You are on page 1of 10

Psychological Medicine (2009), 39, 1607–1616.

f Cambridge University Press 2009 O R I G IN AL A R T IC L E


doi:10.1017/S0033291709005522 Printed in the United Kingdom

The acute effects of synthetic intravenous


D9-tetrahydrocannabinol on psychosis, mood
and cognitive functioning

P. D. Morrison1*, V. Zois1, D. A. McKeown2, T. D. Lee2, D. W. Holt2, J. F. Powell1, S. Kapur1


and R. M. Murray1
1
Institute of Psychiatry, The Biomedical Research Centre, King’s College London, UK
2
Analytical Unit, St George’s, University of London, UK

Background. Recent work suggests that heavy use of cannabis is associated with an increased risk of schizophrenia-
like psychosis. However, there is a dearth of experimental studies of the effects of the constituents of cannabis, such
as D9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC). In a study of intravenous (i.v.) synthetic THC in healthy humans, we aimed to
study the relationship of the psychotic symptoms induced by THC to the consequent anxiety and neuropsychological
impairment.

Method. Twenty-two healthy adult males aged 28¡6 years (mean¡S.D.) participated in experimental sessions in
which i.v. THC (2.5 mg) was administered under double-blind, placebo-controlled conditions. Self-rated and in-
vestigator-rated measurements of mood and psychosis [the University of Wales Institute of Science and Technology
Mood Adjective Checklist (UMACL), the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) and the Community
Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE)] were made at baseline and at 30, 80 and 120 min post-injection.
Participants also completed a series of neuropsychological tests [the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task (RAVLT),
Digit Span, Verbal Fluency and the Baddeley Reasoning Task] within 45 min of injection.

Results. THC-induced positive psychotic symptoms, and participant- and investigator-rated measurements of these
were highly correlated. Participants showed an increase in anxiety ratings but there was no relationship between
either self- or investigator-rated positive psychotic symptoms and anxiety. THC also impaired neuropsychological
performance but once again there was no relationship between THC-induced positive psychotic symptoms and
deficits in working memory/executive function.

Conclusions. These findings confirm that THC can induce a transient, acute psychotic reaction in psychiatrically
well individuals. The extent of the psychotic reaction was not related to the degree of anxiety or cognitive impair-
ment.

Received 30 September 2008 ; Revised 4 February 2009 ; Accepted 8 February 2009 ; First published online 1 April 2009

Key words : Psychosis, schizophrenia, D9-tetrahydrocannabinol.

Introduction 2007), and those patients who continue to use cannabis


exhibit earlier and more frequent relapses compared
Since the nineteenth century there have been descrip-
to non-users (Linszen et al. 1994 ; Caspari, 1999 ; Grech
tions of acute schizophrenia-like psychotic symptoms
et al. 2005). Furthermore, there is now considerable
in healthy subjects who had taken cannabis (Moreau
evidence that regular cannabis use is associated with
de Tours, 1845 ; Ames, 1958 ; Talbott & Teague, 1969 ;
an increased risk of schizophrenic illness (Arseneault
Chopra & Smith, 1974). The prevalence of cannabis
et al. 2004 ; Barnes et al. 2006 ; Hall, 2006 ; Moore et al.
use in psychotic patients presenting to psychiatric
2007 ; Murray et al. 2007).
services for the first time is approximately double that
It is clear that the major psychoactive properties
found in the general population (Hambrecht &
of cannabis are attributable to the molecule D9-
Hafner, 2000 ; Van Mastrigt et al. 2004 ; Barnett et al.
tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), which acts as a partial
agonist at cannabinoid CB1 receptors in the brain
(Mechoulam et al. 1970 ; Pertwee, 2006). Several recent
* Address for correspondence : Dr P. D. Morrison, Institute of
studies have investigated the acute pharmacological
Psychiatry, The Biomedical Research Centre, King’s College London,
De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK. effects of marijuana, plant-derived THC extracts or
(Email : paul.morrison@iop.kcl.ac.uk) synthetic THC under laboratory conditions, either in
1608 P. D. Morrison et al.

healthy controls (Curran et al. 2002 ; D’Souza et al. THC-elicited anxiety. Finally, we wanted to further
2004 ; Henquet et al. 2006 ; Koethe et al. 2006 ; characterize the effect of THC on cognitive function
Ramaekers et al. 2006 ; Juckel et al. 2007) or in schizo- and the relationship between psychotic symptoms and
phrenic patients (D’Souza et al. 2005), using a variety cognitive impairment. Deficits in cognitive function
of drug administration routes. are regarded as a potential intermediate phenotype
Studies using smoked marijuana, although highly in schizophrenia and predict outcome (reviewed in
informative of herbal drug effects, are compromised Solowij & Michie, 2007). We hypothesized that cogni-
by the presence of at least 60 other cannabinoid mol- tive performance would be poorer under THC com-
ecules, some of which are CB1 receptor antagonists pared to placebo conditions.
(Pertwee, 2006, 2008 ; Ryan et al. 2006). Orally admin-
istered cannabinoids have the disadvantage of poor,
irregular absorption and marked inter-individual vari- Method
ation in plasma concentrations (Grotenhermen, 2003). The study was carried out at the Institute of Psychi-
By contrast, intravenous (i.v.) administration of syn- atry following approval by the South London and
thetic THC offers a rapid and reliable delivery method Maudsley (SLAM) Regional Ethics Committee.
with low inter-individual variation in bioavailability
and a pharmacokinetic time-course that mirrors that of
smoked marijuana (Ohlsson et al. 1980 ; Grotenhermen, Design
2003 ; Naef et al. 2004). Previous studies of the effects The psychological properties of i.v. THC (2.5 mg) were
of IV THC have used doses ranging from 2 to 5 mg, investigated in an experimental study using a within-
which approximate the levels of THC from smoking subject, double-blind, placebo-controlled design. Par-
a standard cannabis ‘ joint ’ containing 1–3.5 % THC ticipants attended two experimental sessions, at least
(16–34 mg) (Ohlsson et al. 1980 ; Volkow et al. 1991 ; 2 weeks apart, in which either THC or placebo was
D’Souza et al. 2004, 2005). Using this route, it was given in a random, counterbalanced order.
shown that THC elicited psychotic symptoms and
cognitive impairments in a proportion of healthy sub-
Participants
jects (D’Souza et al. 2004) and worsened psychotic
symptoms in schizophrenic patients (D’Souza et al. Healthy male participants were recruited by way of
2005). This work used the Positive and Negative Syn- email advertisements delivered to the staff and stu-
drome Scale (PANSS ; Kay et al. 1989), an investigator- dents of King’s College London. Prospective partici-
rated scale, to measure THC psychosis (D’Souza et al. pants, assessed by a trained psychiatrist at baseline,
2004). were required to be aged between 21 and 50 years ; to
Although the work by D’Souza and colleagues have previously taken cannabis on at least one oc-
shows that THC induces an elevation of PANSS posi- casion ; to score <15 on the General Health Ques-
tive scores, it is to be recognized that the PANSS is a tionnaire (GHQ-12 ; Goldberg & Blackwell, 1970) ; and
scale developed and validated in patients with chronic be willing to provide written informed consent. Ex-
psychosis, is rated by the observer, and was not de- clusion criteria were a history of mental illness, sub-
signed for immediate repeated administration (i.e. re- stance dependence (excluding nicotine), current or
peatedly within a few hours). Thus, we thought that past severe medical disorders or a history of major
it would be informative to obtain the subjective per- mental illness in a first-degree family member. Al-
spective on the THC-induced experience by adminis- cohol and drug dependence were excluded using the
tering a subjective scale previously used in healthy Michigan Alcohol Screening Test (MAST ; Selzer, 1971)
subjects : the Community Assessment of Psychic Ex- and the Drug Abuse Screening Test (DAST ; Skinner,
periences – state version (CAPE-state ; Henquet et al. 1982). The nature and risks of the study were dis-
2006). Furthermore, to fully explore the phenomen- cussed. Participants were alerted to the possibility of
ology of THC-induced acute psychosis as a possible transient anxiety and psychosis following THC and
‘ model ’ for schizophrenia, we thought it crucial that were made aware that they would be able to stop the
its effects on mood and anxiety be explored con- experimental sessions at any stage without sanction
currently ; therefore, we included a validated struc- and that rescue medication (lorazepam tablets 1–4 mg)
tured instrument that is known to be sensitive to would be available. Participants were asked to avoid
change : the University of Wales Institute of Science alcohol and drugs for 24 h before, and to abstain from
and Technology Mood Adjective Checklist (UMACL ; driving for 24 h after, experimental sessions. Partici-
Matthews et al. 1990). We hypothesized that i.v. pants were followed up by telephone the following
THC would be anxiogenic and aimed to explore day and a small monetary reimbursement was made
the relationship between THC-elicited psychosis and at the end of their involvement in the study.
Acute effects of THC 1609

Pharmaceuticals models) and fits better with experimental data


(Matthews et al. 1990 ; Schimmack & Grob, 2000). For
Dronabinol (THC) was supplied by THC Pharm
each dimension, subjects rated their level of agreement
GmbH (Frankfurt am Main, Germany) and prepared
with four emotionally positive and four emotionally
as (1 mg/ml) vials for i.v. injection by Bichsel Lab-
negative adjectives. The total score in each dimension
oratories (Interlaken, Switzerland) according to the
was calculated by subtracting negatively valenced
method of Naef et al. (2004). Placebo and active vials
from positively valenced items. For each subject the
were identical in composition (except for THC) and
total score in each dimension at baseline, 30, 80 and
identical in appearance. After dilution in normal sa-
120 min post-injection was a value between x12
line, preparations for injection contained 2.5 % (v/v)
and +12.
absolute ethanol.

Cognitive testing
Experimental sessions
Cognitive assessments began 10 min post-injection.
A psychiatrist assessed participants and a brief physi-
The order of tests was consistent across both sessions :
cal examination was conducted at the beginning of
(1) the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task (RAVLT)-
each session. Urine was taken for drug testing. Sterile
immediate recall ; (2) Digit Span ; (3) Verbal Fluency ;
cannulae (20G) were inserted into veins in the ante-
(4) RAVLT-20 min recall ; and (5) the Baddeley Rea-
cubital fossa of both arms, one for administration of
soning Task. Cognitive testing was complete within
pharmaceuticals and one for blood sampling. Partici-
45 min post-injection.
pants were asked to sit comfortably on a clinical couch
and given free access to water. Pharmaceuticals were
RAVLT
administered in pulses at 1 ml/min over 5 min (total
5 ml). The standard administration format of the RAVLT
was used. Immediate recall of a 15-word list was as-
Outcome measures sessed over five trials. Two different, but equivalent,
word lists were used. Free recall was assessed follow-
Psychological assessments and self-rated scales were
ing a 20-min delay. (The interference component, List
administered at baseline (30 min prior to injection)
B, was included but the data are not reported here.)
and at 30, 80 and 120 min following the final injected
pulse. Participants were instructed to complete rating
Digit Span
scales as applied to the present moment. A trained
psychiatrist conducted all assessments. The Digit Span task evaluates the capacity of attention
and working memory. Participants were tested in their
Positive psychotic symptoms immediate recall of a sequence of digits ; and given
two attempts at each level of difficulty. In the reverse
Momentary positive psychotic symptoms were as-
digit span condition, participants were required to re-
sessed using the positive subscale of the PANSS and
call the sequence in the reverse order.
the CAPE-state. The CAPE-state is a validated 42-item
self-reported questionnaire, derived from the Peters
Verbal fluency
Delusions Inventory (PDI), which generates a positive,
a negative and a depressive dimension score (Henquet The verbal fluency task evaluates speed of retrieval
et al. 2006). In the current study, the CAPE-state fre- from semantic memory. Participants were given 60 s
quency score (0=never, 1=sometimes, 2=often, 3= to generate words beginning with a specific letter of
nearly always) was collapsed to a yes/no response the alphabet and instructed that proper nouns and
option. plurals did not score. The mean number of words
generated from four trials was recorded as the total
Affect score.

The UMACL was used to assess changes in affect.


The Baddeley Reasoning Task
Three dimensions of affect are measured : hedonic
tone, energetic arousal and tense arousal (Matthews Participants were given 3 min in which to verify the
et al. 1990). It has been suggested that a three- truth of 32 logical statements containing one of four
dimensional model of affect with separate pleasure– grammatical constructs such as : ‘ B does not precede
displeasure, awake–tiredness and tension–relaxation A … AB ’. The highest obtainable score was 32. This
dimensions provides a more informative description task evaluates the performance of the central execu-
of core affect (compared to one- and two-dimensional tive.
1610 P. D. Morrison et al.

Pharmacokinetic analysis (a)

Blood (5 ml) was collected at baseline and at 1, 5, 15,


14

PANSS positive subscale


30, 60 and 120 min post-injection. Immediately af-
ter collection, samples were stored at 2–5 xC, prior 12
to being centrifuged. Plasma was stored at x70 xC
until analysis. The samples were extracted using 10
solid phase extraction (SPE), then THC and its pri-
mary metabolite 11-OH-THC were derivatized and 8
measured using gas chromatography with mass-
spectrophotometric detection (GC–MS) as described 6
previously (Steinmeyer et al. 2002).
0 30 80 120
Time (min)
Statistical analyses
(b)
All analyses were performed in SPSS version 15.0
5
(SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Data were assessed for
normality using Kolmogorov–Smirnov test statistics.

CAPE positive subscale


Because of absence of variance in positive psychotic 4
scores under placebo conditions, Friedman’s test,
a non-parametric repeated-measures test, was used 3
to compare positive psychotic scores under THC
and placebo conditions. Two-way repeated-measures 2
ANOVA was used to compare immediate recall in the
RAVLT, with Trial (1–5) and Treatment (placebo ver- 1
sus THC) as within-subject factors. The Greenhouse–
Geisser corrected F ratio was used because there was 0
0 30 80 120
a violation of sphericity. For the remaining cognitive
Time (min)
tasks and the UMACL, differences between placebo
and THC were compared using paired t tests. Re- Fig. 1. Mean positive psychotic symptoms as measured by
lationships between normally distributed variables (a) the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) and
were analysed using Pearson’s product moment cor- (b) the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences
relation. Relationships between non-parametric vari- (CAPE) scale following intravenous D9-tetrahydrocannabinol
(THC) (2.5 mg). Error bars show ¡95 % confidence interval.
ables were tested using Spearman’s rank correlation
coefficient. Significance was accepted at p values

– –, Placebo ; –&–, THC.

<0.05. All comparisons were two-tailed.


participate in cognitive testing or to complete
self-rated scales under THC ; and one participant ex-
Results perienced profound anxiety during the THC arm and
requested ‘ rescue medication ’ (lorazepam 3 mg) ; sym-
Of 24 volunteers, 22 attended both sessions. Partici-
ptoms resolved completely within 30 min.
pants were aged 28¡6 years (mean¡S.D.). All partici-
pants were Caucasian. Estimated lifetime exposure to
THC-induced positive psychotic symptoms
cannabis ranged from 2 to about 1000 episodes. Self-
reported last previous use of cannabis ranged from Scores on the PANSS positive subscale were increased
12 h to 10 years (mean¡S.D.=2¡3.3 years). In urine from baseline following THC but not placebo admin-
drug screens, one participant tested positive for THC, istration (Friedman’s x2=62, df=7, n=21, p<0.001).
but this was the case prior to both experimental At 30 min post-THC, PANSS positive scores had in-
sessions, thus the data were included in the main ana- creased by a mean of 3.7 points (range 0–17), returning
lyses. No participants tested positive for other com- to baseline levels by 120 min (Fig. 1 a). Similarly, sub-
mon drugs of abuse (opiates, cocaine, amphetamines, ject-rated positive psychotic symptoms as measured
methamphetamine, methadone, benzodiazepines) at by the CAPE-state increased from baseline following
either session. THC but not placebo (Friedman’s x2=20, df=7, n=17,
Of the 22 participants, three subjects were unable to p=0.005). By 80 min post-injection, CAPE-state scores
complete all parts of the protocol. One experienced had returned to baseline (Fig. 1 b). Investigator-rated
nausea during the THC arm ; another refused to (PANSS) and subject-rated (CAPE-state) positive
Acute effects of THC 1611

psychotic scores at 30 and 80 min post-THC admin- (a)


istration were correlated (Spearman’s r=0.62, p<
10
0.001). Participants who had taken cannabis more ex-
tensively in the past were less likely to exhibit positive
5

Hedonic tone
psychotic symptoms as rated by the PANSS under
THC conditions at 30 min post-injection (Spearman’s
0
r=x0.45, p<0.05).

The impact of THC on core affect –5

Hedonic tone
–10
IV THC elicited transient feelings of dysphoria. Under
0 30 80 120
THC conditions, self-rated hedonic tone as measured
by the UMACL was decreased compared to placebo Time (min)
at 30 min post-injection [THC, mean=4.3, 95 % confi- (b)
dence interval (CI) 2.9–5.8 ; placebo, mean=8.7, 95 %
10
CI 7.3–10.1, t(17)=3.24, p=0.005] and at 80 min post-
injection [THC, mean=7.2, 95 % CI 6.3–8.1 ; placebo,

Energetic arousal
5
mean=9.3, 95 % CI 8.5–10.2, t(15)=2.62, p<0.05]. By
120 min, there was no significant difference in hedonic
0
tone between THC and placebo conditions (Fig. 2 a).

Energetic arousal –5

On the UMACL awake–tiredness dimension, THC in-


–10
duced feelings of tiredness at 30 min [THC, mean=
0.3, 95 % CI x1.8 to 2.4 ; placebo, mean=4.6, 95 % CI 0 30 80 120
3.3–5.8, t(17)=4.09, p=0.001], at 80 min [THC, mean= Time (min)
x1.0, 95 % CI x3.0 to 1.1 ; placebo, mean=5.5, 95 % CI
4.3–6.7, t(15)=4.74, p<0.000] and at 120 min post- (c)
injection [THC, mean=x0.2, 95 % CI x1.6 to 1.3 ; 10
placebo, mean=5.1, 95 % CI 3.7–6.6, t(9)=3.60,
p<0.01] (Fig. 2 b). 5
Tense arousal

Tense arousal 0
On the UMACL tension–relaxation dimension, THC
induced feelings of tense arousal at 30 min [THC, –5
mean=x0.7, 95 % CI x3.2 to 1.9 ; placebo, mean=
x7.3, 95 % CI x8.5 to x6.1, t(17)=x3.93, p=0.001] –10
and at 80 min post-injection [THC, mean=x3.3, 95 %
0 30 80 120
CI x5.7 to x1.0 ; placebo, mean=x8.3, 95 % CI x9.4
Time (min)
to x7.2, t(15)=x3.2, p<0.01]. By 120 min, there was
no significant difference in tense arousal between THC Fig. 2. Mean scores of (a) hedonic tone, (b) energetic arousal
and placebo conditions (Fig. 2 c). and (c) tense arousal as measured by the University of
Notably, there was no relationship at 30 and 80 min Wales Institute of Science and Technology Mood
Adjective Checklist (UMACL) following intravenous
post-injection between the degree of THC-elicited
D9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) (2.5 mg). Error bars show
tense arousal and positive psychotic symptoms whe-
ther measured using the PANSS (Spearman’s r=0.19,

¡95 % confidence interval. – –, Placebo ; –&–, THC.

p=0.30) or the CAPE-state (Spearman’s r=0.28, p=


0.12).
(Trial) of the RAVLT. There was a significant main
The impact of THC on cognitive performance effect of drug [F(1, 19)=16.6, p<0.005] and a signifi-
cant main effect of trial [F(2.71, 51.49)=85.3, p<0.000]
RAVLT
but only a trend towards a drugrtrial interaction
Immediate recall under THC or placebo conditions [F(3.04, 57.82)=2.24, p=0.09]. Whereas THC signifi-
(Treatment) was compared in successive trials A1–A5 cantly decreased immediate recall, there was only a
1612 P. D. Morrison et al.

18 200
16 175

THC conc. (ng/ml)


14
Correct responses

150
12 125
10
100
8
75
6
50
4
25
2
0
0 0 15 30 45 60 75 90 105 120
A1 A2 A3 A4 A5 20-min recall
Time (min)
Trial
Fig. 4. Mean plasma concentrations following intravenous
Fig. 3. Performance in the Rey Auditory Verbal
administration of D9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) (2.5 mg).
Learning Task (RAVLT) following intravenous
Error bars show ¡95 % confidence interval.
D9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) (2.5 mg) or matched placebo.
Mean scores are shown for consecutive trials of working
memory (A1–A5) and for free recall at 20-min post-encoding. p=0.003]. There was no relationship between per-
Error bars show ¡95 % confidence interval. – –, Placebo ;  formance in the Baddeley Reasoning Task and work-
–&–, THC. ing memory scores as indexed by the Digit Span task
(r=x0.1, p=0.7), indicating that poorer executive
trend towards a difference in free recall (at 20 min function was not driven by the effect of THC on
post-encoding) between placebo (mean¡S.D.=11.1¡ working memory. The magnitude of poorer perform-
3.3) and THC (9.7¡3.5) conditions [t(17)=1.75, ance in the Baddeley under THC conditions was
p=0.10] (Fig. 3). unrelated to PANSS positive scores at 30 min post-
Under THC conditions, the total diminution in injection (Spearman’s r=0.08, p=0.77).
immediate recall (sum of trials A1–A5) showed no
relationship to PANSS positive scores at 30 min Pharmacokinetics
(Spearman’s r=x0.29, p=0.20). Similarly, delayed
free recall performance under THC conditions was Following IV THC (2.5 mg over 5 min), plasma con-
unrelated to PANSS positive scores at 30 min centrations reached a maximum and then decreased
(Spearman’s r=x0.39, p=0.12). rapidly over 15 min. Thereafter, concentrations de-
creased at a slower rate (Fig. 4).
At 1 min post-injection the plasma concentration of
Digit Span
THC ranged between 96.1 and 206.6 ng/ml (mean¡
THC decreased performance in the forward [mean S.D.=176.7¡33.7). Peak plasma concentrations of the
raw score¡S.D. : THC=7.0¡1.2, placebo=7.8¡0.9, main psychoactive metabolite of THC (11-OH-THC)
t(18)=2.62, p<0.05] and reverse digit span task occurred at 5 min post-injection and ranged from 1.5
[THC=5.1¡1.4, placebo=6.2¡1.1, t(18)=3.3, p< to 7.8 ng/ml (mean¡S.D.=4.4¡2.1). THC area under
0.005]. Diminished performance under THC condi- the curve from time 0 to infinity (AUC0–1) ranged
tions was unrelated to the magnitude of PANSS posi- from 1110.1 to 2995.4 ng min/ml (mean¡S.D.=
tive scores at 30 min post-injection, both for the 2047.3¡423.7).
forward (Spearman’s r=0.11, p=0.61) and reverse There was no relationship between positive psycho-
(Spearman’s r=0.30, p=0.20) arms. tic symptoms, as measured by the PANSS, and plasma
concentrations of THC at 5 min, or 11-OH-THC con-
Verbal fluency centrations at 5 min. Similarly, PANSS positive scores
following THC administration and AUC0–1 were un-
Participants showed no difference in performance
related.
between THC (mean¡S.D.=19.3¡3.2) and placebo
(18.9¡4.2) conditions [t(15)=x0.3, p=0.7].
Discussion
Baddeley Reasoning Task
The observed plasma concentrations and time-course
Performance in the Baddeley Reasoning Task under of THC in the present study are in a similar range to
THC (mean¡S.D.=22.3¡7.6) was significantly poorer those from previous studies in which individuals
than under placebo conditions (26.9¡5.9) [t(15)=3.6, smoked cannabis cigarettes. In a study of healthy
Acute effects of THC 1613

males, mean plasma levels of THC immediately after throughout the reward system and it has become clear
10 puffs from cannabis cigarettes containing 1.75 % or that some important effects of dopamine are mediated
3.55 % THC were respectively 56.2 and 146.6 ng/ml by downstream endocannabinoid signalling at CB1
(Azorlosa et al. 1995). Ramaekers et al. (2006) inves- receptors (Shen et al. 2008). The pro-psychotic effects
tigated the properties of high-potency marijuana of THC observed here may have arisen from direct
cigarettes containing 13 % THC in a sample of rec- overstimulation of CB1 receptors in the striatum.
reational cannabis users. The mean (S.D.) serum THC
concentration at 5 min post-smoking was 93.6 (63.9)
Affect
ng/ml. In the present study, the mean (S.D.) plasma
THC concentration at 5 min post-injection was 68.0 As hypothesized, THC elicited pronounced changes
(14.1) ng/ml. in core affect. Overall, participants were more likely to
Psychological responses to THC began during the rate themselves on the UMACL as dysphoric and an-
redistribution phase (about 5–10 min post-injection) xious in the first hour following THC. (Anecdotally,
when the plasma concentration of THC had already many subjects gave verbal reports of feeling more re-
fallen sharply (Fig. 4). The majority of the psycho- laxed following THC but in the majority of cases their
logical effects tapered off between 60 and 120 min verbal reports were in contradiction to their self-rated
post-THC, despite the relatively small change in the scores on the tense–arousal dimension of the UMACL
absolute plasma concentration during that period and the overall clinical impression. Perhaps this re-
(Fig. 4). Thus, in agreement with previous reports, flects an underlying, widely held assumption that
the onset, peak and termination of psychological re- cannabis has tranquillizing properties.) Dysphoria and
sponses to THC were not related to concurrent plasma anxiety peaked in the early stages following i.v. THC
concentrations (Grotenhermen, 2003). and subsided more rapidly than subjective feelings
of tiredness, which peaked later and were more per-
sistent.
Psychotic symptoms
There was wide inter-individual variation in psy-
Cognitive function
chosis scores. At 30 min post-injection, 50 % of subjects
had increases in the PANSS positive subscale score In the 30 min following THC administration, there
o4, and similarly, 47 % of subjects endorsed positive were marked deficits in working memory and execu-
subscale items from the CAPE-state. Under THC con- tive functioning and a trend towards impaired epi-
ditions there was a significant correlation between sodic memory, all of which are largely consistent with
increases in PANSS and CAPE-state positive scores at previous studies (Curran et al. 2002 ; D’Souza et al.
30 and 80 min. This suggests that phenomena that 2004 ; Ramaekers et al. 2006). It is well established that
were categorized as psychotic were likely to be ‘ true ’ schizophrenia is associated with deficits in tasks of
psychotic experiences. THC psychosis as measured by working memory, executive functioning and episodic
the PANSS or the CAPE-state did not correlate with memory (Barch & Smith, 2008 ; Ranganath et al. 2008).
THC-elicited anxiety, suggesting that THC-elicited Several recent reviews have drawn attention to the
acute anxiety and THC-elicited acute psychosis are similarities between the cognitive deficits elicited by
separable psychological effects. The most commonly cannabis/THC and the cognitive deficits of schizo-
endorsed items on the CAPE-state under THC condi- phrenia and suggested a shared underlying neuro-
tions at 30 min post-injection were : Q.2 Do you feel as if biology (Fletcher & Honey, 2006 ; Solowij & Michie,
people seem to be dropping hints about you or saying things 2007). In the current study we found no suggestion
with a double meaning ? and Q.30 Do you hear your own of any relationship between the extent of positive
thoughts being echoed back to you ? We find this aspect of psychotic symptoms and working memory/executive
THC-induced phenomenology particularly interesting function.
from the point of view of a model psychosis, as ideas Most human studies that have explored the acute
of reference are one of the commonest symptoms in effects of THC and cannabis on mood have used visual
schizophrenia (Mass, 2000 ; Startup et al. 2003). An analogue scales and have focused on vague concepts
influential hypothesis holds that delusional ideas such as ‘ stoned ’ or ‘ high ’. To our knowledge, this is
emerge from a psychological state in which mundane the first study to incorporate a validated, sensitive in-
events are attributed with inappropriate motivational strument to investigate the effects of cannabis/THC
salience (Kapur, 2003). Aberrant salience is thought to on the three core dimensions of mood. Animal and
reflect pathological hyperdopaminergia in the ventral human work suggests that the effects of THC on an-
striatum. Recent work has shown that the dopamine xiety is biphasic, with lower doses being anxiolytic
and endocannabinoid systems are intimately linked and higher doses being anxiogenic (Ashton, 2001 ;
1614 P. D. Morrison et al.

Iversen, 2003 ; Viveros et al. 2005 ; Rubino et al. 2008). preparation, devoid of other active cannabinoid mol-
The anxiety response observed here and in the study ecules. The plasma concentrations and time-course
by D’Souza et al. (2004) may be attributable to high of THC in this study are in the same range as those
plasma levels of THC. Clearly, the setting and drug- observed following inhalational cannabis use.
delivery method are far removed from the naturalistic In conclusion, in a sample of healthy male sub-
way of taking cannabis and may have facilitated an jects, a pure, synthetic i.v. preparation of THC elicited
anxiety response. Furthermore, the purely synthetic acute positive psychotic symptoms, anxiety, dys-
preparation used in the present study was devoid phoria, working memory/executive deficits and, sub-
of cannabidiol (CBD), which has known anxiolytic sequently, feelings of tiredness. Our findings suggest
properties (Pertwee, 2008). This is of interest because it that THC-induced positive psychotic symptoms are
has been suggested that CBD inhibits the anxiogenic neither secondary to high levels of anxiety nor a cause
properties of THC in humans (Zuardi et al. 1982). of anxiety.
Recent work shows that CBD can potently inhibit the
pharmacological effects of CB1 agonists despite having
low affinity for the orthosteric site of the CB1 receptor Acknowledgements
(Pertwee, 2008). It is possible that the anxiogenic ef-
This study was supported by the Psychiatry Research
fects of THC observed here (and also the pro-psychotic
Trust. We thank M. Diforti, S. Luzi and E. Barkus for
effects) emerge from the unopposed actions of THC,
helpful advice and suggestions.
rather than being a function of the plasma levels of
THC per se. This may have relevance to cannabis con-
sumption in more typical settings, and not be merely a
laboratory phenomenon, given that CBD concentra- Declaration of Interest
tions are almost undetectable in some ‘ street ’ forms of None.
cannabis such as sinsemilla (Potter et al. 2008).
In agreement with a previous study (D’Souza et al.
2008), participants who had taken cannabis more ex- References
tensively in the past were less likely to exhibit positive
psychotic symptoms following i.v. THC. However, Ames F (1958). A clinical and metabolic study of acute
intoxication with Cannabis sativa and its role in the model
because people who had used more cannabis in the
psychoses. Journal of Mental Science 104, 972–999.
past were also more likely to report having taken other
Arseneault L, Cannon M, Witton J, Murray RM (2004).
substances, the finding here of a relationship between Causal association between cannabis and psychosis :
previous cannabis use and psychotic symptoms fol- examination of the evidence. British Journal of Psychiatry
lowing i.v. THC may be attributable to other sub- 184, 110–117.
stances taken previously or to polydrug use rather Ashton CH (2001). Pharmacology and effects of cannabis :
than cannabis alone. a brief review. British Journal of Psychiatry 178, 101–106.
Azorlosa JL, Greenwald MK, Stitzer ML (1995). Marijuana
smoking : effects of varying puff volume and breathhold
Limitations duration. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental
Therapeutics 272, 560–569.
A limitation of this study is that only a single dose of
Barch DM, Smith E (2008). The cognitive neuroscience of
THC was used. Animal work has shown that the
working memory : relevance to CNTRICS and
properties of THC are highly dose dependent and bi- schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 64, 11–17.
directional effects are common (Iversen, 2003). To Barnes TR, Mutsatsa SH, Hutton SB, Watt HC, Joyce EM
date, human studies have characterized the effects (2006). Comorbid substance use and age at onset of
of THC within a narrow dose range. Despite the schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 188, 237–242.
methodological difficulties, more extensive dose– Barnett JH, Werners U, Secher SM, Hill KE, Brazil R,
response studies are required. Furthermore, the pres- Masson K, Pernet DE, Kirkbride JB, Murray GK,
ent study does not address the important question Bullmore ET, Jones PB (2007). Substance use in a
of why some individuals are more susceptible than population-based clinic sample of people with
first-episode psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry 190,
others to experience psychotic symptoms following
515–520.
THC/cannabis.
Caspari D (1999). Cannabis and schizophrenia : results of a
The use of intravenous preparations is clearly dif- follow-up study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical
ferent from the use of cannabis in a more natural set- Neuroscience 249, 45–49.
ting. However, it can be argued that this is balanced by Chopra GS, Smith JW (1974). Psychotic reactions following
the pharmacokinetic advantages of the i.v. route and cannabis use in East Indians. Archives of General Psychiatry
the pharmacodynamic advantage of a pure synthetic 30, 24–27.
Acute effects of THC 1615

Curran HV, Brignell C, Fletcher S, Middleton P, Henry J Schultze-Lutter F, Leweke FM (2006). Disturbances of
(2002). Cognitive and subjective dose-response effects of visual information processing in early states of psychosis
acute oral delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in and experimental delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol
infrequent cannabis users. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 164, altered states of consciousness. Schizophrenia Research 88,
61–70. 142–150.
D’Souza DC, Abi-Saab WM, Madonick S, Forselius-Bielen Linszen DH, Dingemans PM, Lenior ME (1994). Cannabis
K, Doersch A, Braley G, Gueorguieva R, Cooper TB, abuse and the course of recent-onset schizophrenic
Krystal JH (2005). Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol effects in disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 51, 273–279.
schizophrenia : implications for cognition, psychosis, and Mass R (2000). Characteristic subjective experiences of
addiction. Biological Psychiatry 57, 594–608. schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 26, 921–931.
D’Souza DC, Perry E, MacDougall L, Ammerman Y, Matthews G, Jones DM, Chamberlain AG (1990). Refining
Cooper T, Wu YT, Braley G, Gueorguieva R, Krystal JH the measurement of mood : the UWIST Mood Adjective
(2004). The psychotomimetic effects of intravenous Checklist. British Journal of Psychology 81, 17–42.
delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in healthy individuals : Mechoulam R, Shani A, Edery H, Grunfeld Y (1970).
implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, Chemical basis of hashish activity. Science 169, 611–612.
1558–1572. Moore TH, Zammit S, Lingford-Hughes A, Barnes TR,
D’Souza DC, Ranganathan M, Braley G, Gueorquieva R, Jones PB, Burke M, Lewis G (2007). Cannabis use and risk
Zimolo Z, Cooper T, Perry E, Krystal J (2008). Blunted of psychotic or affective mental health outcomes :
psychotomimetic and amnestic effects of delta-9- a systematic review. Lancet 370, 319–328.
tetrahydrocannabinol in frequent users of cannabis. Moreau de Tours JJ (1845). Hashish and Insanity [in French].
Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 2505–2516. Fortin, Masson et Cie : Paris.
Fletcher PC, Honey GD (2006). Schizophrenia, ketamine Murray RM, Morrison PD, Henquet C, Di Forti M (2007).
and cannabis : evidence of overlapping memory deficits. Cannabis, the mind and society : the hash realities. Nature
Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10, 167–174. Reviews Neuroscience 8, 885–895.
Goldberg DP, Blackwell B (1970). Psychiatric illness in Naef M, Russmann S, Petersen-Felix S, Brenneisen R (2004).
general practice. A detailed study using a new method of Development and pharmacokinetic characterization of
case identification. British Medical Journal 1, 439–443. pulmonal and intravenous delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol
Grech A, Van Os J, Jones PB, Lewis SW, Murray RM (2005). (THC) in humans. Journal of Pharmacological Sciences 93,
Cannabis use and outcome of recent onset psychosis. 1176–1184.
European Psychiatry 20, 349–353. Ohlsson A, Lindgren JE, Wahlen A, Agurell S, Hollister LE,
Grotenhermen F (2003). Pharmacokinetics and Gillespie HK (1980). Plasma delta-9 tetrahydrocannabinol
pharmacodynamics of cannabinoids. Clinical concentrations and clinical effects after oral and
Pharmacokinetics 42, 327–360. intravenous administration and smoking. Clinical
Hall W (2006). Is cannabis use psychotogenic ? Lancet 367, Pharmacology and Therapeutics 28, 409–416.
193–195. Pertwee RG (2006). Cannabinoid pharmacology : the first
Hambrecht M, Hafner H (2000). Cannabis, vulnerability, and 66 years. British Journal of Pharmacology 147 (Suppl. 1),
the onset of schizophrenia : an epidemiological S163–S171.
perspective. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry Pertwee RG (2008). The diverse CB1 and CB2 receptor
34, 468–475. pharmacology of three plant cannabinoids :
Henquet C, Rosa A, Krabbendam L, Papiol S, Fananas L, delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol, cannabidiol and
Drukker M, Ramaekers JG, van Os J (2006). An delta9-tetrahydrocannabivarin. British Journal of
experimental study of catechol-O-methyltransferase Pharmacology 153, 199–215.
Val(158)Met moderation of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol- Potter DJ, Clark P, Brown MB (2008). Potency of delta 9-THC
induced effects on psychosis and cognition. and other cannabinoids in cannabis in England in 2005 :
Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 2748–2757. implications for psychoactivity and pharmacology. Journal
Iversen L (2003). Cannabis and the brain. Brain 126, of Forensic Science 53, 90–94.
1252–1270. Ramaekers JG, Kauert G, van Ruitenbeek P, Theunissen
Juckel G, Roser P, Nadulski T, Stadelmann AM, Gallinat J EL, Schneider E, Moeller MR (2006). High-potency
(2007). Acute effects of Delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol and marijuana impairs executive function and inhibitory motor
standardized cannabis extract on the auditory evoked control. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 2296–2303.
mismatch negativity. Schizophrenia Research 97, 109–117. Ranganath C, Minzenberg MJ, Ragland JD (2008). The
Kapur S (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience : cognitive neuroscience of memory function and
a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and dysfunction in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 64,
pharmacology in schizophrenia. American Journal of 18–25.
Psychiatry 160, 13–23. Rubino T, Guidali C, Vigano D, Realini N, Valenti M,
Kay SR, Opler LA, Lindenmayer JP (1989). The Positive and Massi P, Parolaro D (2008). CB1 receptor stimulation in
Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) : rationale and specific brain areas differently modulate anxiety-related
standardisation. British Journal of Psychiatry (Suppl.) 59–67. behaviour. Neuropharmacology 54, 151–160.
Koethe D, Gerth CW, Neatby MA, Haensel A, Thies M, Ryan D, Drysdale AJ, Pertwee RG, Platt B (2006).
Schneider U, Emrich HM, Klosterkotter J, Differential effects of cannabis extracts and pure plant
1616 P. D. Morrison et al.

cannabinoids on hippocampal neurones and glia. 11-hydroxy-THC and 11-nor-9-carboxy-THC in serum,


Neuroscience Letters 408, 236–241. and in human liver microsomal preparations using gas
Schimmack U, Grob A (2000). Dimensional models of core chromatography-mass spectrometry. Journal of
affect : a quantitative comparison by means of structural Chromatography B : Analytical Technologies in the Biomedical
equation modeling. European Journal of Personality 14, and Life Sciences 772, 239–248.
325–345. Talbott JA, Teague JW (1969). Marihuana psychosis. Acute
Selzer ML (1971). The Michigan alcoholism screening test : toxic psychosis associated with the use of cannabis
the quest for a new diagnostic instrument. American Journal derivatives. Journal of the American Medical Association 210,
of Psychiatry 127, 1653–1658. 299–302.
Shen W, Flajolet M, Greengard P, Surmeier JD (2008). Van Mastrigt S, Addington J, Addington D (2004).
Dichotomous dopaminergic control of striatal synaptic Substance misuse at presentation to an early psychosis
plasticity. Science 321, 848–851. program. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 39,
Skinner HA (1982). The drug abuse screening test. Addictive 69–72.
Behaviours 7, 363–371. Viveros MP, Marco EM, File SE (2005). Endocannabinoid
Solowij N, Michie PT (2007). Cannabis and cognitive system and stress and anxiety responses. Pharmacology
dysfunction : parallels with endophenotypes of Biochemistry and Behaviour 81, 331–342.
schizophrenia ? Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 32, Volkow ND, Gillespie H, Mullani N, Tancredi L, Grant C,
30–52. Ivanovic M, Hollister L (1991). Cerebellar metabolic
Startup M, Owen DM, Parsonage RK, Jackson MC activation by delta-9-tetrahydro-cannabinol in human
(2003). Anomalous experiences and the contents of brain : a study with positron emission tomography
persecutory delusions during acute psychotic episodes. and 18F-2-fluoro-2-deoxyglucose. Psychiatry Research 40,
Psychology and Psychotherapy : Theory, Research and Practice 69–78.
76, 315–322. Zuardi AW, Shirakawa I, Finkelfarb E, Karniol IG (1982).
Steinmeyer S, Bregel D, Warth S, Kraemer T, Moeller MR Action of cannabidiol on the anxiety and other effects
(2002). Improved and validated method for the produced by delta 9-THC in normal subjects.
determination of Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 76, 245–250.

You might also like