (stolen)
Creating complex, Trust Guilty/atoning Problems aren’t Structured play treasures from up the
flexible imaginary Taking responsibility Temptation/resisting straightforward – may Teacher-led improvisation beanstalk against
play sequences Loyalty Desperation/hopeful require anticipation of Freeze frame/ potential procurement
Honour Struggle/reward outcome, persuasion, fast-forward/flashbacks (for example, from
Assertiveness Revenge/regard planning Teacher-in-role teacher-in-role as
Unfair/fair There may be several (vague role, agent) tramp, police officer)
possible outcomes Conscience alley Advising Jack on what to
do with the gold
Presenting/devising a Money/power are mirages Creating shared ‘truths’ Rehearsed pairs/group Presenting a possible
play sequence to Integrity – telling lies through organising improvisation consequence if Jack
communicate a Honesty pays ‘frames’ into a Tableau, narration keeps the gold (tableau
meaning to others Love wins through narrative Thought-tracking or group improvisation)
Using paradox, irony to Hot-seating, forum Scene to show life under
convey meaning theatre Queen Cinderella

British Journal of Special Education · Volume 36 · Number 1 · 2009


Physical theatre, mime

15
difficulties. Rather than wait for a mythical point at which the curriculum. This resonates with Elliott’s (2007) call for
children may be deemed ‘ready for drama’, it is possible to aesthetic pedagogy (‘aesthetic’ from the Greek word aesthe-
develop social understanding using an apprenticeship tikos, meaning a heightened state of human consciousness
approach for all (teacher and pupils) that harnesses play, and knowing) in learning contexts that develop from issues
which is the natural way that children learn. This approach of life, open to a diversity of outcomes, with conversation at
builds upon features of early interactive play formats, with the core. Placing drama at the heart of a child-led, interactive
the teacher-in-role as a powerful catalyst, shaping the direc- approach to curriculum planning is a growing trend in main-
tion of the drama in order to offer a reflective window on stream primary schools, although under-explored in relation
experience. This should be based on identifying the genera- to children who are socially challenged. This article has
tive conditions to support a ‘triad of competencies’: demonstrated how they too may be enabled, through drama,
improvements in children’s communication (of shared to gain insight into why people think and behave as they do
meanings), their sensitivity (empathy) and creativity and the consequences of social behaviour. They too may
(problem-solving). Pedagogical implications are that all learn to become more ‘mindful’ (Safran, 2001) and develop
three dimensions need to be appropriately addressed (cog- ‘possibility thinking’ (Craft, 2001); in other words, to have
nitive, affective and narrative structure) at all levels of drama an open attitude to life, and an implicit awareness of more
activity, in order to underpin potentially powerful, memo- than one perspective in situations and their implications.
rable, meaningful and coherent learning experiences. Drama as narrative pedagogy can offer even the most
remote, hard-to-reach, socially challenged children the
Neelands (1992) posits how drama also offers a sense of opportunity to develop a sense of narrative identity, and to
coherence through the effect of creating ‘real life’ demands move towards more effective participation within a social
and situations for the learner, which provides relevance to world.

References Dunn, J. (1988) The Beginnings of Social Understanding.


Boal, A. (1979) Theatre of the Oppressed. London: Pluto Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Press. Dunn, J. (1991) ‘Young children’s understanding of other
Bolton, G. (1992) New Perspectives on Classroom Drama. people: evidence from observations within the family’,
Hemel Hempstead: Simon & Schuster Education. in D. Frye and C. Moore (eds) Children’s Theories of
Bolton, G. & Heathcote, D. (1999) So You Want to Use Mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Role-Play? A new approach in how to plan. Stoke on Elliott, J. (2007) ‘Re-thinking pedagogy as the aesthetic
Trent: Trentham Books. ordering of learning experiences’, in J. Elliott,
Booth, D. (2000) ‘Reading the stories we construct Reflecting Where the Action Is. Oxon: Routledge.
together’, in J. Ackroyd (ed.) Literacy Alive! London: Fein, G. G. (1984) ‘The self-building potential of pretend
Hodder & Stoughton. play or “I got a fish, all by myself ” ’, in T. D. Yawkey
Bruner, J. (1986) Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. and A. D. Pellegrini (eds) Child’s Play: developmental
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. and applied. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bruner, J. & Feldman, C. (1993) ‘Theories of mind and Gilligan, C. (1982) In a Different Voice. Cambridge, MA:
the problem of autism’, in S. Baron-Cohen (ed.) Harvard University Press.
Understanding Other Minds: perspectives from autism. Grove, N. (2006) ‘Storytelling for all.’ Unpublished paper
Oxford: Oxford University Press. presented at Sunfield School, Worcs., 27 September
Connolly, J. A. & Doyle, A.-B. (1984) ‘Relation of social 2006.
fantasy play to social competence in pre-schoolers’, Grove, N. & Park, K. (2001) Social Cognition through
Developmental Psychology, 20, 797–806. Drama and Literature for People with Learning
Craft, A. (2001) ‘Little c creativity’, in A. Craft, B. Jeffrey Disabilities. London: Jessica Kingsley.
and M. Leibling (eds) Creativity in Education. London: Harris, P. (1989) Children and Emotion. Oxford:
Continuum. Blackwell.
Damasio, A. R. (2003) Looking for Spinoza: joy, sorrow Harris, P. (2000) The Work of the Imagination. Oxford:
and the feeling brain. New York: Harcourt. Blackwell.
DfES (Department for Education and Skills) (2004) Hobson, P. L. (2002) The Cradle of Thought. London:
Primary National Strategy – speaking, listening and Macmillan.
learning. London: DfES. Hobson, J. A. & Stickgold, R. (1994) ‘A neurocognitive
DfES (Department for Education and Skills) (2005) approach to dreaming’, Consciousness and Cognition, 3
Primary National Strategy – speaking, listening and (1), 1–15.
learning: working with children who have special Howes, C. (1983) ‘Peer interaction of young children’,
educational needs. London: DfES. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child

16 British Journal of Special Education · Volume 36 · Number 1 · 2009 © 2009 The Author(s). Journal compilation © 2009 NASEN
Development no. 217, 53 (1). Chicago: University of Jeffrey and M. Leibling (eds) Creativity in Education.
Chicago Press. London: Continuum.
Iveson, S. D. (1996) ‘Communication in the mind.’ Paper Sherratt, D. & Peter, M. (2002) Developing Play and
presented at the International Congress of Psychology Drama in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders.
XXVI Montreal. International Journal of Psychology, London: David Fulton Publishers.
31, 254. Singer, D. & Singer, J. (1990) The House of
Jordan, R. (1999) Autistic Spectrum Disorders. London: Make-Believe: children’s play and the developing
David Fulton Publishers. imagination. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Jordan, R. (2001) Autism with Severe Learning Press.
Difficulties. London: Souvenir Press. Smilansky, S. (1968) The Effects of Sociodramatic Play on
Lawler, S. (2002) ‘Narrative in social research’, in T. May Disadvantaged Pre-school Children. New York: Wiley.
(ed.) Qualitative Research in Action. London: Sage. Trevarthen, C. (1979) ‘Communication and co-operation
Neelands, J. (1984) Making Sense of Drama. Oxford: in early infancy: a description of primary
Heinemann. intersubjectivity’, in M. Bullowa (ed.) Before Speech.
Neelands, J. (1992) Learning through Imagined Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Experience. London: Hodder & Stoughton. Vygotsky, L. (1978) Mind in Society: the development of
Oates, J. (1994) ‘First relationships’, in J. Oates (ed.) The higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA:
Foundations of Child Development. Oxford: Blackwell/ Harvard University Press.
The Open University. Wagner, B. J. (1976) Dorothy Heathcote: drama as a
Peter, M. (1994) Drama for All. London: David Fulton learning medium. London: Hutchinson.
Publishers. Whitehead, M. (1997) Language and Literacy in the Early
Peter, M. (2003) ‘Drama, narrative and early learning’, Years. London: Paul Chapman Publishing.
British Journal of Special Education, 30 (1), 21–27. Wing, L. (1996) The Autistic Spectrum: a guide for
Peter, M. (2005) Making Drama Special. London: David parents and professionals. London: Constable.
Fulton Publishers. Winston, J. (1998) Drama, Narrative and Moral
Peter, M. & Sherratt, D. (2003) ‘Play-drama intervention: Education. London: Falmer Press.
a narrative approach to developing social understanding
in children with autistic spectrum disorders’, in
conference handbook: Autism: alternative strategies
(3–5 September), pp. 52–67. New Delhi: Tamana
Association. Address for correspondence:
Ramachandran, V. (2003) ‘The emerging mind: lecture 5 – Dr Melanie Peter
neuroscience, the new philosophy’, Reith Lecture 2003, Faculty of Education
BBC Radio 4, 30 April. Anglia Ruskin University
Ramachandran, V. & Lindsay, M. (2006) ‘Broken mirrors: Rivermead Campus
a theory of autism’, Scientific American, November, Chelmsford
63–69. Essex CM1 1SQ
Rizzolati, G., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. (2006) ‘Mirrors in Email: melanie.peter@anglia.ac.uk
the mind’, Scientific American, November, 63–69.
Safran, L. (2001) ‘Creativity as “mindful” learning: a case Article submitted: October 2007
from learner-led home-based education’, in A. Craft, B. Accepted for publication: September 2008

© 2009 The Author(s). Journal compilation © 2009 NASEN British Journal of Special Education · Volume 36 · Number 1 · 2009 17

You might also like