You are on page 1of 20

Conflict Management Style: Accounting for Cross-National Differences

Author(s): Michael W. Morris, Katherine Y. Williams, Kwok Leung, Richard Larrick, M.


Teresa Mendoza, Deepti Bhatnagar, Jianfeng Li, Mari Kondo, Jin-Lian Luo and Jun-Chen
Hu
Source: Journal of International Business Studies, Vol. 29, No. 4 (4th Qtr., 1998), pp. 729-
747
Published by: Palgrave Macmillan Journals
Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/155407
Accessed: 02-09-2018 22:52 UTC

REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/155407?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Palgrave Macmillan Journals is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend
access to Journal of International Business Studies

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Conflict Management Style: Accounting
for Cross-National Differences

Michael W. Morris, Katherine Y. Williams, Kwok Leun


Richard Larrick, M.Teresa Mendoza, Deepti Bhatnagar, Jianfeng
Mari Kondo, Jin-Lian Luo, Jun-Chen Hu

A problem in joint ventures be- flict style and the cultural values
tween U.S. and Asian firms is that that account for these differences:
cultural differences impede the Chinese managers rely more on an
smooth resolution of conflicts avoiding style because of their
between managers. In a survey of relatively high value on conformity
young managers in the U.S., and tradition. U.S. managers rely
China, Philippines, and India we more on a competing style because
find support for two hypotheses of their relatively high value on
about cultural differences in con- individual achievement.

exchange of signals. Since cultures


A recurring theme in studies of inter-
national business is the idea that have different signalling languages,
problematic misunderstandings arise as negotiators faced with a counterpart
a result of cultural differences in styles from another culture can easily misread
of negotiating and handling conflict a signal or transmit an unintended mes-
(Adler, 1986; Adler & Graham, 1989; sage. The literature suggests that U.S.
Hofstede, 1991; Maddox, 1993). Nego- negotiators struggle with such crossed
tiation can be thought of as a mutual signals not only with counterparts from

Michael W. Morris is an Associate Professor of Organizational Behavior at Stanford


University's Graduate School of Business, and research affiliate of the Institute for Social a
Personality Research at U.C. Berkeley. Katherine Y. Williams is an advanced graduate student
in Organizational Behavior at Stanford University's Graduate School of Business. Kwok
Leung is Professor and Chairman of the Department of Psychology at the Chinese University
of Hong Kong. Richard Larrick is an Associate Professor of behavioral science at the
University of Chicago Graduate School of Business. M. Teresa Mendoza is a faculty assistant
at Stanford University's Graduate School of Business. Deepti Bhatnagar is a Professor of
Organizational Behavior at the Indian Institute of Management, Ahmedabad, India. Jianfeng Li
is a Professor of Organizational Behavior at The People's University of China's College of
Business Administration. Mari Kondo is an Associate Professor of Asian Institute of
Management in Manila. Jin-Lian Luo is an Associate Professor in the School of Economics &
Management, Tongji University, Shanghai, China. Jun-Chen Hu is an Associate Professor in
the School of Management, Fudan University, Shanghai, China.
The authors acknowledge helpful comments from Michael Bond, Roderick Kramer, Joanne
Martin, and Margaret Neale as well as insights from research colloquia at Stanford's Graduate
School of Business and at IPSR at UC Berkeley.

JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS STUDIES, 29, 4 (FOURTH QUARTER 1998): 729-748. 729

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CULTURE AND CONFLICT STYLE

completely unfamiliar cultures, such as several times, but, like so many


indigenous tribal groups, but also with Americans I've known, he's not inter-
counterparts from cultures with which ested in listening when he thinks he's
they have a surface familiarity, such as right. He wants this thing done yes-
Japan (Graham & Sano, 1984), China terday. He has practically screamed
(Pye, 1982) and India (Gopalan & this at me at our last few meetings-
Rivera, 1997). As Pye (1982, pp. 20-23) once in front of a few of my employ-
explained: ees" (Roongrerngsuke and Chansu-
"Unquestionably the largest and pos- thus, in press).
sibly the most intractable category of The many examples of joint ventures
problems in Sino-American business that have run aground on cultural dif-
negotiations can be traced to the cul- ferences have been an impetus for
tural differences between the two research on cultural differences in
societies.... Conscious efforts to take styles of handling conflict with co-
into account the other party's cultural workers. Researchers have shifted from
practices can eliminate gross misun- the method of inductively generalizing
derstandings, but cultural factors con- from qualitative interviews (Pye, 1982)
tinue to surface and cause problems to the method of testing hypotheses
in more subtle and indirect ways." with carefully matched samples of man-
Although cultural differences present agers and quantitative measures
a challenge in a one-time formal negoti- (Graham, 1985). Many studies have
ation, the problem of cultural differ- investigated so-called "East-West differ-
ences is even more endemic in joint ences" by comparing U.S. managers to a
ventures where managers need to matched group in an Asian society.
resolve everyday conflicts with co- Two patterns of findings have been
workers from other cultures (Baird, observed repeatedly, albeit the precise
Lyles, Ji, Wharton, 1990; Miller, Glen, cultural boundaries on these differences
Jaspersen, Karmokolias, 1997). In the are not well understood. First, com-
literature on joint ventures between pared to U.S. managers, Asian managers
U.S. and Asian firms, two types of mis- rely on a style of avoiding explicit dis-
understanding in conflicts are frequent- cussion of the conflict. Second, com-
ly identified. In one type of misunder- pared to Asians, U.S. managers are more
standing, U.S. managers make the error inclined toward a style of assertively
of reading silence from their Asian competing with the other person to see
counterpart as an indication of consent. who can convince the other of their pre-
U.S. managers may fail to pick up on ferred resolution of the conflict.
the indirectly expressed objections of Although many researchers have specu-
Asian colleagues (see Graham and Sano, lated that these behavioral differences
1984). A different type of misunder- reflect underlying differences in cultur-
standing occurs when Asian managers al values (Bond & Hwang, 1986;
make the error of reading an U.S. col- Kirkbride, Tang, & Westwood, 1991),
league's direct adversarial arguments as this has not been rigorously investigat-
indicating unreasonableness and lack of ed. We review the cross-cultural litera-
respect. Consider the reaction of a Thai ture on conflict style and values to
manager to his assertive U.S. colleague: derive more precise predictions. Then
"I've tried to explain all this to Max we compare the conflict management

730 JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS STUDIES

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
MICHAEL W. MORRIS

styles and values of young managers in cal assessments of the five-fold taxono-
the U.S. and three Asian societies to test my as a model of the overall structure of
predictions about the values underlying conflict behavior is mixed (Jehn &
cultural differences in conflict style. Weldon, 1997; Rahim, 1983; Womack,
1988). Nevertheless, the Thomas and
MODELS OF CONFLICT MANAGEMENT Kilmann scales for tapping particular
STYLE conflict styles, such as avoiding and
Researchers in social psychology and competing, compare favorably to other
organizational behavior have proposed methods in terms of validity and relia-
models that reduce the myriad tactics of bility (Brown, Yelsma, & Keller, 1981;
negotiators and managers to several Killman & Thomas, 1977).
basic styles. Early models of strategy in On theoretical grounds, Pruitt and
conflict (Deutsch, 1973) followed the Rubin (1986) have argued that model-
intuitive notion that styles can be ling conflict style in terms of five dispo-
arrayed on a single dimension ranging sitions is redundant. The important
from selfishness (concern about own insight is that low concern for the oppo-
outcomes) to cooperativeness (concern nent occurs with two quite different
about the other party's outcomes). styles: Passively avoiding discussion of
However, a limitation of single-dimen- conflict as opposed to actively collabo-
sion models is that they fail to encom- rating, and competing as opposed to
pass styles that involve high concern for accommodating. These two styles,
both self and other and likewise, styles then, seem particularly likely to under-
that involve a high concern for neither lie friction in a working relationship,
self nor other (e.g., Thomas & Killman, and this may explain why these styles
1974; Pruitt & Rubin, 1986). have been the focus in cross-cultural
Subsequent theorists have drawn on conflict management. To understand
Blake, Shepard and Mouton's (1964) the roots of cultural differences in
taxonomy of managerial styles to model avoiding and competing in conflicts,
conflict styles within a framework of however, we need measures of underly-
two orthogonal motivational dimen- ing values.
sions, a self-oriented and an other-ori-
ented concern (see Thomas & Killman, MODELS OF VALUES
1974; Pruitt & Rubin, 1986). Within this Researchers have taken several ap-
framework, Thomas and Kilmann proaches to conceptualizing and measur-
(1974) developed an instrument for ing values. Most research focuses on
measuring an individual's dispositions individual differences within cultures
toward five discrete styles. We will rather than cross-cultural differences;
focus on two of these, on avoiding (low nevertheless, researchers assume that
self-concern and low other-concern) one's values represent cultural demands
and competing (high self-concern and as well as idiosyncratic goals (Rokeach,
low other-concern). The remaining 1973). Members of the same culture are
styles are, respectively, the polar oppo- likely to share a set of values acquired in
sites of avoiding (collaborating) and of the process of socialization - values that
competing (accommodating) and a represent the acceptable modes of con-
blend of the four foregoing styles (com- duct in a particular society. Furthermore,
promising). The evidence from empiri- a separate research tradition has utilized

VOL. 29, No. 4, FOURTH QUARTER, 1998 731

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CULTURE AND CONFLICT STYLE

values as a way of distinguishing cultures Culture Connection, 1987).


(Kluckhorn & Strodbeck, 1961). These Schwartz (1992, 1994) has attempted to
researchers measure values that are encompass the Western values studied by
equally interpretable, yet differentially Rokeach, as well as values identified in
endorsed, across cultures. non-Western settings, into a multidimen-
The primary method for the study of sional model of the structure of basic
individual differences in values has been human values. With regard to the relia-
inventories of abstract terms. The semi- bility of the measurement instrument and
nal work of Rokeach (1973) measured an the representativeness of the sample
individual's profile on thirtysix terms within and across cultures, this research
that are central to Western discourse on dominates previous work. Schwartz's
values, such as "equality" and "freedom." model begins with respondents' endorse-
By contrast, the most influential cross- ment of value descriptors (such as "obe-
cultural studies have involved more spe- ;dience," "politeness," etc.) which are
cific statements of attitudes and prefer- then clustered into measures of ten val-
ences (Hofstede, 1980). Hofstede (1980) ues, such as "Conformity." These values
compared managers in a large sample of are further aggregated into a few broad
countries on a set of statements of atti- value dimensions, for example,
tudes about work and life, which allowed "Conformity" and "Tradition" make up
him to position the countries on several the "Societal Conservatism" dimension.
dimensions, such as Individualism-Col- "Achievement" and "Power" make up
lectivism. One limitation of this study is the "Self-Enhancement" dimension.
that value scores could be derived only at
the country level rather than at the indi- RELATING VALUES TO CONFLICT
vidual level. Triandis and colleagues STYLE
(Triandis et al., 1986) have developed a
scale to measure Individualism-Col- Is Individualism-Collectivism the
lectivism at the level of individual val- Key?
ues; however, it increasingly appears that Most previous researchers who have
this construct is not coherent at the indi- linked cultural values to conflict style
vidual level, and different components have pointed to the Individualism-Col-
need to be conceptualized separately lectivism dimension. The most explicit
(Triandis, 1995). Another limitation is argument in the previous literature is
that Hofstede's (1980) instruments were the thesis of Ting-Toomey (1988) and
developed in Western countries and then colleagues (Trubisky, Ting-Toomey &
translated. Because of the possibility that Lin, 1991) that country differences in
values not salient in Western societies communication style can be accounted
were omitted from the supposedly uni- for in terms of the Individualism-
versal space of values, researchers in non- Collectivism dimension. Specifically,
Western settings have developed mea- collectivism is associated with indirect
sures that concentrate on the values cen- communication, such as the Avoiding
tral to their traditions. For example, a style of handling conflict, whereas indi-
distinct value dimension that emerged in vidualism is associated with direct
studies of Chinese values, Moral modes of expression, such as the
Discipline, involves self-regulation and Competing style of handling conflict.
attention to role obligations (Chinese, Two predictions follow from an

732 JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS STUDIES

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
MICHAEL W. MORRIS

Individualism-Collectivism (IC) Which Values in Chinese Culture


account. First, measures of Avoiding Lead to Conflict Avoidance?
and Competing should dramatically
A number of theorists have suggested
separate U.S. managers from Asian
that Chinese culture promotes an indi-
managers; for example, in Hofstede's IC
rect, avoiding style of handling conflicts
data, the U.S. score (91) is far higher
(Bond & Wang, 1983). Some studies
than those of Asian societies, which are
have employed conflict style scales to
relatively close together (for example,
test that Chinese managers are more dis-
India=48, Phillipines=32, and
posed to an avoidant style than Western
Taiwan=17). Moreover, Asian patterns
managers. Tang and Kirkbride (1986)
should resemble those in other highly
measured the conflict styles of Hong
collectivist societies, such as Middle
Kong Chinese and British executives in
Eastern and Latin societies. This gener-
the Hong Kong Civil Service, and found
al prediction of similarity across all
that the Chinese managers were higher
highly collectivist cultures is not tested
on the Avoiding style. However, given
in the current study because it has been
that ingroup/outgroup differences influ-
disconfirmed by careful comparative
ence conflict avoidance (Leung, 1988),
studies of conflict style (Graham, 1985);
it is ambiguous whether British culture
we compare across Asia.
or expatriate status was the key to the
A second prediction is that differ-
behavior of this sample of British man-
ences between the countries in conflict
agers. Trubisky, Ting-Toomey and Lin
style should be mediated by individual
(1991) compared Taiwanese and U.S.
differences on measures of Individual-
students and found that Taiwanese par-
ism-Collectivism. Again, the existing
ticipants relied on an indirect avoiding
data is not encouraging: Researchers
style more than U.S. participants. Yet,
who have correlated participants' scores
as Leung (1997) pointed out, this is one
on Individualism-Collectivism scales
of many studies in the literature that
with conflict behaviors have found no
suffers from interpretive difficulties
relationship (Leung, 1988). The prob-
owing to the fact that the responses
lem may be that the Individualism-
were not standardized before making
Collectivism construct conflates a num-
cultural comparisons; higher scores in
ber of distinct values and attitudes and
one culture may thus reflect differing
hence obscures relations between spe-
response sets, such as acquiescence
cific values and social behaviors. The
bias.
reliability of Individualism-Collectivism
What underlies the difference that
scales has proved quite low, and in
Chinese respondents rely on Avoiding
recent years Triandis (1995) and col-
more than comparable groups of U.S.
leagues have shifted from the position
respondents? The evidence clearly sug-
that individualism versus collectivism
gests that not all highly collectivist cul-
is a unitary dimension of values.
tures share this tendency (Graham,
Similarly, our view is that cross-cultural
1985). A clue is suggested by a study
differences in conflict management
comparing conflict styles of Japanese
style cannot be reduced to a single
and U.S. students, which found that
value dimension running from individ-
twice as many Japanese students report-
ualism to collectivism (see review by
ed reliance on avoiding in their most
Morris & Leung, 1999).
recent conflict (Ohbuchi & Takahashi,

VOL. 29, No. 4, FOURTH QUARTER, 1998 733

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CULTURE AND CONFLICT STYLE

1994). One of the most important rea- Which Values in U.S. Culture
sons for avoiding explicit discussion of Lead to Competing in Conflicts?
the conflict for the Japanese students
Now let us review the evidence about
was the desire to preserve their personal
cultural differences in competitive
relationships. Interestingly, though
styles of handling conflict. A robust
both Japanese and U.S. respondents
pattern of findings comes from studies
agreed that avoidance is the least effec-
of choices between dispute resolution
tive strategy for resolving the issues, for
procedures. Leung and colleagues
Japanese it was the preferred style
found that whereas competitive adver-
because they value the conservation of
sarial procedures are preferred by North
existing relationships. Adjusting one-
Americans, less competitive proce-
self to the stable social structure-to
dures, such as mediation, are preferred
relationships, organizations, and insti-
in many other cultural contexts, such as
tutions-is a virtue in Confucian tradi-
Hong Kong and Spain (Leung & Lind,
tion of role-appropriate behavior, which
1986; Leung et al., 1992). Other studies
is a central strain of Chinese culture
have measured participants' choices
also influential in Japanese culture (Su,
between competitive and cooperative
Chiu, Hong, Leung, Peng & Morris,
strategies in conflict games. Li, Cheung
1998). Confucian ethics lays out certain
and Kau (1979) found that U.S. children
"rules of propriety" which structure
rely on competitive strategies to a
interpersonal relationships, and adjust-
greater extent than do matched samples
ment to these prescribed patterns is val-
of children in Hong Kong and Taiwan.
ued. This Confucian virtue was tapped
Although not a cross-national compari-
in studies of Chinese values by the fac-
son, a study by Cox, Lobel and McCleod
tor of Moral Discipline (Chinese Culture
(1991) found that Anglo-Americans
Connection, 1987). Chiu and Kosinski
competed more than African-, Asian-, or
(1994) compared U.S. and Hong Kong
Hispanic-Americans.
Chinese participants in their endorse-
What value orientation might under-
ment of Chinese values and in their
lie the tendency of U.S. managers
conflict management styles. Results
toward a Competing style? One possi-
showed that Chinese respondents were
bility is that competing reflects the
higher on both Moral Discipline and
value-orientation that Parsons (1951)
Conflict Avoidance. This dimension
referred to as an achievement versus
corresponds to Schwartz's value dimen-
ascription-orientation, and McClelland
sion of Societal Conservatism. Drawing
(1961) later operationalized as need for
together these ideas, we hypothesize:
achievement. An achievement orienta-
Hi: Chinese culture fosters an Avoid-
tion means "looking out for number
ing style of conflict management.
one," placing a higher concern for one's
H2: An Avoiding style of conflict man-
own outcome than on the other's out-
agement reflects an individual's orien-
come. Achievement orientation is high
tation toward Societal Conservatism
in societies, such as the United States,
values (e.g., Conformity).
that traditionally permit individual
H3: Country differences in the
social mobility, and low in societies
Avoiding style are mediated by country
such as India where ascribed character-
differences in orientation toward
istics (e.g., caste) determine one's life
Societal Conservatism.
outcomes. Value surveys have long

734 JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS STUDIES

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
MICHAEL W. MORRIS

revealed that U.S. respondents endorse style less inclined toward competing
individual achievement more than that in the than U.S. managers, but this
South and East Asian respondents does not take the form of avoidance that
(Singh, Huang & Thompson, 1962; it takes in Chinese contexts. Similarly
Morris, Podolny & Ariel, 1999). An ori- in the Philippines, where the historical
entation toward achievement and influence of Chinese culture has been
mobility is captured by the Self- moderated by the more recent influence
Enhancement dimension in Schwartz's of Spanish and U.S. cultures, it has
model. Hence, we hypothesize the fol- been noted that managers avoid overt
lowing: competing in conflicts with colleagues,
H4: U.S. culture fosters a Competing but not through avoidance of addressing
style of conflict management. the issues. Rather the tendency is to ex-
H5: A Competing style of conflict man- press one's point indirectly, or to cush-
agement reflects an individual's orien- ion one's statements so as to preserve
tation toward Self-Enhancement values smooth relationships (Gouchenour,
(e.g., Achievement). 1990).
H6: Country differences in the Com- We tested our hypotheses in a compara-
peting style are mediated by country tive survey involving MBA students in
differences in orientation toward Self- four countries. This choice of sample
Enhancement. was designed to satisfy several impor-
tant methodological goals. A first goal
Expectations About Other was to sample enough sites to test our
Countries hypotheses that conflict management
We have proposed hypotheses about styles vary as a function of specific cul-
distinct value dimensions underlying tural traditions as opposed to a very
cultural differences in Avoiding and general Individualism-Collectivism
Competing, which can be contrasted dimension. We compared a U.S. sam-
with previous arguments that cultural ple with Chinese, Indian, and
differences in both conflict styles are a Philippine samples. Our key variables
function of a general Individualism- were scales measuring Avoiding and
Collectivism dimension. To find sup- Competing styles in conflict and mea-
port for our hypotheses it is useful to sures of the Schwartz value dimensions
not only compare U.S. and Chinese relevant to our hypotheses, "Social
managers, but also to observe managers Conservatism" and "Self-Enhance-
in other Asian cultures that, while high- ment." We also analyzed a standard
ly collectivist, have cultural heritages scale of Individualism-Collectivism and
that lead us to expect conflict styles dif- a scale measuring the value dimension
fering from Chinese managers. First let that Schwartz has described as most
us consider India. Observers have similar to Individualism-Collectivism,
argued that Indian managerial conflict which is "Openness to Change."
resolution tendencies reflect Hindu
norms of seeking a solution that pleases METHOD
everyone, as well as British norms of
active, mutual problem solving (Moran Participants
& Stripp, 1991). Hence, we might To compare groups who differ in cul-
expect that Indian managers have a ture yet are relatively similar otherwise,

VOL. 29, No. 4, FOURTH QUARTER, 1998 735

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CULTURE AND CONFLICT STYLE

we sampled students at highly ranked report conflict style scale. This version
masters of business administration involved a rating scale format, which is
(MBA) programs in each country - in important in cross-cultural studies
the United States (Stanford University because it facilitates checking the inter-
and University of Chicago), in China item reliability of the scale, which cannot
(Tong Ji and Fudan Universities), in the be presumed to carry across cultures.
Philippines (Asian Institute of Participants were asked to consider inter-
Management), in India (Indian Institute personal conflicts at work, and rate how
of Management-Ahmedabad) (see well their typical behavior is described
Tripathi, 1996). These students have by a series of 53 statements, such as "I try
relatively similar academic training, to win my position."
work experiences, and career goals. We Next, participants received the 57-
recruited participants in large classes item instrument for measuring value
that comprised a cross-section of the orientations (Schwartz, 1994).
students enrolled in the program, and Respondents were asked to indicate on
participation rates were above 80 per- a 9-point scale ranging from -1 to 7, how
cent in each country. For the sake of important each value was to them per-
clear comparisons, we only analyzed sonally. A score of -1 indicated that the
data from participants who were citi- item was "opposed to my values," a 0
zens of the country where the data was indicated "not important," and 7 indi-
collected. There were 454 participants cated of "supreme importance."
included in the analyses: 132 partici- Finally, we also employed a widely
pants from the United States, 100 from used 18-item scale designed to measure
China, 160 from India, and 62 from the the Individualism-Collectivism dimen-
Philippines. The percentage of females sion of social values (Triandis et al.,
was 28 percent in the United States, 24 1986). The scales were presented in the
percent in China, 11 percent in India, language of instruction of the MBA pro-
and 44 percent the Philippines, respec- gram: English in the United States,
tively. The average age of respondents India, and Philippines, and Mandarin in
varied somewhat in the four countries. China. Scales were translated and back-
In the United States the average age was translated to achieve comparability.
28.69 years. It was 30.05 years in Completing the full survey took partici-
China, 23.31 years in India, and 26.26 pants about 20 minutes.
years in the Philippines. Overall 76
percent of the respondents were male, Scale Construction
and the average age of the respondents A first step in preparing the data was to
was 26.75 years. standardize participants' responses to
each instrument so that response biases
Materials could not enter into the cultural differ-
Participants received a booklet entitled ences. This was done by subtracting
"Managerial Style Inventories" with brief from the raw score for each item the
instructions on the cover and a request mean of all the items on the focal scale,
for demographic information, such as and dividing this by the standard devia-
country of citizenship, age, and gender. tion of items on the scale. Next we exam-
Next appeared Rahim's (1983) adapted ined, within each country, the inter-item
version of the Killman-Thomas self- reliability of the specific factors from the

736 JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS STUDIES

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
MICHAEL W. MORRIS

instruments relevant to our hypotheses. hypotheses using Analysis of Variance


For the conflict style and value factors, (ANOVA) and planned comparisons.
acceptable levels of reliability were Consistent with Hi, Chinese managers
reached. The appendix shows the items relied more on the Avoiding style than
that made up each scale. Table 1 below managers in the other countries (t =
shows Cronbach a reliability scores for 2.68, df = 449, p < .01 one-sided) lead-
Avoiding and Competing scales and the ing to a main effect of Country (F = 3.14,
three Schwartz value dimensions rele- p < .03). Consistent with H2, U.S. man-
vant to hypotheses (Openness to Change, agers relied more on a Competing style
Societal Conservatism, and Self- than managers from the other three
Enhancement). As may be seen in Table countries (t = 1.92, df = 449, p < .05
1, all these scales reached acceptable lev- one-sided), which resulted in a main
els within each country and satisfactory effect of Country (F = 2.60, p < .05).
levels across countries. However, the Table 2 also shows the profile across
Individualism-Collectivism scale did not countries on the Schwartz value dimen-
show an adequate level of reliability (and sions relevant to the hypotheses.
no subset of items could be found that Because the Schwartz instrument com-
improved its performance). Hence, this prehensively covers the semantic space
scale was not used further. of values, it is again appropriate to in-
terpret the standardized scores (shown
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION in bold). Factors with higher standard-
ized scores are those that respondents
Country Differences place above most other values. Overall,
Table 2 indicates the extent to which our MBA respondents endorsed Societal
MBA respondents in the four countries Conservatism less than Self-Enhance-
rely on Avoiding and Competing strate- ment or Openness to Change; however,
gies for managing conflicts. We tested there were strong and readily inter-

TABLE 1
RELIABILITY SCORES FOR CONFLICT AND SCHWARTZ
VALUES SCALES WITHIN EACH COUNTRY

Cronbach's Alpha
United China India Philippines Total
States
Construct

Conflict Style
Avoidant .87 .60 .83 .76 .77
Competitive .78 .75 .73 .83 .77
Schwartz Values
Societal

Conservatism .72 .71 .79 .76 .75


Self-enhancement .80 .80 .84 .83 .82
Openness to Change .77 .74 .77 .87 .79
Individualism/Collectivism .34 .69 .41 .49 .48

VOL. 29, No. 4, FOURTH QUARTER, 1998 737

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CULTURE AND CONFLICT STYLE

pretable country differences. Endorse- nent subscales for Conformity and


ment of Societal Conservatism varied as Tradition values.
a function of Country (F(3, 449) =22.17, Endorsement of the Self-Enhancement
p <.01). Consistent with our expecta- dimension was similar across the four
tion that this factor taps Confucian val- countries. However, the flat profile on
ues, it was relatively high in China and this general dimension masks interesting
the Philippines compared with India patterns on the component subscales for
and especially compared with the Achievement and Power. Achievement
United States (t=6.44, df=449, p<.O1). varied as a function of Country (F(3,
This pattern with the Social Conser- 449) =11.16, p <.01) in the predicted pat-
vatism scale reflected virtually identical tern of greater endorsement by U.S.
profiles across countries on its compo- managers (M=.48) compared with man-

TABLE 2

CONFLICT STYLE AND MAJOR VALUE DIMENSIONS OF


MANAGERS IN 4 COUNTRIES

United China India Philippines


States

CONFLICT STYLE

Avoiding Style
Raw 3.17 (.84) 3.21 (.50) 2.96 (.86) 3.42 (.73)
Standardized -.37 (.61) -.19 (.37) -.39 (.55) -.32 (.53)

Competing Style
Raw 3.75 (.55) 3.45 (.55) 3.39 (.57) 3.82 (.63)
Standardized .12 (.61) .05 (.47) -.06 (.52) .02 (.61)

MAJOR VALUE
DIMENSIONS

Social Conservatism
(conformity, tradition)
Raw 2.74 (.93) 4.27 (.93) 3.64 (1.13) 4.33 (.97)
Standardized -.69 (.40) -.36 (.38) -.43 (.44) -.23 (.37)

Self-enhancement
(power, achievement)
Raw 3.81 (.91) 4.68 (.99) 4.25 (1.09) 4.53 (1.05)
Standardized -.08 (.43) -.03 (.36) -.05 (.48) -.07 ( .38)

Openness to Change
(hedonism,
stimulation,
self-direction)
Raw 4.44 (.77) 4.31 (.94) 4.44 (.89) 4.17 (1.22)
Standardized .33 (.32) -.14 (.31) .14 (.36) -.14 (.31)

N 131 99 160 62

Note. Means and (sds) for raw and standardized variables are shown.
Means of standardized variables are in boldface.

738 JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS STUDIES

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
MICHAEL W. MORRIS

-agers from China (M =.19), India (M at the level of its specific component
=.30) or the Philippines (M =.25; (t=5.46, values, Achievement and Power.
df=449, p<.01). Yet the other compo- Finally, let us turn to the dimension
nent value, Power, revealed an opposite in Schwartz's model closest to Indivi-
pattern (F(3, 449) =49.58, p <.01) in dualism-Collectivism, viz., Openness to
which U.S. managers were lower (M Change. As expected, it varied across
.63) than managers from China (M countries (F(3, 449) =49.58, p <.01) in
.24), India (M =-.39) or the Philippines the pattern of U.S. managers being high-
(M =-.40). This finding resonates with er than the other three groups (t=10.79,
Hofstede's (1980) finding that U.S. df=449, p<.01). This pattern summa-
respondents were lower in Power rizes consistent profiles on the compo-
Distance than those in the other three nent values of Self-Direction, Hedon-
samples. Apparently, U.S. managers ism, and Stimulation. A further detail
believe in trying to get ahead, but they that can be noted at the end of our dis-
are uncomfortable with the notion that cussion of Table 2 concerns the relative
people have privileges once they get size of country differences. Consistent
ahead. Because the components of this with our conceptual model that values
general dimension differ in their profiles come between country and conflict
across countries, it will be important to style, value-orientations differ more dra-
examine relations to conflict style both matically across country than do con-
at the level of the general dimension and flict styles.

TABLE 3
CONFLICT STYLES REGRESSED ON COUNTRY AND MAJOR VALUE DIMENS

Avoiding Competing

Predictors la 2a 3a lb 2b 3b

China .14* .08 -.05 .03


India -.02 -.07 -.15** -.12*
Philippines .04 -.02 -.06 .02

Social
Conservatism .16** .19** -.03 -.00

Self
Enhancement .01 .02 .26** .27**

Openness
To Change -.02 .02 .15** .20**

Adjusted R2 .01 .03 .03 .01 .10 .12

d.f. 449 448 445 449 448 445

F 3.14* 4.82** 3.51** 2.60 18.18** 10.76**

Note: Coefficients are standardized beta weights. The country variables are dummy vari
ables with the United States as the excluded category. All variables are standardized.
* p < .05; **p < .01

VOL. 29, No. 4, FOURTH QUARTER, 1998 739

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CULTURE AND CONFLICT STYLE

Do Values Mediate Conflict Style predict Avoiding. A mediation relation-


Differences? ship is seen in that the country effect is
reduced when values are simultaneous-
To demonstrate that value differences
ly entered, yet the effect of the value
account for the differences in conflict
dimension is undiminished (see
style, several criteria must be met (see
Equation 3a). In sum, results unequivo-
Baron & Kenny, 1986). The putative
cally support H3 that the value dimen-
mediating variable should predict the
sion of Social Conservatism accounts
dependent variable. Moreover, when
for the greater Chinese tendency to
the independent variable and the puta-
avoid conflict. Seeking a more fine-
tive mediating variables are simultane-
grained understanding of the mediating
ously entered into an equation predict-
variable, we conducted parallel analy-
ing the dependent variable, the coeffi-
ses using the specific component values
cient on the independent variable
of Tradition and Conformity, one at a
should be markedly reduced. Whereas
time, and found that either serves to
the coefficient on the mediating variable
completely account for the greater
should be less affected. Our analytic
Chinese tendency to rely on an avoiding
strategy will be to first test whether
strategy in conflicts.
country effects on conflict style are
Now let us consider the Competing
mediated by values in an analysis that
style. As shown in Equation lb, consis-
includes all the relevant value dimen-
tent with the ANOVA results, there is
sions. Then we will try to pinpoint the
an effect of the India dummy variable
values responsible for effects by exam-
on Competing (indicating that U.S.
ining the role of specific component
managers are higher than Indian man-
values of the general value dimensions.
agers on competing). Moreover, consis-
Models with gender and age as controls
tent with H5, the value dimension of
were run first. Gender had no effect,
Self Enhancement predicts a Competing
and age had a slight effect only in the
style (see Equation 2b). A sign of a par-
model for Competing, which did not
tial mediation relation is that the coun-
alter the pattern of inter-relationships
try effect drops by an order of signifi-
between variables of interest. Hence,
cance when the value scores are simul-
these controls are dropped in our fea-
taneously entered in the model (com-
tured analyses.
pare Equation 3b to Equation ib). The
Let us first consider the result of
decrease in the beta coefficients is
regressing the Avoiding style on
small, but it is best appreciated in oppo-
Country dummy variables and value
sition to increase in the beta coefficients
measures. As may be seen in Table 3,
on the value scores. This increase indi-
and specifically in Equation la, there is
cates that their causal relation to the
an effect of the China dummy variable
conflict style is not diminished; it is
on Avoiding (Chinese managers are
instead clarified by the inclusion of the
higher than U.S. managers). Consistent
country dummy variable (compare
with H2, the value-orientation of Social
Equation 3b to Equation 2b).
Conservatism predicts conflict avoid-
To look for more fine-grained rela-
ance (see Equation 2a). By contrast,
tionships, we conducted parallel analy-
Self-Enhancement and Openness to
ses with the components of Self-
Change, which corresponds most close-
Enhancment (Power and Achievement)
ly to Individualism-Collectivism, do not

740 JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS STUDIES

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
MICHAEL W. MORRIS

and of Openness to Change (Hedonism, approach in workplace conflicts.


Self-Direction, and Stimulation) exam- Another general pattern that can be
ined one at a time as possible mediators seen by comparing standardized scores
of the country difference in Competing. in Table 2 is that the country differ-
Not surprisingly given the pattern of ences on value dimensions are sharper
means, Power does not mediate the than the country differences in conflict
country difference at all. Achievement style. This makes sense given that
performs better than the overall mea- individuals are more or less free to
sure, and hence seems to capture the value what they want, but the role
value that partially mediates the coun- requirements of a manager require use
try difference. The component values of all of the different conflict manage-
of Openness to Change do not perform ment strategies. It is consistent with
as well as the overall scores in Table 3. our argument that values are proximally
Hence, we can conclude that compared related to country, and that the influ-
with other values, individual achieve- ence of country on conflict styles arises
ment is most relevant to country differ- through the values into which managers
ences in the competing style of conflict are socialized.
management. Our use of managers in elite MBA
programs raises another important ques-
GENERAL DISCUSSION tion in the literature on cultural differ-
ences in international business, which
Contribution of currentfindings is whether the most cosmopolitan
The current findings make a substan- groups in every country have converged
tial contribution to the research evi- to a common global business culture
dence that conflict management behav- (Barnet & Cavanaugh, 1994). Our Asian
ior differs as a function of cultural val- participants are arguably among the
ues. Using samples that provide a con- most Westernized members of their
servative test of cultural differences, we societies, and yet they still differed
have identified two patterns of differ- quite markedly in their values from the
ences between U.S. and Asian managers U.S. participants. Hence, our data are
in conflict management style. Chinese consistent with the view that even the
managers tend toward an Avoiding most cosmopolitan sectors of these soci-
style, U.S. managers, toward a eties have not completely converged in
Competing style. More importantly, we their values and managerial behaviors.
have provided an analysis of how these Evidence about cultural differences in
differences in managerial behavior style and underlying values, can be of
reflect underlying differences in value- help to managers in joint ventures who
orientations. A Societal Conservatism must interact as colleagues and resolve
value-orientation, tapping values such conflicts with managers from other cul-
as Conformity and Tradition, underlies tures. Although a U.S. manager in
the tendency of Chinese managers to China may find it difficult that col-
avoid explicit negotiation of workplace leagues withhold their critical feedback,
conflicts. An orientation toward Self knowing that he or she should not
Enhancement, and specifically expect direct expression of conflict will
Achievement, underlies the tendency of prevent the error of taking the lack of
U.S. managers to take a competing expressed disagreement as an indication

VOL. 29, No. 4, FOURTH QUARTER, 1998 741

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
of support. Correctly interpreting the ues is likely to be a useful strategy. Yet to
source of this behavior in the Confucian the extent that we want to predict cultur-
values of the accommodating oneself to al differences in responses to particular
the social structure will guide against situations, then, it is likely that theories
erroneous attributions to personal char- will have to shift from reliance on general
acteristics or intentions that can have value constructs to more specific belief
harmful and self-fulfilling effects constructs (for a review, see Morris &
(Morris, Leung & Sethi 1996; Morris, Leung, 1999).
Larrick & Su 1999). Likewise, for Asian
managers, an understanding that a U.S. REFERENCES

manager's competitive style is not based Adler, Nancy J. 1986. International


on a lack of respect for the others in the dimensions of organizational behav-
room, but merely in a value on achieve- ior. Boston, MA: Kent Publishing
ment, may help this behavior to be Company.
accepted without offence. & John L. Graham. 1989. Cross-
cultural interaction: The international
Issues for future research comparison fallacy? Journal of
The current findings lay the ground- International Business Studies, 20(3):
work for future analyses that include 515-37.

more variables. One issue of interest is Argyle, Michael, Kazuo Shmoda & Brian
the role of personality in determining Little. 1978. Variance due to persons
conflict style. Sternberg and colleagues and situations in England and Japan.
(Sternberg & Dobson, 1987) have found British Journal of Social and Clinical
mixed evidence that North American Psychology, 17: 335-37.
college students' conflict styles are pre- Baird, Inga S., Marjorie A. Lyles, Shao-
dicted by personality variables. It is bo Ji & Robert Wharton. 1990. Joint
interesting to consider whether person- venture success: A Sino-U.S. Per-
ality plays an equal role in other coun- spective. International Studies of
tries, given that some studies have Management & Organization, 20(1):
found that social behavior is driven 125-34.

more by personality in the individualis- Barnet, Richard J. & John Cavanaugh.


tic context of the U.K. than in the col- 1994. Global dreams: Imperial corpo-
lectivist context of Japan (Argyle, rations and the new world order.
Shimoda & Little, 1978). New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.
Another important variable to manipu- Baron, Reuben & David A. Kenny. 1986.
late in future studies is the status of the The moderator-mediator variable dis-
other person in the conflict. For exam- tinction in social psychological
ple, the difference in conflict avoidance research: Conceptual, strategic, and
may interact with status, such that man- statistical consideration. Journal of
agers who show the most deference to a Personality and Social Psychology,
superordinate will also demand the most 51(6): 1173-82.
deference from a subordinate. In predict- Blake, Robert R., Herbert Shepard &
ing general styles of conflict management Jane S. Mouton. 1964. Managing
in recurrent roles, the current strategy of intergroup conflict in industry.
adducing these behavioral style differ- Houston, TX: Gulf Publishing.
ences to fairly general differences in val- Bond, Michael H. 1979. Winning either

742 JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS STUDIES

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
MICHAEL W. MORRIS

way: The effect of anticipating a com- Goldenberg, Susan. 1988. Hands across
petitive interaction on person percep- the ocean: Managing joint ventures
tion. Personality and Social with a spotlight on China and Japan.
Psychology Bulletin, 5(3): 316-19. Boston, MA: Harvard University
& Kwang-kuo Hwang. 1986. Press.
The social psychology of Chinese Gopalan, Suresh & Joan B. Rivera. 1997.
people. In M. H. Bond, editor, The Gaining a perspective on Indian value
psychology of the Chinese people. orientations: Implications for expatri-
New York, NY: Oxford University ate managers. International Journal of
Press. Organizational Analysis, 5(2): 156-
_ & Sung-Hsing Wang. 1983. 179.
Aggressive behavior in Chinese soci- Gouchenour, Theodore. 1990. Con-
ety: The problem of maintaining order sidering Filipinos. Yarmouth:
and harmony. In A. P. Goldstein & M. International Press.
Segall, editors, Global perspectives on Graham, John. 1985. The influence of
aggression. New York, NY: Pergamon. culture on business negotiations.
Brown, Charles T., Paul Yelsma & Paul Journal of International Business
W. Keller. 1981. Communication-con- Studies, 16(1): 81-96.
flict predisposition: Development of a & Yoshihiro Sano. 1984.
theory and an instrument. Human Smart bargaining: Doing business
Relations, 34(12): 1103-17. with the Japanese. Cambridge:
Chinese Culture Connection. 1987. Ballinger Publishing Co.
Chinese values and the search for cul- Hofstede, Geert H. 1980. Culture's con-
ture-free dimensions of culture. sequences: International differences
Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, in work-related values. Beverly Hills:
18(2): 143-64. Sage Publications.
Chiu, Randy K. & Frederick A. Kosinski, 1991. Cultures and organiza-
Jr. 1994. Is Chinese conflict-handling tions: Software of the mind. London,
behavior influenced by Chinese val- UK: McGraw-Hill.
ues? Social Behavior & Personality, Hui, C. Harry & Harry C. Triandis. 1985.
22(1): 81-90. Measurement in cross-cultural psy-
Cox, Taylor, Sharon Lobel & Poppy chology: A review and comparison of
McLeod. 1991. Effects of ethnic group strategies. Journal of Cross-Cultural
cultural differences on cooperative Psychology, 16(2): 131-52.
and competitive behavior on a group Jehn, Karen & Elizabeth Weldon. 1997.
task. Academy of Management Managerial attitudes toward conflict:
Journal, 34(4): 827-47. Cross-cultural differences in resolu-
Davis, Herbert J. & Anvaar S. Rasool. tion styles. Journal of International
1988. Values research and managerial Management, 34: 102-24.
behavior: Implications for devising Kilmann, Ralon H. & Kenneth W.
cultural consistent managerial styles. Thomas. 1977. Developing a forced-
Management International Review, choice measure of conflict-handling
28(3): 11-20. behavior: The "MODE" instrument.
Deutsch, Morton. 1973. The resolution Education and Psychological
of conflict. New Haven, CT: Yale Measurement, 37(2): 309-25.
University Press. Kirkbride, Paul S., Sarah Tang & Robert

VOL. 29, No. 4, FOURTH QUARTER, 1998 743

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CULTURE AND CONFLICT STYLE

I. Westwood. 1991. Chinese conflict ing society. New York, NY: Free Press,
preferences and negotiation behavior: London: Collier-Macmillan Limited.
Cultural and psychological influ- Miller, Robert, Jack Glen, Fred Jas-
ences. Organization Studies, 12(3): persen, & Yannis Karmokolias. 1997.
365-86. International joint venture in devel-
Kluckhorn, Florence & Fred Strodbeck. oping countries. Finance & Develop-
1961. Variations in value orienta- ment, 34(1): 26-9.
tions. Evanston, IL: Peterson Row. Moran, Robert T. & William G. Stripp.
Leung, Kwok. 1988. Some determinants 1991. Dynamics of successful interna-
of conflict avoidance. Journal of tional business negotiations. Houston,
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 19(1): 125- TX: Gulf Publishing.
36. Morris, Charles W. 1956. Varieties of
. 1997. Negotiation and human value. Chicago, IL: University
reward allocations across cultures. In of Chicago Press.
P. Earley & M. Erez, editors, New per- Morris, Michael W., Richard Larrick &
spectives on international industri- Steven Su. 1999. Misperceiving nego-
al/organizational psychology. San tiation counterparts: Ascribing per-
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. sonality traits for situationally deter-
, Yuk-fai Au, Jose M. mined bargaining behaviors. Journal
Fernandez-Dols & Saburo Iwawaki. of Personality and Social Psychology
1992. Preference for methods of con- (forthcoming).
flict processing in two collectivist , Kwok Leung & Sheena Sethi.
cultures. International Journal of 1996. Person perception in the heat of
Psychology, 27(2): 195-209. conflict: Attributions about the oppo-
& Michael Bond. 1989. On the nent and conflict resolution in two
empirical identification of dimen- cultures. Stanford University Re-
sions for cross-cultural comparisons. search Paper No. 1360, Stanford.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, & Kwok Leung. 1999. Justice
20(2): 133-51. for all? Understanding cultural influ-
& Allan E. Lind. 1986. Pro- ences on judgments of outcome and
cedural justice and culture: Effects of process fairness. Applied Psychology:
culture, gender, and investigator sta- An International Review (forthcom-
tus on procedural preferences. Journal ing).
of Personality and Social Psychology, J. Podolny & S. Ariel. 1999.
50(6): 1134-40. Missing relations: Incorporating rela-
Li, Mei-chih, See-fat Cheung & Shwu- tional constructs into models of cul-
ming Kau. 1979. Competitive and ture. New Approaches to Internation-
cooperative behavior of Chinese chil- al Differences in Organizational
dren in Taiwan and Hong Kong. Acta Behavior (forthcoming).
Psychologica Taiwanica, 21(1): 27-33. Ohbuchi, Ken-Ichi & Yumi Takahashi.
Maddox, Robert C. 1993. Cross - cultur- 1994. Cultural styles of conflict man-
al problems in international business: agement in Japanese and Americans:
The role of the cultural integration Passivity, covertness, and effective-
function. Westport, CT: Quorum ness of strategies. Journal of Applied
Books. Social Psychology, 24(15): 1345-66.
McClelland, David C. 1961. The achiev- Parkhe, Arvind. 1996. Messy research,

744 JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS STUDIES

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
MICHAEL W. MORRIS

methodological predispositions, and editor, Advances in experimental


theory development in international social psychology. New York, NY:
joint ventures. Academy of Manage- Academic Press.
ment Review, 18(2): 227-68. 1994. Beyond individualism/
Parsons, Talcott. 1951. The social sys- collectivism: New cultural dimen-
tem. New York, NY: Free Press. sions of values. In U. Kim, H. C.
Pruitt, Dean G. & Jeffrey Z. Rubin. 1986. Triandis, C. Kacitcibasi, S. C. Choi &
Social conflict: Escalation, stalemate, G. Yoon, editors, Individualism and
and settlement. New York, NY: collectivism: Theory, methods, and
Random House. applications. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Pye, Lucian. 1982. Chinese commercial Sage.
negotiating style. Cambridge: Shenkar, Oded & Simcha Ronen. 1987.
Oelgeschlager, Gunn & Hain. The cultural context of negotiations:
Rahim, M. Afzalur. 1983. A measure of The implications of Chinese interper-
styles of handling interpersonal con- sonal norms. Journal of Applied
flict. Academy of Management Behavioral Science, 23 (2): 263-75.
Journal, 26(2): 368-76. Singh, Paras N., Sophia C. Huang &
Ralston, David A., David H. Holt, Robert George G. Thompson. 1962. A com-
H. Terpstra & Yu Kai-Cheng. 1997. parative study of selected attitudes,
The impact of national cultural and values, and personality characteristics
economic ideology on managerial of American, Chinese, and Indian stu-
work values: A study of the United dents. The Journal of Social
States, Russia, Japan and China. Psychology, 57(1): 123-32.
Journal of International Business Sternberg, Robert J. & Diane M. Dobson.
Studies, 28(1): 177-207. 1987. Resolving interpersonal con-
Rokeach, Milton. 1973. The nature of flicts: An analysis of stylistic consis-
human values. New York, NY: Free tency. Journal of Personality and
Press. Social Psychology, 52(4): 794-812.
Roongrerngsuke, Siriyupa & Daryl Su, Steve K., Chi-yue Chiu, Ying-yi
Chansuthus. In press. Conflict Hong, Kwok Leung, Kaiping Peng &
Management in Thailand. In K. Leung Michael W. Morris. Self organization
& D. W. Tjosvold, editors, Conflict and social organization: American
management in the Asia Pacific. and Chinese constructions. In T. R.
Singapore: Wiley. Tyler, R. M. Kramer and 0. P. John,
Schnepp, Otto, Mary Anne Von Glinow editors, Psychology of the Social Self.
& Arvind Bhambri. 1990. United In press.
States - China technology transfer. Tang, Sara & Paul Kirkbride. 1986.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Development of conflict management
Schuster, Camille P. & Michael skills in Hong Kong: An analysis of
Copeland. 1996. Global business, some cross-cultural implications.
planning for sales and negotiations. Management Education and
Fort Worth, TX: Dryden. Development, 17(3): 287-301.
Schwartz, Shalom H. 1992. Universals Thomas, Kenneth W. & Ralph H.
in the content and structure of values: Kilmann. 1974. The Thomas-Kilmann
Theoretical advances and empirical mode instrument. New York, NY:
tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna, Xicom.

VOL. 29, No. 4, FOURTH QUARTER, 1998 745

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CULTURE AND CONFLICT STYLE

Ting-Toomey, Stella. 1985. Toward a


theory of conflict and culture. In W.
B. Gudykunst, I. P. Stewart and S.
Ting-Toomey, editors, International
and intercultural communication
annual: Vol 9. Communication, cul-
ture and organizational processes.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Ge Gao, Paula Trubisky, Zhiz-
hong Yang, H. S. Kim, Sungl-ling Lin
& T. Nishida. 1991. Culture, face
maintenance, and styles of handling
interpersonal conflict: A study in five
cultures. The International Journal of
Conflict Management, 2(4): 275-96.
Triandis, Harry C. 1995. Individualism
and collectivism. Boulder, CO:
Westview Press.
______ Robert Bontempo, Hector
Betancourt, Michael Bond, Kwok
Leung, Abelando Brenes, James
Georgas, C. Harry Hui, Gerardo Marin,
Bernadette Setiadi, Jai B. P. Sinha,
Jyoti Verma, John Spangenberg,
Hubert Touzard & Germaine de
Montmollin. 1986. The measurement
of the etic aspects of individualism
and collectivism across cultures.
Australian Journal of Psychology,
38(3): 257-67.
Tripathi, Salil. 1996. Who needs
Stanford? Asia's business schools
can't match the West's best MBA pro-
grams, but the product is improving
and some prefer the Asianized cur-
riculum. Asia, Inc. September: 26-33.
Trubisky, Paula, Stella Ting-Toomey &
Sung-ling Lin. 1991. The influence of
Individualism-Collectivism and self-
monitoring on conflict styles.
International Journal of Intercultural
Relations, 15(1): 65-84.
Womack, Deanna F. 1988. Assessing the
Thomas-Kilmann conflict mode sur-
vey. Management Communication
Quarterly, 1(3): 321-49.

746 JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS STUDIES

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
MICHAEL W. MORRIS

APPENDIX

Scales Measuring Avoiding and Competing Styles of Managing Conflict and Schwartz Value
Factors. Component Value Scale Reliabilities Indicated by Cronach Alpha Statistics.

Conflict Style: AVOIDING


1. I attempt to avoid being "put on the spot" and try to keep my conflict with others to
myself.
2. I usually avoid open discussion of my differences with the other person.
3. I generally avoid an argument.
4. I try to stay away from disagreement with the other person.
5. I avoid an encounter with others.
6. I try to keep my disagreement with others to myself in order to avoid hard feelings.
7. I try to avoid unpleasant exchanges.
8. I sometimes avoid taking positions which would create controversy.
9. I try to do what is necessary to avoid useless tensions.
10.I feel that differences are not always worth worrying about.*
l1.There are times when I let others take responsibility for solving problems.*

Conflict Style: COMPETING


1. I usually hold on to my solution to a problem.
2. I use my influence to get my ideas accepted.
3. I use my authority to make a decision in my favor.
4. I argue my case to show the merits of my position.
5. I am generally firm in pursuing my side of the issue.
6. I sometimes use my power to win a competitive situation.
7. I try to win my position.
8. I am usually firm in pursuing my goals.
9. I try to show others the logic and benefits of my position.
10.1 assert my wishes.

*Indicates item dropped from scale due to low correlations with other items.

Value Factor: SOCIETAL CONSERVATISM


Component Value: Conformity (cx = .66):
self-discipline, politeness, honoring of parents and elders

Component Value: Tradition (cx = .55):


accepting of my portion in life, moderate, respect for tradition, devout, humble

Value Factor: SELF ENHANCEMENT


Component Value: Power (cx = .75):
preserving my public image, social recognition, authority, wealth, social power

Component Value: Achievement (<x = .67):


ambitious, influential, successful, capable, intelligent

Value Factor: OPENNESS TO CHANGE


Component Value: Hedonism (a- = .75):
pleasure, enjoying life, self-indulgent

Component Value: Self-Direction (<x = .63):


self-respect, creativity, choosing own goals, curious, independent, freedom

Component Value: Stimulation (a- = .71):


a varied life, an exciting life, daring

VOL. 29, NO. 4, FOURTH QUARTER, 1998 747

This content downloaded from 200.89.140.134 on Sun, 02 Sep 2018 22:52:23 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like