You are on page 1of 13

ZDM Mathematics Education (2012) 44:175–187

DOI 10.1007/s11858-012-0399-9

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

The quality of non-textual elements in mathematics


textbooks: an exploratory comparison between South Korea
and the United States
Rae Young Kim

Accepted: 19 March 2012 / Published online: 12 April 2012


Ó FIZ Karlsruhe 2012

Abstract As an exploratory investigation, this study aims visual representations may attract students’ attention and
to analyze non-textual elements in some Korean and US generate educational effects. In fact, these assumptions
mathematics textbooks using a conceptual framework have been supported by many studies (e.g. Evans et al.
whose components include accuracy, connectivity, con- 1987; Levie and Lentz 1982; Levin and Mayer 1993;
textuality, and conciseness. By analyzing three US text- Pettersson 1990), while other researchers have reported
books and three Korean ones, the study not only shows that visual images may not help students improve their
patterns in the use of non-textual elements in mathematics understanding and may even contribute to misunderstand-
textbooks in different contexts but also provides insights ing of the text (Watkins et al. 2004). Such contradictory
into how to assess the quality of non-textual elements in findings about visual representations have been debated
mathematics textbooks, which I hope will contribute to the mostly in literacy and science textbooks. However, these
provision of more meaningful and productive learning arguments have not been carefully tested in mathematics
opportunities to school children. Overall, the results from textbooks.
this study show that there is significant difference across In reading and science textbooks, Levin and Mayer
topics and textbooks, which implies different opportunities (1993) find five functions of visual images: decorational,
to learn through non-textual elements. This study makes a representational, organizational, interpretational, and
unique contribution to the conceptualization of non-textual transformational. However, although the findings are useful
elements in mathematics education and has implications for seeing the roles of pictures and illustrations in reading
for textbook analysis and curriculum development. and science textbooks, they are not enough to understand
visual representations in mathematics textbooks. Unlike
Keywords Non-textual elements  Mathematics typical pictures and illustrations in reading and science,
education  Textbook analysis  Curriculum development visual representations in mathematics textbooks can serve
not only as informative agents but also as ‘‘tools for
thinking’’ (Cuoco 2001) with which students manipulate
1 Introduction and experiment in their minds and with their hands. Con-
sidering ‘‘representation refers both to process and prod-
More pictures, illustrations, and diagrams have been used uct—to the act of capturing a mathematical concept or
in recent textbooks than in the past (Bazerman 2006; relationship in some form and to the form itself’’ (National
Boling et al. 2004; Evans et al. 1987). According to Council of Teachers of Mathematics 2000, p. 67), it is
Woodward (1993), this is due to the assumptions that such important to see how representations are used in mathe-
matics textbooks because ‘‘representation is more than a
process; it is a way of teaching and learning mathematics’’
R. Y. Kim (&) (Fennell and Rowan 2001, p. 288).
Department of Mathematics Education,
However, previous studies have generally failed to
Ewha Womans University, 52 Ewhayeodae-gil,
Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 120-750, South Korea capture the analytical significance of understanding the
e-mail: kimrae@ewha.ac.kr roles of pictures, illustrations, and diagrams in mathematics

123
176 R. Y. Kim

textbooks for teaching and learning. Although drawings, the various messages’’ (Bishop 1992, p. 185). From this
graphical displays, and diagrams are all representations that perspective, although some regard mathematics as an
present mathematical concepts (Stylianou 2010), most objective global language that has been taught across time
studies on visual representations in mathematics textbooks and place as a core subject (Kamens et al. 1996), it would
have focused mostly on mathematical representations such be important to investigate how non-textual elements in
as graphs and abstract figures while pictorial representa- mathematics textbooks are constructed and used in dif-
tions such as photos and illustrations are often regarded as ferent contexts. In particular, there are big gaps between
decoration and part of visual design (Levin and Mayer Asian and American students’ representational compe-
1993). Notwithstanding both visual mathematical repre- tence (Brenner et al. 1999). This calls attention to the
sentations and pictorial representations, called non-textual need to investigate what opportunities to learn by non-
elements in this study, are parts of mathematics textbooks, textual elements in mathematics textbooks are provided in
systematic research on their overall quality in mathematics different contexts such as South Korea and the United
textbooks has not been conducted yet. Although many States where substantial differences are observed in their
studies have analyzed visual representations in mathemat- student achievement (Schmidt et al. 2001). As a mediator
ics textbooks, most of them have focused on how specific between planned and enacted curriculum, textbooks are
topics or tasks are represented (e.g. Li et al. 2009; Stigler ‘‘components of opportunities to learn school subjects and
et al. 1986) or on general features of visual representations have their own characteristic impact on instruction’’
such as size, quantity, and layout (e.g. Valverde et al. (Valverde et al. 2002, p. 10). Therefore, by scrutinizing
2002). Little is known about how non-textual elements are the characteristics of non-textual elements in mathematics
used in mathematics textbooks in different topics and how textbooks in different contexts, this study may offer a new
well such non-textual elements represent mathematical way of understanding ‘‘the nature of the provided edu-
concepts and ideas. cational opportunities’’ (Blömeke and Paine 2008,
A non-textual element in this study denotes a visual p. 2036).
representation that is comprised of components that are not This study is an attempt to address the following
purely verbal, numerical, or mathematical symbolic rep- research questions: How are non-textual elements actually
resentations (Kim 2009c). For instance, the equation used in mathematics textbooks? Specifically, how are the
a2 ? b2 = c2 used in the Pythagorean Theorem is not a non-textual elements in current mathematics textbooks
non-textual element because it consists solely of symbolic used for particular topics (angle, slope, and prime factor-
notations. However, if it is explained by a picture of a right ization) in terms of accuracy, connectivity, conciseness,
triangle where necessary symbols and marks are used, the and contextuality? What kinds of variation or commonality
picture is a non-textual element since even though it has are found in the non-textual elements in current mathe-
some symbols and marks, it is not a purely symbolic rep- matics textbooks both within and across countries (i.e.
resentation. Non-textual elements thus include, for exam- South Korea and the United States) in terms of these four
ple, graphic representations including photos and pictorial aspects?
illustrations/pictures which portray objects in the real To answer these questions, I begin with the conceptual
world, and mathematical visual representations including framework used for the analysis of non-textual elements in
graphs and abstract mathematical figures. mathematics textbooks. Then, with explanation of the data
As an exploratory investigation, this study aims to and methodology, I describe the results from the textbook
analyze non-textual elements in some Korean and US analysis which shows the patterns of non-textual elements
mathematics textbooks using a conceptual framework in some Korean and US textbooks. A discussion of
informed by both theoretical and practical perspectives implications for non-textual elements in mathematics
(Kim 2009b, 2009c). By doing so, the study not only textbooks will follow at the end.
shows patterns in the use of non-textual elements in
mathematics textbooks in different contexts but also
provides insights into how to analyze the quality of these 2 Conceptual framework
elements, which I hope will contribute to the provision of
more meaningful and productive learning opportunities to Existing research has emphasized the importance of
school children. Since non-textual elements in textbooks mathematical representations in teaching and learning,
are easily recognized and apprehended without any lin- including verbal, visual, and symbolic representations
guistic knowledge, their interpretation may be influenced (Abrams 2001; Arcavi 2003; Brenner et al. 1997; Duval
by preconceptions, knowledge, and experiences generated 2006; Herman 2007; Pape and Tchoshanov 2001). The
in particular contexts because ‘‘people bring their cultural National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM)
histories into the classroom and interpret and reconstruct proposes representations as an integral component of its

123
The quality of non-textual elements in mathematics textbooks 177

standards that all students are expected to meet in school applied to various contexts (contextuality). In South
mathematics (National Council of Teachers of Mathemat- Korea, such ideas can be found in the most recent
ics 2000). Since representations are related to both content national curriculum, the 2009 revised curriculum, that
and process in learning mathematics, it is widely believed students should solve problems in various contexts (con-
that representations, as ‘‘tools for thinking’’ (Cuoco 2001), textuality), find relations among fundamental mathemati-
play important roles in one’s creation of ‘‘concept image’’1 cal concepts, skills, principles, and rules (connectivity),
(Tall and Vinner 1981), communication with others, and and use appropriate representations (accuracy) (Ministry
mathematical reasoning. Visual representations, in partic- of Education Science and Technology 2011). Further, I
ular, allow us to see what is not easily seen only from texts, added conciseness to the framework as a result from the
such as patterns, visualized objects and images that can be interview analysis with 21 teachers and seven curriculum
manipulated and experimented with, and tendencies of data developers from the two countries. Most of the intervie-
which allow us to predict the future (Arcavi 2003). Since wees argued that even a simple non-textual element could
visual representations provide students with concrete and be more effective for student learning than a complicated
concise images of related concepts, they help improve non-textual element to explain a particular concept (Kim
students’ understanding of the contents (Levin and Mayer 2009c). Based on the results from a student survey (29
1993). middle school students and seven graduate students from
Despite the importance of visual representations in the the two countries) and from interviews with practitioners
teaching and learning of mathematics, little effort has been (21 secondary mathematics teachers and seven curriculum
made to specify in any systematic way the characteristics developers from the two countries), this framework has
and roles of non-textual elements in mathematics text- been developed and tested with iterative textbook analysis
books. Most studies on visual representations in mathe- (Kim 2009c). More detailed descriptions about each
matics have mainly focused on their cognitive and aspect follow.
psychological effects on students’ mathematical learning, Accuracy indicates mathematical clarity and rigor of
such as how students generate their own concept images non-textual elements by definition of a concept. In other
mentally (i.e. internal representations), how students words, it measures how non-textual elements represent
express their mental images to communicate with others concepts and ideas clearly and correctly in mathematical
(i.e. external representations), and how students interpret ways. Obviously, it is important to communicate the con-
‘‘presented’’ representations (Goldin and Shteingold 2001). cepts and contents without misunderstanding. If an illus-
However, they have mainly analyzed the artifacts students tration offers unclear or wrong information, a student may
created or interaction with others in classrooms through have more confusion and difficulty in solving the related
representations. problem. Since it is not easy to objectify mathematical
In order to obtain more systematic understanding of concepts (Sfard 1991), an unclear illustration could be an
non-textual elements in mathematics textbooks, I have impediment to constructing concept images (Zazkis and
developed a conceptual framework which has emerged Liljedahl 2004). Further, if the student creates a concept
not only from theoretical ideas and scholarly debates but image with flaws from the illustration, it could impede his
also from practitioners’ (teachers and curriculum devel- or her future learning. For example, Fig. 1 shows examples
opers) points of view (Kim 2009a, 2009c). The important of accuracy. Both photos show real situations, but they
aspects of non-textual elements in the conceptual frame- have different levels of accuracy. Although Fig. 1(1) is
work are: accuracy, connectivity, contextuality, and con- used to show parallel lines, such an inaccurate picture can
ciseness. Each conceptual aspect in the framework is mislead students to conclude that any two curvy lines can
grounded in the basis of a diverse body of research on be parallel if they do not meet or parallel lines can meet
mathematics education, semiotics, metaphor theory, visual somewhere if they are extended further. In fact, the results
rhetoric, and information design. The concepts of accu- from the student survey with 29 middle school students and
racy, connectivity, and contextuality mainly draw on the seven graduate students who have teaching experience in
NCTM standards (2000) that emphasize the importance of K-12 schools and have majored in mathematics education
using appropriate representations (accuracy) with inter- show that some of the middle school students misunder-
connection among mathematical ideas (connectivity) stood the concept of parallel lines from the picture while
the graduate students understood the concept appropriately.
1
Concept image means ‘‘the total cognitive structure that is This implies that, unlike teachers who already know the
associated with the concept, which includes all the mental pictures concept very well, young students may be easily misguided
and associated properties and processes’’ (Tall and Vinner 1981,
by an inaccurate picture, especially when learning a new
p. 151). Students often create their own concept image, think about
mathematical concepts and solve problems, and communicate with concept. In contrast, Fig. 1(2) shows an example of an
others through representations. angle in a real-life situation appropriately in terms of the

123
178 R. Y. Kim

Fig. 1 Examples of low (1) and


high (2) levels of accuracy. (1)
Source: CMP Shapes and
Designs (p. 38). (2) Source:
Daekyo Middle School Soohak 1
(p. 184)

definition of angle, ‘‘a pair of rays with a common end-


point’’ (Mitchelmore and White 2000).
Connectivity means how closely non-textual elements
are related to the mathematical content embodied in the
texts. It is crucial to analyze non-textual elements to
understand how they convey the meaning because students,
especially young children, may be influenced by non-tex-
tual elements in their learning (Bazerman 2006). Braden
(1983) coins the terms ‘‘visual–verbal symbiosis’’ and
‘‘visual–verbal discontinuity’’ for describing the relation-
ship between visual and verbal elements. Visual–verbal
symbiosis means well-connectedness and supportiveness to
each other between visual and verbal elements, while
visual–verbal discontinuity indicates disconnection
between visual and verbal representations. Many studies Fig. 2 An example showing high (upper right) and low (lower right)
have found that a symbiotic connection between verbal and levels of connectivity. Source: CMP Moving Straight Ahead (p. 77)
visual literacy helps improve student achievement when
the two are united (Braden 1983; Dwyer 1988; Herbel- may lead to a misconception by simply associating the
Eisenmann 2002; Levin and Mayer 1993). Since visual mathematical concept of slope with the surface of a hill for
representations can serve as models to show what students skiing, which is also the meaning of slope in everyday
cannot see in texts and symbols (e.g. patterns and concrete language. In other words, when the picture of a skier is
images) and as tools to solve problems (Arcavi 2003), it is given, ‘‘slope’’ can be understood as the surface of a hill,
very important to have close connection between verbal especially a ski slope, which is different from the degree of
and visual representations to support students’ learning. In tilt in mathematics.
fact, as each student may not have the same level of textual Contextuality denotes presentation of mathematical
literacy as of visual literacy, students who have difficulty in ideas in realistic context(s). NCTM standards (2000)
reading and understanding texts may get more information emphasize the importance of students’ mathematical
from, and better understand, visual representations. Winn experiences in a context because ‘‘using mathematics in
(1987) asserts that graphical representations have, often but applied situations leads to deeper understanding’’ (p. 93).
not always, more influence on improving performance of Lesh and English (2005) argue that students develop their
students with low ability than on those with high ability. knowledge in the process of modeling and are able to apply
This suggests that visual representations also can provide their mathematical knowledge to the situations which they
some students with opportunities to learn mathematics experience. Considering that non-textual elements can
which they cannot understand only from textual represen- provide contexts that facilitate the students’ modeling
tations. Figure 2 shows an example of connectivity. process, it is thus important to examine what kinds of sit-
Although students are asked to decide if an avalanche is uations are provided in mathematics textbooks and how
likely for Hotdog Hill, the picture of a skier is not well they represent mathematical concepts and ideas in varying
connected with the text while the red mountain is well situations. Wiggins (1993) asserts that the compartmen-
connected by showing how the hill looks. Although the talization of knowledge and the decontextualization of
problem in the text is about the relationship between ava- knowing are problematic because competency requires
lanche and slope of the hill, the picture of a female skier both context and reasoning. This claim is consistent with

123
The quality of non-textual elements in mathematics textbooks 179

Fig. 4 Examples of high (1) and low (2) levels of conciseness. (1)
Source: CMP Thinking with Mathematical Models (p. 35). (2) Source:
Didimdol Middle School Soohak 2 (p. 120)

Fig. 3 Examples of varying levels of contextuality (p. 241). This becomes more important when a new con-
cept is introduced because a simple non-textual element
can convey the meaning and idea clearly and effectively.
the results from Ferratti and Okolo’s research (1996) that Simple non-textual elements might allow students to better
students’ thinking skills and attitudes are enhanced when understand the core ideas of the concept as well as find
they collaborate in the solution of authentic problems. connections among the related concepts if reiterated
Contextuality is not only a matter of texts and problems. through related ideas. Since mathematics is a remarkably
Contextual non-textual elements may influence students’ precise subject, ambiguity from complicated non-textual
understanding and learning through links between mathe- elements may hinder students’ understanding of the con-
matics and real life. Such experiences in the textbooks give cept (Goldin and Shteingold 2001). Figure 4 shows two
students an opportunity to think about mathematics in graphs of linear functions. Figure 4(1) shows a linear
contexts and deepen their understanding (National Council function and two points on the line so that students can
of Teachers of Mathematics 2000). Figure 3 shows exam- easily recognize which line they should pay attention to.
ples of contextuality. However, even though realistic con- However, Fig. 4(2) shows too many lines and the lines are
texts are helpful for students to understand mathematics, not clear enough to read, so it is not easy to find which lines
simply giving realistic contexts does not makes it mathe- students should focus on even though two equations for the
matics (Freudenthal 1991). Although both a and b in Fig. 3 lines are written.
show real objects (i.e. scissors), they play different roles in
terms of mathematization. While students may have diffi-
culties in finding mathematical ideas from Fig. 3(1), they 3 Data and methodology
may easily see the usefulness of mathematics in Fig. 3(2)
because it shows how the real object can be used to In order to find the patterns of non-textual elements used in
understand the concept of angle. In other words, Fig. 3(1) mathematics textbooks in different contexts—South Korea
shows just a real object while Fig. 3(2) gives ideas about and the US—I analyzed non-textual elements in the lessons
how to use the object in mathematical ways. Figure 3(3) of angle, slope, and prime factorization in three US sec-
shows an example of a non-textual element without con- ondary mathematics textbook series and three South Kor-
textuality. In other words, it does not have any realistic ean ones. In terms of the content area, the three topics were
contexts to explain the concept. Although Fig. 3(3) has a examined to see if non-textual elements are used similarly
high level of mathematical connectivity, it does not have or differently across the concepts. Angle is one of the basic
contextuality because it is not represented in a realistic ideas in geometry while prime factorization is one of the
context. topics in algebra. Slope is an important idea in both algebra
Conciseness implies mathematical succinctness in a and geometry. Although angle, slope, and prime factor-
non-textual element. Conciseness suggests how a non- ization are fundamental topics in mathematics, many
textual element is concise and neat in presenting a concept studies have reported that students have a hard time
or problem without any redundant, unnecessary or dis- applying mathematical ideas to realistic contexts as well as
tracting factors. Pettersson (2001) argued that ‘‘too many constructing mathematical ideas from realistic situated
details and too much complexity reduce the interest for the problems (e.g. Freudenthal 1991; Meyer 2001; Woleck
content in the visual. Too few details or too little com- 2001). With regard to the lack of research on the relationship
plexity makes it impossible to understand the picture’’ between mathematical and pictorial representations in

123
180 R. Y. Kim

Table 1 Information about the textbooks selected for analysis in this Table 2 Coding scheme for textbook analysis
study Aspect of non- Score Description
Country Textbook series Year Publisher textual elements
published
Accuracy 2 An NTE correctly shows the definition
South Middle School Soohak 2008/ Didimdol of a concept, or it is accurate to show a
Korea (K1) 2009 concept based on its definition
Middle School Soohak 2008/ Daekyo 1 An NTE makes sense in terms of the
(K2) 2009 definition or meaning of a concept. But
it does not show every required
Middle School Soohak 2008/2009 Doosan Donga
mathematical condition (e.g. some
(K3)
requisite notations are missing or
USA Connected Mathematics 2006 Pearson Prentice misleading) or some attributes are not
Project (CMP) (U1) Hall appropriate to explain a concept
Holt Middle School 2004 Holt, Rinehart 0 An NTE is inaccurate in terms of the
Math (U2) and Winston definition of a concept (e.g. it has an
MathThematics (U3) 2006 McDougal Littell obvious error). Or an inappropriate
realistic object is used to present a
concept. There is a major error or
concern to use the realistic object or
mathematics textbooks, it is necessary to investigate how context for a concept. Or there is no
these concepts are described. In addition, these three topics mathematical concept in the NTE
are more comparable across the country contexts than other Connectivity 2 An NTE is explicitly and fully
associated with the mathematical
topics because they are taught at similar grade levels in content in the text. It directly shows a
both countries. concept or problem
The selection of the textbooks for this study was done by 1 An NTE is partly related to the
a purposive sampling strategy. All the selected textbooks mathematical content in the text. There
were among the most popularly used textbooks in each is some missing or irrelevant
information in an NTE. It shows the
country. All Korean textbooks are written based on the
content but it does not explicitly show
2007 revised national curriculum and are those most how it is connected with the content
recently published. Student workbooks were excluded from 0 An NTE has nothing to do with the
the analysis. Two of the US textbooks (U1 and U3) are content mathematically. It can give
standards-based textbooks and one (U2) is a conventional some clue about contexts in texts (e.g.
river when the problem is about length
textbook. Although they are not representative of all text-
of river)
books in each kind, it would be a useful exploration to see
Conciseness 2 An NTE is straightforward to show a
if there is any notable pattern in their use of non-textual concept or problem without any
elements. Table 1 gives the information about the selected distracting or other factor
textbooks. 1 An NTE is straightforward to show a
A coding scheme was developed according to the con- concept or problem with some other
ceptual framework for analyzing the four aspects of non- factors that might be helpful for the
concept
textual elements (see Table 2). Each aspect was coded to
0 An NTE has distracting or other factors
make the score range from zero for the lowest degree to that are useless in addition to factors
two for the highest degree in terms of the extent to which a needed to show a concept or problem
given non-textual element has characteristics of an aspect, Contextuality 2 A realistic object or context is used in an
so that every non-textual element can have a distinct score NTE with mathematical connection
for each aspect (see Kim 2009b). 1 No mathematical ideas or concepts exist,
In order to check inter-rater reliability and take into but there is some realistic contextual
information (that is used to provide
account cultural differences, three coders who had different contexts or objects in the problem or to
national backgrounds and experiences with textbooks facilitate related activities)
participated in coding the textbooks. When the coders 0 Neither realistic object nor realistic
disagreed, the median of the three coders’ code for a non- context is included in an NTE
textual element was used as its final code. An inter-rater
reliability analysis using the Kappa statistic was performed
to determine consistency among the raters. The inter-rater (American) and C (Korean), Kappa = .75 (p \ .001), and
reliability for the raters A (researcher) and B (American) for the raters A and C, Kappa = .93 (p \ .001). That is, the
was found to be Kappa = .77 (p \ .001), for the raters B overall Kappa coefficients found in this study can be

123
The quality of non-textual elements in mathematics textbooks 181

interpreted as indicating a substantial or outstanding level Table 4 Descriptive statistics of the three aspects of non-textual
of inter-rater reliability. In order to find patterns of non- elements: mathematical representations and pictorial representations
textual elements, the data were analyzed and compared in Mathematical Pictorial t p-value
terms of topics, textbook, and country. representations representations (2-tailed)
(n = 202) (n = 95)
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
4 Results
Accuracy 1.78 (.43) .86 (.79) 10.59 \.001
Connectivity 1.97 (.17) 1.23 (.80) 8.85 \.001
4.1 Overall patterns of non-textual elements
Conciseness 1.94 (.59) .80 (.68) 15.87 \.001
in the selected textbooks

The number of non-textual elements coded in this study


was 297 from a combined total of 107 pages from South
Korean and US textbooks. The average numbers of non- SD = .81) is somewhat lower than those for other aspects.
textual elements per page used in each topic in each This implies that non-textual elements in mathematics
country context were calculated as shown in Table 3. In textbooks are likely not to use realistic contexts to explain
comparison between the two countries, South Korean concepts across topics. In fact, only 95 out of 297
textbooks have more non-textual elements per page than (approximately 32 %) non-textual elements have realistic
their US counterparts for each topic. However, general contexts. They are all pictorial representations such as
patterns in the use of non-textual elements in each topic are photos and illustrations. Further, it is noticeable that around
similar between South Korea and the United States. The 56 % of them are used as decorational (those without
greatest number of non-textual elements is used in lessons relation to texts) or representational (having to do with
about angle, and the least number in lessons about prime parts of texts, e.g. a picture of ships is used for a problem to
factorization in both South Korean and US textbooks. get the distance between two ports) (Levin and Mayer
Around 50 % of the total number of non-textual elements 1993). Table 4 shows that pictorial representations in
is used in angle in both South Korean and US textbooks. mathematics textbooks are relatively lower in terms of
Since angle is one of the basic ideas in geometry for accuracy, connectivity, or conciseness compared with
understanding of geometric shapes and structures as well as mathematical representations.
for analyzing their relationships, this is not surprising. In
the process of reasoning, it is evident that representations 4.2 Patterns of non-textual elements by topic
in geometry are crucial to developing geometric ideas.
Since the concept of slope can be found in formulas and Overall, as shown in Fig. 5, mathematical representations
equations in algebra, in graphs in geometry, and as the are more often used than pictorial representations across
tangent of an angle in trigonometry (Stump 2001), slopes topics in both Korean and US textbooks. Interestingly, even
are sometimes explained with only verbal representations. though there is the smallest number of non-textual ele-
Noticeably, non-textual elements in prime factorization are ments in the lessons of prime factorization, the proportion
not used as often as in the other two topics. of pictorial representations such as photos and illustrations
Considering that a code of two points means the full in the topic is larger than in the other topics. This means
score for each aspect, the mean scores for each aspect show that prime factorization as an algebraic topic is taught by
that non-textual elements in the textbooks are by and large many non-textual elements with realistic contexts in the
accurate, well connected and concise: accuracy textbooks. However, around 71 % of them have no math-
(mean = 1.49, SD = .71); connectivity (mean = 1.73, ematical ideas or concepts. In other words, they are used as
SD = .59); and conciseness (mean = 1.58, SD = .69). decorational. For instance, a picture of a sundial is included
However, the mean score for contextuality (mean = .53, without any mathematical idea. There is no significant
difference between the two countries with regard to the
proportion of such decorative non-textual elements. This
Table 3 Number of non-textual elements used in each topic and the
average number per page implies that even though many non-textual elements with
realistic contexts are used for prime factorization, most of
Angle Slope Prime factorization Overall
them are not likely to contribute to students’ mathematical
n Mean n Mean n Mean n Mean understanding.
Korea 71 5.5 59 2.3 26 1.9 156 2.9
In addition, slope is taught by either mathematical visual
non-textual elements as an important mathematical concept
USA 82 3.9 43 2.0 16 1.2 141 2.6
to understand linear functions or contextual non-textual

123
182 R. Y. Kim

Fig. 5 Proportions of mathematical representations and pictorial representations by topic and country

elements as understanding of steepness in real life. As lessons of prime factorization are not well connected with
shown in Fig. 5, the concept of slope is likely to be taught the concept and are more complex than those in the lessons
using abstract mathematical representations in South Korea on angle. Considering that the total number of non-textual
while it is taught using contextual non-textual elements in elements in the lessons of prime factorization is much
the US as often as is the concept of angle. smaller than in other topics, this calls special attention to
Table 5 presents means and the results for the four the need for well-connected and concise non-textual ele-
aspects of non-textual elements used in mathematics text- ments to be developed for prime factorization.
books by topic. Connectivity and conciseness of non-tex- In order to see if there is any difference in aspects of
tual elements are significantly different across topics, non-textual elements among topics in different contexts, I
especially between angle and prime factorization, while calculated mean differences in the four aspects in the
accuracy and contextuality are not significantly different selected South Korean textbooks and US textbooks by
across topics. This implies that non-textual elements in the topic as shown in Tables 6 and 7. While accuracy,

Table 5 Mean differences in the four aspects of non-textual elements Table 6 Mean differences in the four aspects of non-textual elements
in textbooks, by topic in the selected South Korean textbooks, by topic
F test Scheffe’s test F test Scheffe’s test

n Mean (SD) F A–B B–C C–A n Mean (SD) F A–B B–C C–A

Accuracy Accuracy
Angle (A) 153 1.543 (.639) .954 Angle (A) 71 1.73 (.63) 2.646
Slope (B) 102 1.441 (.739) Slope (B) 59 1.53 (.63)
Prime 42 1.405 (.885) Prime 26 1.42 (.86)
factorization factorization
(C) (C)
Connectivity Connectivity
Angle (A) 153 1.837 (.465) 5.889** * Angle (A) 71 1.87 (.48) 4.136* *
Slope (B) 102 1.667 (.635) Slope (B) 59 1.75 (.54)
Prime 42 1.524 (.773) Prime 26 1.50 (.81)
factorization factorization
(C) (C)
Conciseness Conciseness
Angle (A) 153 1.647 (.590) 3.481* * Angle (A) 71 1.63 (.64) 2.631
Slope (B) 102 1.569 (.725) Slope (B) 59 1.73 (.52)
Prime 42 1.333 (.874) Prime 26 1.38 (.85)
factorization factorization
(C) (C)
Contextuality Contextuality
Angle (A) 153 .569 (.864) .536 Angle (A) 71 .54 (.84) .652
Slope (B) 102 .461 (.779) Slope (B) 59 .39 (.72)
Prime 42 .524 (.707) Prime 26 .54 (.71)
factorization factorization
(C) (C)

* p \ .05; ** p \ .01 * p \ .05

123
The quality of non-textual elements in mathematics textbooks 183

Table 7 Mean differences in the four aspects of non-textual elements Table 8 Descriptive statistics of the four aspects of non-textual
in the selected US textbooks, by topic elements in the selected South Korean and US textbooks
F test Scheffe’s test South Korea United t p value
(n = 156) States (n = 141) (2-tailed)
n Mean (SD) F A–B B–C C–A
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Accuracy
Angle (A) 82 1.39 (.62) .097 Accuracy 1.60 (.68) 1.37 (.73) 4.53 \.01
Slope (B) 43 1.35 (.84) Connectivity 1.76 (.58) 1.70 (.60) 2.92 .374
Prime 16 1.31 (.95) Conciseness 1.63 (.64) 1.51 (.75) 3.03 .171
factorization (C) Contextuality .48 (.77) .57 (.86) 1.51 .325
Connectivity
Angle (A) 82 1.79 (.49) 2.801
Slope (B) 43 1.58 (.70) Table 9 Descriptive statistics of the four aspects of non-textual
Prime 16 1.50 (.73) elements in the South Korean textbooks, by textbook
factorization (C)
Textbook F-test
Conciseness
Angle (A) 82 1.63 (.62) 2.869 n Mean (SD) F
Slope (B) 43 1.37 (.87)
Accuracy
Prime 16 1.25 (.93)
K1 50 1.62 (.64) .120
factorization (C)
K2 58 1.62 (.70)
Contextuality
K3 48 1.56 (.71)
Angle (A) 82 .60 (.89) .097
Connectivity
Slope (B) 43 .56 (.85)
Prime 16 .50 (.73)
K1 50 1.78 (.58) .065
factorization (C) K2 58 1.74 (.58)
K3 48 1.77 (.59)
Conciseness
conciseness, and contextuality in the South Korean text- K1 50 1.66 (.63) .200
books are not different across topics, connectivity is sig- K2 58 1.59 (.62)
nificantly different, especially between angle and prime K3 48 1.65 (.70)
factorization. This means that non-textual elements used Contextuality
for prime factorization are not well connected with texts K1 50 .50 (.79) .107
compared with those for angle in Korean textbooks. K2 58 .50 (.78)
In the US textbooks studied, however, there is no sig- K3 48 .44 (.77)
nificant difference in each aspect across topics. Even
though there are significant differences in connectivity and
conciseness across the textbooks (see Table 5), such dif- for the four aspects. The results from the t tests show that
ferences are not observed in different topics (see Table 7). the mean difference in the scores of accuracy between the
In other words, although connectivity and conciseness vary two countries is significant. This seems to be consistent
across the selected textbooks, there are no significant dif- with the results from the report that many errors were
ferences in connectivity and conciseness across topics. This found in many US secondary mathematics textbooks
implies that such differences do not come from the nature (National Mathematics Advisory Panel 2008).
of different topics. ANOVA was used to analyze variations in the use of
non-textual elements across textbooks and explore the
4.3 Patterns across the selected US and South Korean patterns of these elements in different types of textbooks.
textbooks Textbooks K1, K2, and K3 are South Korean textbooks and
textbooks U1, U2, and U3 are US textbooks. While there is
Table 8 presents descriptive statistics for the scores of the no significant difference in the mean scores of the four
four aspects of non-textual elements between South Korea aspects of non-textual elements across the selected South
and the United States. Except for contextuality, the mean Korean textbooks as shown in Table 9, the US textbooks
scores of all the aspects of non-textual elements in the studied have more variation in non-textual elements across
selected South Korean textbooks are higher than their US textbooks as shown in Table 10. The Korean textbooks are
counterpart. However, the standard deviations for the US thus more uniform in terms of the four aspects of non-
textbooks are greater than those of the Korean textbooks textual elements across textbooks.

123
184 R. Y. Kim

Table 10 Descriptive statistics of the four aspects of non-textual 5 Conclusion and discussion
elements in the US textbooks, by textbook
F-test Scheffe’s test Overall, the results from this study show that non-textual
elements in the selected textbooks are by and large accu-
n Mean (SD) F U1–U2 U2–U3 U3–U1
rate, well connected and concise. However, pictorial rep-
Accuracy resentations which are highly contextualized tend not to be
U1 70 1.29 (.78) .910 accurate, well connected, or concise enough. Such repre-
U2 21 1.43 (.60) sentations are not likely to contribute to students’ mathe-
U3 50 1.46 (.71) matical understanding (Zazkis and Liljedahl 2004).
Connectivity Although pictorial representations are part of mathematics
U1 70 1.56 (.65) 4.112* textbooks, people usually assume that they are used only
U2 21 1.90 (.30) decoratively or motivationally so that they may not affect
U3 50 1.80 (.57) students’ mathematical understanding. However, many
Conciseness studies have already revealed the influence of pictorial
U1 70 1.33 (.79) 4.680* * representations on student learning (e.g. Misailidou and
U2 21 1.81 (.51) Williams 2003; Peeck 1993; Stylianidou 2002). In addi-
U3 50 1.64 (.72) tion, since pictorial representations can be used as tools for
Contextuality thinking by providing realistic contexts or images related to
U1 70 .93 (.95) 14.250*** ** *** problems or situations, it is important to ensure that they
U2 21 .29 (.72) are accurate, well connected, and concise enough to help
U3 50 .20 (.49)
students develop mathematical knowledge.
Considering the importance of understanding of math-
* p \ .05; ** p \ .01; *** p \ .001 ematics within contexts, this calls attention to the devel-
opment of non-textual elements with not only contextuality
but also accuracy, connectivity, and conciseness. Also,
Compared with the South Korean textbooks, the US when curriculum developers and publishers choose a non-
textbooks studied have more variation in non-textual ele- textual element with realistic contexts, they should pay
ments across textbooks. Table 10 shows that there are more attention to which non-textual element could be the
significant mean differences in connectivity, conciseness, most appropriate one to explain the related concept with
and contextuality across textbooks. Unlike the South Kor- regard to accuracy, connectivity, and conciseness.
ean textbooks, the sampled US textbooks vary in terms of Regarding the variation of non-textual elements across
the four aspects of non-textual elements. For example, the topics, the results show that connectivity and conciseness
degree of mathematical connectivity of non-textual ele- of non-textual elements are significantly different across
ments differs across textbooks. In the lessons about angle, topics, especially between angle and prime factorization,
slope, and prime factorization, textbook U1 has more while accuracy and contextuality are not significantly dif-
complicated non-textual elements while textbook U2 has ferent across topics. In particular, significant difference is
simpler non-textual elements. The mean scores of con- observed in the Korean textbooks, especially between
ciseness between the two textbooks are also significantly angle and prime factorization. This means that non-textual
different. In terms of contextuality, textbook U1, a stan- elements in the lessons of prime factorization are less
dards-based textbook, is significantly different from text- concise and more disconnected with the concept compared
books U2 and U3. Considering that U2 is a conventional with those of angle. The greatest number of non-textual
textbook and U3 is a standards-based textbook, this shows elements is used in lessons about angle, and the least
that there is a variation not only between standards-based number is used in lessons about prime factorization in both
textbooks and the conventional textbook but also between South Korean and US textbooks. However, the result
the standards-based textbooks. Even though standards- indicates that there are few well-connected and concise
based textbooks are assumed to have more mathematical non-textual elements for prime factorization. In fact, only
problems embedded in real contexts, the extent of use of one type of mathematical representation, a tree graph, is
non-textual elements in realistic contexts can vary across found in the lessons of prime factorization. Even when it
different textbooks. Overall, whereas the South Korean comes to pictorial representations, around 71 % of them
textbooks studied are not significantly different in terms of are used as decorational. The rest of them are used as
aspects of non-textual elements, the US textbooks in the informative, for instance a picture of Eratosthenes with a
sample have significant differences in terms of all the sieve, which can hardly be used as a tool for thinking. Such
aspects of non-textual elements except accuracy. patterns of non-textual elements are not only a matter of

123
The quality of non-textual elements in mathematics textbooks 185

type of representations but also reflect opportunities to 2007; Mitchelmore and White 2000). It could be hard for
learn mathematics. students to understand the connection between the two
This study offers a range of important implications. different approaches as well as the connection between the
First, it shows one possible way to assess the quality of concept of slope and its real-world application (Wagener
non-textual elements in mathematics textbooks and other 2009). While steepness of a hill can be recognized intui-
educational materials. Recently, increasing numbers of tively from our lives, rate of change in a graph may be hard
non-textual elements have been incorporated into mathe- for students to understand. The different way of using non-
matics textbooks to attract students’ and teachers’ atten- textual elements means not merely using different types of
tion. However, there has been no analytic framework or non-textual elements. It is more related to mathematical
tool to examine those non-textual elements. The results concepts. Considering that students should understand the
from this study analyzing the selected South Korean and two different ideas together to learn advanced algebra and
US textbooks cannot be generalized. Yet, as an exploratory geometry, it would be important to give opportunities to
investigation, the study provides not only an example of learn from both approaches.
how to analyze non-textual elements but also some International comparative studies may allow us to see
meaningful ideas about their current status. For instance, students’ opportunities to learn in different countries as
there is significant difference between the Korean and US well as to find alternative ways beyond our local discourse
textbooks in terms of accuracy. In addition, significant and practice (Blömeke and Paine 2008, p. 2036). Many
differences are found not only between conventional and comparative studies have found substantial gaps between
standards-based textbooks but also among the standards- Korean and US students’ performance (e.g. Mayer et al.
based textbooks in terms of connectivity, conciseness, and 1995; Schmidt et al. 2001; Stigler 1990; Stigler et al. 1982)
contextuality. Such comparison using the framework as well as similar gaps between Korean and American
allows us to get information about characteristics of each teachers’ mathematical content knowledge (Schmidt et al.
textbook as well as to determine which aspect of non- 2007). Differences in opportunities to learn mathematics
textual elements should be improved. through non-textual elements could be one more explana-
Additionally, even though realistic contexts are useful tion for the gaps. The mean scores of all the aspects in the
for students in learning mathematics, this cannot be an South Korean textbooks are higher than those in the US
excuse for inaccurate, disconnected, or complicated real- textbooks. However, while the South Korean textbooks do
istic contexts being used in textbooks. Many pictorial not differ in terms of accuracy, connectivity, or concise-
representations are used as merely decorational or infor- ness, there are significant variations among the selected US
mative, especially for prime factorization, as reported in textbooks in terms of all the aspects but accuracy. This
this study. Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen (2001) warns that could be also related to opportunities to learn mathematics
only giving realistic contexts which do not have any provided in classrooms. In addition, by looking at different
mathematical connection does not mean that students can approaches of non-textual elements in mathematics text-
do ‘‘mathematization’’ (Freudenthal 1991) in which stu- books across contexts, one can learn from the other to find
dents re-create mathematics by solving problems in real alternative ways to use and improve the quality of non-
life. Reflecting on the results from the study, we need to textual elements in its own mathematics textbooks.
reconsider the quality of non-textual elements that are Lately, in order to attract students’ interest, many
contextualized as well as to find effective ways of mathematics textbooks in both countries have included
enhancing their quality. more non-textual elements than before. However, the
Furthermore, with regard to mathematical conceptuali- impact of such movement on students’ learning remains
zation, we should introduce both contextual and mathe- unknown. Further discussions and policy debates on how to
matical non-textual elements in teaching and learning. develop and use non-textual elements in appropriate ways
Through different approaches, textbooks offer different in textbooks and other curriculum materials should be held.
opportunities to learn mathematical ideas. For example, In addition, further systematic analyses from practice
whereas the South Korean textbooks are likely to use remain to be done, such as how non-textual elements have
abstract mathematical non-textual elements to explain the been used historically in mathematics textbooks, how non-
concept of slope, the US textbooks tend to use contextual textual elements in mathematics textbooks are actually
non-textual elements to explain it. Such different approa- used in classrooms, and how non-textual elements have
ches may provide students in each country with different been used in other topics and textbooks. This study makes a
opportunities to conceptualize the concept. For example, unique contribution to the literature on non-textual ele-
slope can be explained as an algebraic idea that is rate of ments in mathematics education and provides implications
change or as a geometric idea that is steepness (Battista for textbook analysis and curriculum development.

123
186 R. Y. Kim

Acknowledgments This paper evolved out of a part of my doctoral Freudenthal, H. (1991). Revisiting mathematics education: China
dissertation. I would like to thank Punya Mishra, Lynn Paine, Ralph lectures. Dordrecht, NL: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Putnam, Sharon Senk, and Nathalie Sinclair for their useful comments Goldin, G. A., & Shteingold, N. (2001). Systems of representations
and constructive suggestions. I also want to thank Sung Je Cho, and the development of mathematical concepts. In A. A. Cuoco
Gabriele Kaiser, and Oh Nam Kwon for their thoughtful consideration & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), The roles of representation in school
and substantial support. mathematics. Reston, VA: NCTM.
Herbel-Eisenmann, B. A. (2002). Using student contributions and
multiple representations to develop mathematical language.
Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 8(2), 100–105.
References Herman, M. (2007). What students choose to do and have to say about
use of multiple representations in college algebra. Journal of
Abrams, J. P. (2001). Teaching mathematical modeling and the skills Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching, 26(1), 27–54.
of representation. In A. A. Cuoco & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), The Kamens, D. H., Meyer, J. W., & Benavot, A. (1996). Worldwide
roles of representation in school mathematics (pp. 269–282). patterns in academic secondary education curricula. Compara-
Reston, VA: NCTM. tive Education Review, 40(2), 116–138.
Arcavi, A. (2003). The role of visual representations in the learning of Kim, R. Y. (2009a). Mathematics teachers’ pedagogical understand-
mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 52, 215–241. ing of textbook-embedded non-textual elements in South Korea
Battista, M. T. (2007). The development of geometric and spatial and the United States [written in Korean with English abstract].
thinking. In F. K. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on Journal of Korean Teacher Education, 26(3), 199–217.
mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 843–908). Charlotte, Kim, R. Y. (2009b). Pedagogical uses of non-textual elements in
NC: Information Age Publishing. mathematics textbooks: South Korean and United States Cases.
Bazerman, C. (2006). Analyzing the multidimensionality of texts in San Diego, CA: Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
education. In J. L. Green, G. Camilli, & P. B. Elmore (Eds.), American Educational Research Association.
Handbook of complementary methods in education research (pp. Kim, R. Y. (2009c). Text ? book = textbook? Development of a
77–94). New York, NY: Routledge. conceptual framework for non-textual elements in middle school
Bishop, A. J. (1992). Cultural issues in the intended, implemented and mathematics textbooks. Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State Uni-
attained mathematics curriculum. In G. C. Leder (Ed.), Assess- versity, East Lansing, MI.
ment and learning of mathematics (pp. 169–189). Hawthom, Lesh, R., & English, L. D. (2005). Trends in the evolution of models
Victoria: Australian Council for Educational Research. & modeling perspectives on mathematical learning and problem
Blömeke, S., & Paine, L. W. (2008). Getting the fish out of the water: solving. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics
Considering benefits and problems of doing research on teacher Education, 37(6), 487–489.
education at an international level. Teaching and Teacher Levie, W. H., & Lentz, R. (1982). Effects of text illustrations: A
Education, 24(8), 2027–2037. review of research. Educational Communication and Technol-
Boling, E., Eccarius, M., Smith, K., & Frick, T. (2004). Instructional ogy, 30(4), 195–232.
illustrations: Intended meanings and learner interpretations. Levin, J. R., & Mayer, R. E. (1993). Understanding illustrations in
Journal of Visual Literacy, 24(2), 185–204. text. In B. K. Britton, A. Woodward, & M. R. Binkley (Eds.),
Braden, R. A. (1983). Visualizing the verbal and verbalizing the Learning from textbooks: Theory and practice (pp. 95–111).
visual. In R. A. Braden & A. D. Walker (Eds.), Seeing ourselves: Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
visualization in a social context. VA: International Visual Li, Y., Chen, X., & An, S. (2009). Conceptualizing and organizing
Literacy Association, Blacksburg. content for teaching and learning in selected Chinese, Japanese
Brenner, M. E., Herman, S., Ho, H.-Z., & Zimmer, J. M. (1999). and US mathematics textbooks: the case of fraction division.
Cross-national comparison of representational competence. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education,
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30(5), 41(6), 809–826. doi:10.1007/s11858-009-0177-5.
541–557. Mayer, R. E., Sims, V., & Tajika, H. (1995). A comparison of how
Brenner, M. E., Mayer, R. E., Moseley, B., Brar, T., Duran, R., Reed, textbooks teach mathematical problem solving in Japan and the
B. S., et al. (1997). Learning by understanding: The role of United States. American Educational Research Journal, 32(2),
multiple representations in learning algebra. American Educa- 443–460.
tional Research Journal, 34(4), 663–689. Meyer, M. R. (2001). Representation in realistic mathematics
Cuoco, A. A. (2001). The roles of representation in school education. In A. A. Cuoco & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), The roles of
mathematics. Reston, VA: NCTM. representation in school mathematics (pp. 238–250). Reston,
Duval, R. (2006). A cognitive analysis of problems of comprehension VA: NCTM.
in a learning of math. Educational Studies in Mathematics, Ministry of Education Science and Technology. (2011). Mathematics
61(1–2), 103–131. curriculum [in Korean]. Seoul, South Korea: Author.
Dwyer, F. M. (1988). Examining the symbiotic relationship between Misailidou, C., & Williams, J. (2003). Children’s arguments in
verbal and visual literacy in terms of facilitating student discussion of a ‘‘difficult’’ ratio problem: the role of a pictorial
achievement. Reading Psychology, 9, 365–380. representation. Paper presented at the 3rd conference of the
Evans, M. E., Watson, C., & Willows, D. M. (1987). A naturalistic European Society for Research in Mathematics Education,
inquiry into illustrations in instructional textbooks. In H. Bellaria, Italy.
A. Houghton & D. M. Willows (Eds.), The psychology of Mitchelmore, M. C., & White, P. (2000). Development of angle
illustration (Vol. 2, Instructional Issues). New York: Springer. concepts by progressive abstraction and generalisation. Educa-
Fennell, F., & Rowan, T. (2001). Representation: An important tional Studies in Mathematics, 41(3), 209–238.
process for teaching and learning mathematics. Teaching National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and
Children Mathematics, 7(5), 288–292. standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: NCTM.
Ferratti, R. P., & Okolo, C. M. (1996). Authenticity in learning: National Mathematics Advisory Panel. (2008). Foundations for
multimedia design projects in the social studies for students with success: The final report of the National Mathematics Advisory
disabilities. Journal of Learning Disability, 29(5), 450–460. Panel. Washington, DC: US Department of Education.

123
The quality of non-textual elements in mathematics textbooks 187

Pape, S. J., & Tchoshanov, M. A. (2001). The role of representa- Stylianidou, F. (2002). Analysis of science textbook pictures about
tion(s) in developing mathematical understanding. Theory into energy and pupils’ readings of them. International Journal of
Practice, 40(2), 118–127. Science Education, 24(3), 257–283.
Peeck, J. (1993). Increasing picture effects in learning from illustrated Stylianou, D. A. (2010). Teachers’ conceptions of representation in
text. Learning and Instruction, 3, 227–238. middle school mathematics. Journal of Mathematics Teacher
Pettersson, R. (1990). Teachers, students and visuals. Journal of Education, 13(4), 325–343.
Visual Literacy, 10(1), 45–62. Tall, D., & Vinner, S. (1981). Concept image and concept definition
Pettersson, R. (2001). Assessing image contents. In R. E. Griffin, V. in mathematics with particular reference to limits and continuity.
S. Williams & J. Lee (Eds.), Exploring the visual future: Art Educational Studies in Mathematics, 12, 151–169.
design, science, and technology (pp. 233–246). Arizona: Inter- Valverde, G. A., Bianchi, L. J., Wolfe, R. G., Schmidt, W. H., &
national Visual Literacy Association. Houang, R. T. (2002). According to the book: Using TIMSS to
Schmidt, W. H., McKnight, C. C., Houang, R. T., Wang, H., Wiley, investigate the translation of policy into practice through the
D. E., Cogan, L. S., et al. (2001). Why schools matter: A cross- world of textbooks. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
national comparison of curriculum and learning. San Francisco: van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. (2001). Realistic mathematics educa-
Jossey-Bass. tion as work in progress. Taipei, Taiwan: Paper presented at The
Schmidt, W. H., Tatto, M. T., Bankov, K., Blömeke, S., Cedillo, T., Netherlands and Taiwan Conference on Mathematics Education.
Cogan, L. S., et al. (2007). The preparation gap: Teacher Wagener, L. L. (2009). A worthwhile task to teach slope. Mathe-
education for middle school mathematics in six countries (MT21 matics Teaching in the Middle School, 15(3), 168–174.
report). East Lansing, MI: Center for Research in Mathematics Watkins, J. K., Miller, E., & Brubaker, D. (2004). The role of the
and Science Education, Michigan State University. visual image: What are students really learning from pictorial
Sfard, A. (1991). On the dual nature of mathematical conceptions: representations? Journal of Visual Literacy, 24(1), 23–40.
Reflections on processes and objects as different sides of the Wiggins, G. (1993). Assessment: Authenticity, context, and validity.
same coin. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 22, 1–36. Phi Delta Kappan, 75(3), 200–214.
Stigler, J. W. (1990). Mathematical knowledge of Japanese, Chinese, Winn, W. D. (1987). Charts, graphs, and diagrams in educational
and American elementary school children. Reston, VA: National materials. In D. M. Willows & H. A. Houghton (Eds.), The
Council of Teachers of Mathematics. psychology of illustration (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Stigler, J. W., Fuson, K. C., Ham, M., & Kim, M. S. (1986). An Woleck, K. R. (2001). Listen to their pictures: An investigation of
analysis of addition and subtraction word problems in American children’s mathematical drawings. In A. A. Cuoco & F. R. Curcio
and Soviet elementary mathematics textbooks. Cognition and (Eds.), The roles of representation in school mathematics
Instruction, 3(3), 153–171. (pp. 215–227). Reston, VA: NCTM.
Stigler, J. W., Lee, S.-Y., Lucker, G. W., & Stevenson, H. W. (1982). Woodward, A. (1993). Do illustrations serve an instructional purpose in
Curriculum and achievement in mathematics: A study of US textbooks? In B. K. Britton, A. Woodward & M. Binkley (Eds.),
elementary school children in Japan, Taiwan, and the United Learning from textbooks: Theory and practice (pp. 115–134).
States. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74(3), 315–322. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Stump, S. L. (2001). High school precalculus students’ understanding Zazkis, R., & Liljedahl, P. (2004). Understanding primes: The role of
of slope as measure. School Science and Mathematics, 101(2), representation. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
81–89. 35(3), 164–186.

123

You might also like