You are on page 1of 6

Journal of Research in Personality 43 (2009) 721–726

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Research in Personality


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp

Need for power and affective response to negative audience reaction to an


extemporaneous speech
Eugene M. Fodor *, David P. Wick
Department of Psychology, Clarkson University, Potsdam, NY 13699-5825, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: An experiment explored the relationship between power motivation and what David McClelland [McC-
Available online 18 June 2009 lelland, D. C. (1976a). Sources of stress in the drive for power. In Serban, G. (Ed.), Psychopathology and
human adaptation (pp. 247–270). New York: Plenum Press] termed as power stress, namely, an event that
Keywords: both arouses and thwarts the power need in power-motivated persons. Need for power was assessed via
Power motivation the Picture Story Exercise [PSE; McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-
Speech anxiety attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690–702] and analyzed by means of
Electromyography
the coding system developed by David Winter [Winter, D. G. (1973). The power motive. New York: Free
Stress
Public Speaking
Press; Smith, C. P. (Ed.). (1992). Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis. Cam-
bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press]. The power-stress induction assumed the form of negative reac-
tion by an audience of two persons to an extemporaneous speech. Conceived in this way, the power-
stress manipulation represents a different operational definition of power stress than has characterized
previous studies. As such, the investigation is an attempt to enlarge McClelland’s concept to explain a
broader set of circumstances. In the control condition, audience reaction was supportive and favorable,
but not to an extreme. Half the research participants scored high on the PSE measure of power motiva-
tion, and half scored low. Half were women, and half were men. Persons high in power motivation dis-
played more negative affect to the negative audience reaction to their speech than did persons low in
power motivation, and higher than both high- and low-power persons in the control condition as indexed
by electromyographic readings from the brow supercilii (p < .05) and by Anxiety scores (p < .05) on the
Affect Balance Scale [Downey, G., & Feldman, S. J. (1996). Implications of rejection-sensitivity for intimate
relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327–1343]. We viewed these findings as
corroborative of McClelland’s power-stress theory.
Ó 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction poraneous speech, a discernibly different operational definition of


power stress than those employed in the past. A visibly negative
The present experiment was an effort to extend David McClel- audience reaction to the speech, we hypothesized, would consti-
land’s concept of power stress (McClelland, 1976a, 1979, 1982). tute a power stress for persons who scored high in power motiva-
McClelland conceived power stress as a social event that simulta- tion. We predicted that the negative audience reaction would
neously both arouses and thwarts the power need, namely, the produce negative affect in high-power individuals both at the cog-
need to influence and create impact on others, and also to com- nitive and physiological levels.
mand recognition or acclaim for these power-oriented behaviors. A power stress can occur in either of two ways (McClelland,
To establish the construct validity of a hypothetical construct like 1979, 1982). The first is an internal control mechanism which bot-
power stress, the ideal strategy requires that one show a relation- tles up the anger, assertiveness, and physiological activation that
ship between the measure of the construct and a variety of behav- the power motive mobilizes – what McClelland termed activity
ioral manifestations that logically derive from the construct, inhibition. The second consists of social circumstances that require
thereby expanding the construct’s explanatory range (Furlong, strong assertive action of a magnitude that exceeds the high-power
Lovelace, & Lovelace, 2000). The manner in which we operationally person’s capacity to deliver. For this latter form of power stress,
defined power stress was negative audience reaction to an extem- McClelland acknowledged that the evidence was scanty at the time
of his writings. In either case – internal restraint or a social event
that exceeds the person’s assertive capabilities – the high-power
person’s attempts at self-assertion are checked and denied
* Corresponding author. Fax: +1 315 268 7118.
E-mail address: fodorem@clarkson.edu (E.M. Fodor). expression, theoretically giving rise to harmful physiological

0092-6566/$ - see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.06.007
722 E.M. Fodor, D.P. Wick / Journal of Research in Personality 43 (2009) 721–726

consequences if experienced repeatedly or for sustained periods easily accessible to conscious awareness but which nevertheless
(cf. Selye, 1973; Steele, 1973). affect feelings and behavior in important ways. McClelland et al.
A principal dependent variable in the present experiment con- chose the term ‘‘implicit” to describe these motives because the
sisted of electromyographic (EMG) recordings from the corrugator persons writing PSE stories are not explicitly describing them-
supercilii (frown muscles). EMG measurements detect muscle selves as having the motive as are people who complete self-re-
activity through non-invasive means. Prompted by the seminal port questionnaires.
studies of Cacioppo and his colleagues (Cacioppo, 1982; Cacioppo
& Petty, 1981a, 1981b; Cacioppo, Petty, Losch, & Kim, 1986), 3. Evidence for the power stress concept
EMG recordings have become a popular procedure among social
psychophysiologists as a means for tracing people’s affective reac- What is most necessary to test the power-stress theory, McClel-
tions to social stimuli. Corrugator EMG activity yields information land (1979) suggested, are experiments that present ‘‘strong situa-
as to how much negative affect an individual experiences when ex- tional challenges” (p. 189) to the power need. Using Winter’s PSE
posed to various attitude objects (Bradley, 2000). Electrical im- measure of power motivation (1973; Smith, 1992), Fodor (1984,
pulses come through the corrugator supercilii, and measurement 1985) conducted industrial simulation experiments with college
of these impulses indicates the degree of unpleasantness the indi- students. Situational challenges took the form of a hard-to-manage
vidual feels at a given time period. The contractions that these im- work group (Fodor, 1984) and experimentally-induced conflict
pulses cause are commonly undetectable as frowns by the naked among ‘‘managers” that the student participant, as ‘‘company pres-
eye. ident,” was supervising and which was virtually impossible to re-
Many studies have shown predicted correlations between EMG solve (Fodor, 1985). Both experiments corroborated McClelland’s
recordings from the corrugator brow muscles and negative affect thesis that high-power persons evince higher sympathetic activa-
(Allen, Horne, & Trinder, 1996; Cacioppo et al., 1986; Fiorito & Si- tion in the face of power stress than do persons low in the power
mons, 1994; Fodor, Wick, & Hartsen, 2006; Larsen, Norris, & Caci- motive.
oppo, 2003; McHugo, Lanzetta, Sullivan, Masters, & Englis, 1985; Fodor et al. (2006) obtained evidence that when told that they
Tassinary, Cacioppo, & Geen, 1989). Fiorito and Simons (1994), were to imagine themselves as manager to the person being inter-
for example, conducted an experiment in which participants heard viewed, persons high in the power motive gave higher electromyo-
scripted, tape-recorded scenes that varied along a dimension from graphic readings from the corrugator brow (frown) muscles when
positive to negative affective valence. The experimenter then asked viewing an assertive candidate on video than did low-power per-
that they visualize these scenes as vividly as possible, as if they sons, and higher than both high- and low-power participants
were actually experiencing the event described. Positive scenes who viewed a compliant candidate. Imagining oneself as having
produced an increase in zygomatic (smile) activity, whereas nega- to interact with an assertive subordinate likely constitutes a power
tive scenes gave rise to a more dominant corrugator (frown) pat- stress for high-power persons.
tern. Conducting research in a similar vein, Bradley (2000) found Working in the McClelland tradition, Wirth, Welsh, and Schul-
significant contractions of the corrugator brow muscle when per- theiss (2006) reasoned that the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal
sons viewed pictures that were rated as unpleasant. (HPA) axis responds to both physical and psychological stressors
by increasing corticosteroid release. They obtained evidence that
the experience of losing a competitive contest did indeed produce
2. The McClelland model enhanced salivary cortisol secretion in power-motivated partici-
pants but not in persons low in power motivation.
The McClelland model (McClelland, 1976b, 1985; McClelland,
Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989) proposes that persons of varying
motive dispositions (i.e., need for achievement, need for affilia- 4. Power motivation and emotion
tion/intimacy, or need for power) differ in their responsivity to
the events they encounter. Learned cues (activity incentives) sig- Closely integrated with McClelland’s concept of power stress is
nal an opportunity to engage in an emotionally reinforcing activ- research evidence linking power motivation with emotion. McClel-
ity specific to a given motive. Should there occur a cue that is land et al. (1989) theorized that individuals engage in behaviors
congruent with the individual’s capacity to experience and sus- that impact the social environment in ways that link to their dom-
tain a certain affective state, the result may be what Woike inant motives, be they need for achievement, need for affiliation/
(1994) terms as a ‘‘kick,” that is, a surge in affect specifically asso- intimacy, or need for power. High-power individuals, by this rea-
ciated with a given motive. On encountering an opportunity to soning, should enact behaviors that create and maintain a feeling
deliver a speech that might possibly influence and impress an of energy and personal excitement. Accordingly, Woike (1994) at-
audience, for instance, a high-power person might well anticipate tempted to induce specific affective states by asking power- and
feelings of strength, vigor, and energy. Should the high-power intimacy-motivated individuals to vividly recall an event that
individual then experience a thwarting of the anticipated surge had made them very happy or an event that occurred yesterday
in these feelings, that experience would constitute a power stress. and was ordinary. Later on, research participants completed a
McClelland et al. (1989) conceive the power motive as based on questionnaire on their current affective state. Using McAdams’s
the incentive of having impact, a kind of conditioned emotion (1982) scoring system for autobiographical memories, Woike
that results in the hormonal release of norepinephrine which is found that individuals high in power motivation generally used
a physiological indicator of stress adaptation. They report evi- more power imagery in their recollections of the happy event (per-
dence that impactful experience indeed is associated with the re- sonal strength, control, vigor, prestige, and recognition) and indi-
lease of norepinephrine in high-power persons vs. those scoring viduals high in intimacy motivation wrote memories more
low in power motivation. Research in the McClelland tradition re- associated with intimacy imagery (involving loving, caring, empa-
lies on what has come to be termed the Picture Story Exercise thy, and closeness to people) when recalling a happy event. In their
(PSE; McClelland et al., 1989; Winter, 1999). The PSE requires questionnaire responses, power-motivated individuals in the
the participant to write stories in response to pictures that pleasant-recall condition reported more excitement and anger
trained scorers analyze by means of a precise and detailed coding than did intimacy-motivated individuals in the same condition.
system. The PSE measures implicit motives, that is, motives not Anger energizes one’s efforts to create impact and therefore elicits
E.M. Fodor, D.P. Wick / Journal of Research in Personality 43 (2009) 721–726 723

positive emotions such as excitement in power-motivated persons Cox (1957) provides evidence that cutoff points at the top and bot-
(Woike & McAdams, 2005). tom 27% most ideally suit this purpose. The experiment consisted
Previous research has consistently shown that people can rate of a 2 (high vs. low need for power)  2 (positive vs. negative audi-
the degree to which facial expressions denote dominance (power) ence reaction)  2 (male vs. female participants) factorial design
and affiliation (cf. Knutson, 1996). Specifically, people see joyful consisting of eight cells each containing 16 participants.
expressions as high on both dominance and affiliation, whereas
they perceive angry faces as connoting high dominance but low 6.2. The Picture Story Exercise (PSE) measure of need for power
affiliation. Looking at these data, Schultheiss and Hale (2007)
hypothesized that the incentive value of a facial expression is recip- When taking the PSE, research participants write stories to a
rocally related to the dominance it displays, because an expression series of ambiguous drawings, usually six when the research focus
from another that connotes dominance signals to high-power per- is human motivation. The PSE is an implicit measure of motivation,
sons that they have failed to exert control. Stated differently, facial that is, psychologists view the stories people write less as a
expressions that suggest high dominance, notably, anger and joy, description of objective reality than as a reflection of inner person-
act as disincentives in the minds of high-power persons. Surprise, ality dynamics that at least partly reside below the level of con-
on the other hand, figures as an incentive for high-power persons, scious awareness. Scoring is by means of a standardized coding
implying that they have registered an impact on the person exhib- system. There is an abundant literature that testifies to the PSE’s
iting a surprise reaction. validity in research on motivation (cf. Fodor, in press; McAdams,
Through a procedure involving Mogg and Bradley’s (1999) dot- 2005; Winter, 1996). Implicit motives as measured by the PSE do
probe task, Schultheiss and Hale obtained evidence that supported not correlate with self-report measures that purport to assess the
their predictions. Power-motivated individuals showed high vigi- same motives (McClelland et al., 1989; Winter, 1999). Indeed, the
lance for low-dominance, and therefore rewarding, facial expres- available evidence suggests that these two types of measures re-
sions that denoted surprise. On the other hand, they oriented flect different kinds of motives and therefore should not be ex-
away from the high-dominance, and therefore aversive, anger pected to correlate with each other (Schultheiss, Yankova,
and joy facial expressions. Durlikov, & Schad, in press; Winter, John, Stewart, Klohnen, & Dun-
can, 1998).
5. Overview We employed six pictures in this experiment, all of them taken
from the set compiled by Smith (1992): (1) conference group: se-
We designed an experiment calculated to further explore McC- ven men around a table, (2) man with cigarette behind woman,
lelland’s power-stress concept. The form power stress assumed in (3) architect at desk, (4) two women in lab coats in laboratory,
this experiment was negative audience reaction to an extempora- (5) ship captain, and (6) trapeze artists. Noting that PSE pictures
neous speech that student participants delivered in an attempt to differ in the extent to which they elicit imagery specific to a given
demonstrate that they were deserving of a $20,000 college scholar- motive, Schultheiss and Brunstein (2001) recommend that when
ship based on their capabilities, achievements, and extracurricular studying a specific motive researchers select pictures that elicit
activities. Although they understood that the situation was hypo- imagery for that motive in roughly 50% of respondents. The pic-
thetical, i.e., no scholarship was in the offing, their written instruc- tures we chose met that criterion for the power motive. The scor-
tions exhorted them to be persuasive in extolling their virtues, ing system commonly in use that has survived through time is the
trying their best to make a favorable impression on the seated one created by Winter (1973). He presents a procedure by which a
audience. The two-person audience playacted either a negative researcher can achieve high interrater reliability calibrated against
or a moderately positive script in reaction to the student partici- expert scoring. The manual presents four practice sets of 30 stories
pant’s presentation. each. The person wishing to achieve scoring proficiency scores
Two hypotheses guided our research endeavor. We predicted these sets one at a time, stopping at the end of each to make a stud-
that power-motivated participants would exhibit higher levels of ied comparison against the expert scoring the manual provides.
electromyographic activity in the brow supercilii when confronted The investigator then determines scoring proficiency by scoring
by a negative audience reaction to their speech than would low- three final sets without pausing at the end of each. Each set again
power participants and higher than all participants experiencing contains 30 stories. The manual describes two measures by which
a favorable reaction. Second and closely related to the foregoing to assess interrater reliability (between the researcher and the ex-
hypothesis, we expected the same outcome pattern for the Anxiety pert scoring). The first is category agreement, which assesses how
subscale of Downey and Feldman’s (1996) Affect Balance Scale. closely the scorer agrees with the standard (expert scoring) on
whether power imagery is present or absent. The second is a rank
correlation between the researcher’s scoring and the expert scoring
6. Methods provided by the manual. This measure is expressed as a q coeffi-
cient. For each measure, Winter recommends that the researcher
6.1. Participants strive to achieve a reliability figure of .85. Strictly following this
procedure, the first author obtained a figure of .94 for category
Participants consisted of 259 male and female students ob- agreement and a q coefficient of .88 for correlation of ranks. He
tained through introductory psychology classes in exchange for re- alone scored the PSEs participants wrote for the present experi-
search-participation credit toward their course grade. In small ment. (However, he and a trained undergraduate research assistant
groups of 10–25 students, all completed the Picture Story Exercise independently scored 29 PSE protocols for power imagery and ob-
(PSE) which was scored for power imagery. Mean scores for men tained a Pearson correlation for raw score of .89.) Raw power mo-
and women were virtually identical. Students scoring in the top tive scores ranged from a low of zero to a high of 23. The Pearson
and bottom 27% of the need-for-power distribution qualified for correlation coefficient between raw score and word length was .11
the laboratory experiment. Carver and Scheier (2004) recommend (ns), so no correction was made for word length. Raw scores were
a procedure whereby one selects as experimental participants per- converted to T-scores (M = 50, SD = 10). T-scores ranged from 28.6
sons who score high and low on a personality measure. This proce- to 79.6. A T-score of 57.7 was the cutoff for the top 27% (high need
dure best demonstrates that the personality variable exerts an for power), and a score of 43.4 for the lower 27% (low need for
effect on observed behavior if indeed such a relationship exists. power). Past research indicates that men and women on average
724 E.M. Fodor, D.P. Wick / Journal of Research in Personality 43 (2009) 721–726

do not differ in power motivation (Woike & McAdams, 2005). Our Immediately thereafter, participants first answered the ques-
data likewise showed that the mean, range, and variance were vir- tion, How much interest would you say the audience showed in
tually identical across gender. your presentation? (Circle your answer.) There were seven points
along the scale with scale point 1 designated as little interest, point
6.3. Electromyographic recordings from the corrugator supercilii 4 as moderate interest, and point 7 as much interest. Next, they com-
pleted the Anxiety subscale of the Affect Balance Scale (Downey &
A Coulbourn electromyograph was used to measure electrical Feldman, 1996). Five items comprise the subscale: nervous, agi-
activity in the corrugator/depressor supercilii complex of the brow tated, tense, on edge, and anxious. These are five-point scales rang-
muscles in the upper facial region. One positive and one negative ing from 0 to 4. These items are embedded within other items that
electrode, centering a ground electrode, monitored electrical activ- bear no relevance to the present inquiry. The Anxiety items, there-
ity at the skin surface associated with muscle contractions. The fore, do not blatantly stand out as suggesting that they are the fo-
electrodes were contained in a single unit, approximately two in cus of the investigation. We obtained an alpha coefficient for this
long, for ease of application. Signal quality and consistency were scale of .76. Lastly, the experimenter provided a brief explanation
enhanced with the application of a conducting gel (TECA Model of the experiment, including the need for deception, and urgently
912–6039). Appropriate placement of the electrodes was verified requested that participants not discuss the experience with other
by observing signal quality with a Tektronix oscilloscope prior to students.
data collection.
The processing of the EMG signal included amplification (High- 8. Results
Performance Bioamplifier Model S75-03) using a constant gain of
100,000, and band pass filter (Model S75-76) to eliminate high-fre- The experimental manipulation was effective. Participants’ rat-
quency noise above 1,000 Hz and low-frequency noise below ings of judges’ interest in their talks ranged from 1 (little interest)
100 Hz. A cumulating-resetting integrator (Model S76-22) de- to 7 (much interest). Responses were as predicted to the question,
tected area content of the time trace and converted the resulting ‘‘How much interest would you say the audience showed in your
analog signal into corresponding 0.1 lV s digital pulses, whose fre- presentation?” The mean for the stress condition (bored judges)
quency was thus proportional to the magnitude of the filtered sig- was 1.7 (SD = .7) and 4.2 (SD = 1.3) for the nonstress condition
nal. A 60 s timer (Model S53-21) permitted the pulses to be (mildly interested judges). The difference yielded an F value of
counted and recorded during successive intervals before the inte- 15.13 (p < .001, df = 1, 126). We treated the EMG data, expressed
grator was reset. The resulting numbers that emerge from the prin- as microvolts, first by adding the readings per minute across the
ter represent microvolts. last 3 min of the 5-min relaxation period. The readings for the 3-
min speech period underwent the same calculation. We subtracted
7. Procedure the former figure from the latter to obtain a difference measure.
There is no absolute scale in surface electromyography, so method
Participants went through the experiment one at a time. The of quantification varies according to specific circumstances (Cram
experimenter began the laboratory session by attaching electrodes & Kasman, 1998; Fridlund & Cacioppo, 1986; Jennings & Stine,
to the brow supercilii to obtain electromyographic recordings. The 2000). In the present instance, the EMG difference figures varied
participant then underwent a 5-min progressive relaxation exer- widely, ranging from very low to very high, and were positively
cise to determine base rate in EMG activity (Fridlund & Cacioppo, skewed. We chose to reduce the skewness and normalize these
1986; Jacobson, 1938; Woolfolk & Richardson, 1978). The experi- data by means of logarithmic transformation. This method con-
menter then presented the participant with written instructions forms closely to recommended procedure for analyzing psycho-
on how the participant should spend 10 min preparing a speech physiological data (Jennings & Stine, 2000). The results of this
while making notations on index cards. Instructions strongly transformation appear in Fig. 1 as bar graphs. Inspection of these
emphasized that the participant should attempt to be persuasive, graphs suggests that for both men and women, persons scoring
to put his or her best foot forward. high in power motivation evince high EMG responsivity from the
Random placement determined which condition of the experi- brow supercilii in the speech-stress condition, higher than persons
ment a participant experienced. The speech itself lasted 3 min.
Two people – one female, one male – comprised the audience.
The stress condition followed a precisely enacted script. It started
off with both actors looking politely at the participant. Fifteen sec- 5.00
onds into the speech, the young woman crossed her legs and began
Electromyographic Measure

looking at her hands. The young man began to shift in his chair. The
woman continued looking around. The man looked at his watch,
then briefly gaped out the window. At approximately 1 min into 4.00
the speech, the actors looked at each other and raised their eye-
brows. They then looked back at the participant delivering the
speech. Both continued to shift their gaze to their hands or the 3.00
floor, rarely looking at the participant. Approximately 2 12 min into
the speech, near the end, the woman gave off a visible sigh. The ac-
tors continued to look around for the remaining 30 s. The man, for
his part, twiddled his thumbs a lot, looked at the clock a few times, 2.00
yawned at specific junctures, avoided eye contact with the partic- Speech Speech Nonstress/High Nonstress/Low
ipant, and attempted to create an impression of indifference to the Stress/High Stress/Low Need for Power Need for Power
Need for Power Need for Power
speech. During the unstressed condition, both actors expressed
moderate but not excessive, interest in the participant’s presenta- Men Women
tion, occasionally nodding in agreement with what the participant
was saying and making continuous eye contact, giving off an Fig. 1. Mean EMG measures from the brow supercilii muscles expressed as
impression that they were ‘‘holding on to every word.” logarithmic transformations. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.
E.M. Fodor, D.P. Wick / Journal of Research in Personality 43 (2009) 721–726 725

low in power motivation, and higher than all participants sub- validity between the two measures. Both appear to measure the
jected to the nonstress condition. same construct, presumably tension, one at the level of the central
A three-way ANOVA applied to the EMG data produced a main nervous system and the other at the level of cognition.
effect for the power-stress vs. nonstress variable (F = 6.13; df = 1,
120; p < .02), for the high- vs. low-power variable (F = 10.59;
df = 1, 120; p < .01), and for the gender variable (F = 8.22; df = 1, 9. Discussion
120; p < .01). Most importantly and as predicted, there occurred
an interaction effect between the power-motivation variable and The findings we obtained in this experiment further support
condition of the experiment (F = 4.5; df = 1, 120; p < .05). It was McClelland’s power-stress theory. Negative audience reaction to
the power-motivated participants who most registered an EMG a person’s efforts to persuade constitutes a reasonable operational
reaction from the brow corrugator muscles in response to negative definition of power stress, i.e., an event likely to thwart the power
audience reaction to their extemporaneous speech. The ANOVA need in a power-motivated person. By both the EMG and Anxiety
yielded no additional evidence for interaction effects. We em- subscale measures, power-motivated persons gave evidence of an
ployed Tukey’s honestly significant difference test (HSD) to evalu- aversive affective response to power stress in the form we pre-
ate pairwise comparisons. Combining scores across gender, the sented it to them. By neither measure did persons low in power
mean of 3.73 for power-motivated participants in the stress condi- motivation prove significantly more reactive in the stress than in
tion was higher than the mean of 3.22 for low-power participants the nonstress manipulation. Affective response was of two related
in the same condition to a degree that was significant at the .01 le- but different types. EMG response is mediated through the central
vel, and higher at the .01 level than the means of 3.30 and 3.19 for nervous system and therefore does not directly represent an aspect
high- and low-power participants, respectively, in the nonstress of autonomic nervous system functioning. It nevertheless closely
condition. parallels physiological responsivity, especially as regards the car-
Fig. 2 displays the data from the Anxiety subscale of the Affect diovascular system (Levenson, 1983). The evidence suggests that
Balance Scale. For both men and women, those persons high in EMG activity is largely unconscious and also undetectable to an ob-
power motivation appear to have expressed greater anxiety in re- server (Bradley, 2000). Response to the Anxiety subscale, on the
sponse to negative audience reaction than did persons low in other hand, is fully conscious and capable of verbal expression.
power motivation and lower than all persons subjected to the non- The two kinds of responsivity reflect different categories of func-
stress manipulation. A three-way ANOVA resulted in a main effect tioning: one at the level of the central nervous system and uncon-
for the stress vs. nonstress variable (F = 12.57; df = 1, 120; p < .001), scious, the other cognitive and fully conscious.
a main effect for the power-motivation variable (F = 11.66; df = 1, Seen in aggregate, studies of people scoring high in power moti-
120; p < .001), but no main effect for gender (F < 1). Again, there vation show that they exhibit chronic, high levels of sympathetic
was an interaction between the power variable and condition of nervous system arousal in response to threat and stress (McClel-
the experiment (F = 4.98; df = 1, 120; p < .05), meaning that the land, 1989; Winter, 1996). That is, they respond to threat with a
power-motivated participants were the ones who most reacted fight-or-flight reaction. High levels of sympathetic activation over
to the power-stress manipulation. There was no evidence for a time, the evidence shows, leads to lowered efficiency of the im-
three-way interaction. All pairwise comparisons between the mune system, thereby producing more infectious diseases and also
high-power/speech-stress cell and the remaining cells of the a variety of health problems, especially high blood pressure. Inso-
experiment again were via Tukey’s HSD test, once again combining far as EMG readings correlate with cardiovascular responsivity, the
scores across gender. The mean of 9.16 for the high-power partic- present findings would appear to extend the range of experiences
ipants in the stress condition was higher than the corresponding that may impair cardiovascular health in power-motivated
mean of 6.47 for low-power participants in the stress condition persons.
at the .01 level of significance, and higher at the .01 level than The finding of a main effect for gender in EMG responsivity is
the means of 6.41 and 5.84 for high- and low-power participants worthy of mention. Women, it will be recalled, showed greater
in the nonstress condition. The Pearson correlation between the reactivity than men from the brow supercilii in response to both
EMG measure as we calculated it and the Anxiety subscale mea- conditions of the experiment. Diener, Sandvik, and Larsen (1985)
sure was .43 (p < .01). This figure suggests a degree of convergent as well as Brody and Hall (2000) found that women experience
more intense emotions than men. The main effect for gender did
not occur with the Anxiety subscale of the Affect Balance Scale,
however.
13.00
The bar graphs represented by Figs. 1 and 2 do not suggest a
12.00
phobic reaction to public speaking situations in general, that is,
11.00
no significant differences between high- and low-power persons
10.00 appear evident in the nonstress condition. Internal analysis of the
9.00 data indeed validates the visual impression. The small differences
Anxiety

8.00 are nonsignificant. Nevertheless, power-motivated persons appear


7.00 vulnerable to the power stress implicit in a negative audience reac-
6.00 tion. As Comer (2007) observes, however, anxiety that arises from a
5.00 public performance does not necessarily eventuate in a poor per-
4.00 formance as evaluated by others. Woody (1996) found that social
3.00 phobics when speaking before an audience rated themselves as
Speech Speech Nonstress/High Nonstress/Low more anxious than did trained clinical psychology graduate stu-
Stress/High Stress/Low Need for Power Need for Power dents who observed videotapes of their presentations.
Need for Power Need for Power
There are careers that regularly place their occupants before
groups toward whom they are attempting to exert influence but
Men Women
from whom they are equally likely to experience resistance and
Fig. 2. Mean scores on the Anxiety scale. Error bars represent standard errors of the antagonism. One thinks immediately of politicians and labor-man-
mean. agement negotiators. The findings we obtained, together with the
726 E.M. Fodor, D.P. Wick / Journal of Research in Personality 43 (2009) 721–726

theoretical rationale on which they are based, suggest that certain McAdams, D. P. (1982). Experiences of intimacy and power: Relationships between
personal motives and autobiographical memory. Journal of Personality and Social
occupations may pose repeated exposures to stress of a kind that
Psychology, 42, 292–302.
can threaten cardiovascular health for persons high in power McAdams, D. P. (2005). An integrated introduction to personality. Somerset, NJ: John
motivation. Wiley.
McClelland, D. C. (1976a). Sources of stress in the drive for power. In G. Serban (Ed.),
Psychopathology and human adaptation (pp. 247–270). New York: Plenum Press.
References McClelland, D. C. (1976b). The achieving society. New York: Irvington.
McClelland, D. C. (1979). Inhibited power motivation and high blood pressure in
Allen, N. B., Horne, D. J. deL., & Trinder, J. (1996). Sociotropy, autonomy, and men. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 182–190.
dysphoric emotional responses to specific classes of stress: A McClelland, D. C. (1982). The need for power, sympathetic activation, and illness.
psychophysiological evaluation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 25–33. Motivation and Emotion, 6, 31–41.
Bradley, M. M. (2000). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. McClelland, D. C. (1985). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.
G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 602–642). Cambridge, UK: McClelland, D. C. (1989). Motivational factors in health and disease. American
Cambridge University Press. Psychologist, 41, 675–683.
Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2000). Gender, emotion, and expression. In M. Lewis & J. M. McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and
Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 338–349). New York: implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690–702.
Guilford Press. McHugo, G. J., Lanzetta, J. T., Sullivan, D. G., Masters, R. D., & Englis, B. G. (1985).
Cacioppo, J. T. (1982). Social psychophysiology: A classic perspective and Emotional reactions to a political leader’s expressive displays. Journal of
contemporary approach. Psychophysiology, 19, 241–251. Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1513–1529.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1981a). Electromyograms as measures of extent Moog, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Some methodological issues in assessing
and affectivity of information processing. American Psychologist, 36, attentional biases for threatening faces in anxiety: A replication study using a
441–456. modified version of the probe detection task. Behavior Research and Therapy, 37,
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1981b). Electromyographic specificity during covert 595–604.
information processing. Psychophysiology, 18, 518–523. Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Assessment of implicit motives with a
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Losch, M. E., & Kim, H. S. (1986). Electromyographic research version of the TAT: Picture profiles, gender differences, and relations to
activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and other personality measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77, 71–86.
intensity of affective reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Schultheiss, O. C., & Hale, J. A. (2007). Implicit motives modulate attentional
5, 260–268. orienting to facial expression of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 13–24.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2004). Perspectives on personality. Boston: Allyn and Schultheiss, O. C., Yankova, D. Durlikov, B., & Schad, D. J. (in press). Are implicit and
Bacon. explicit motive measures statistically independent? A fair and balanced test
Comer, R. J. (2007). Abnormal psychology. New York: Worth. using the Picture Story Exercise and a cue- and response-matched
Cox, D. R. (1957). A note on grouping. Journal of the American Statistical Association, questionnaire measure. Journal of Personality Assessment.
52, 543–547. Selye, H. (1973). The evolution of the stress concept. American Scientist, 61,
Cram, J. R., & Kasman, G. S. (1998). Introduction to surface electromyography. 672–699.
Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen Publishers. Smith, C. P. (Ed.). (1992). Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content
Diener, E., Sandvik, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1985). Age and sex effects for emotional analysis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
intensity. Developmental Psychology, 21, 542–546. Steele, R. S. (1973). The physiological concomitants of psychogenic motive arousal
Downey, G., & Feldman, S. J. (1996). Implications of rejection-sensitivity for intimate in college males. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Harvard University.
relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327–1343. Tassinary, L. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Geen, T. R. (1989). A psychometric study of surface
Fiorito, E. R., & Simons, R. F. (1994). Emotional imagery and physical anhedonia. electrode placements for facial electromyographic recording: I. The brow and
Psychophysiology, 31, 513–521. cheek muscle regions. Psychophysiology, 26, 1–16.
Fodor, E. M. (1984). The power motive and reactivity to power stresses. Journal of Winter, D. G. (1973). The power motive. New York: Free Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 853–859. Winter, D. G. (1996). Personality: Analysis and interpretation of lives. New York:
Fodor, E. M. (1985). The power motive, group conflict, and physiological arousal. McGraw-Hill.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1408–1415. Winter, D. G. (1999). Linking personality and scientific psychology: The
Fodor, E. M. (in press). Power motivation. In O. C. Schultheiss, & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), development of empirically derived Thematic Apperception Test measures. In
Implicit motives. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. L. Geiser & M. I. Stein (Eds.), Evocative images: The Thematic Apperception Test
Fodor, E. M., Wick, D. P., & Hartsen, K. M. (2006). The power motive and affective and the art of projection (pp. 107–124). Washington, DC: American Psychological
response to assertiveness. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 598–610. Association.
Fridlund, A. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic Winter, D. G., John, O. P., Stewart, A. J., Klohnen, E. C., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Traits
research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567–589. and motives: Toward an integration of two traditions in personality research.
Furlong, N. E., Lovelace, E. A., & Lovelace, K. L. (2000). Research methods and statistics: Psychological Review, 105, 230–250.
An integrated approach. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. Wirth, M. M., Welsh, K. M., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2006). Salivary cortisol changes in
Jacobson, E. (1938). Progressive relaxation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. humans after winning or losing a dominance contest depend on implicit power
Jennings, J. R., & Stine, L. A. (2000). Salient method, design, and analysis concerns. In motivation. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 346–352.
J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. T. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Woike, B. A. (1994). Vivid recollection as a technique to arouse implicit motive-
psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 870–899). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge related affect. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 335–349.
University Press. Woike, B. A., & McAdams, D. P. (2005). Motives. In V. J. Derlega, B. A. Winstead, & W.
Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal trait H. Jones (Eds.), Personality: Contemporary theory and research (3rd ed.,
inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 165–182. pp. 156–189). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Larsen, J. T., Norris, C. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Effects of positive and negative Woody, S. R. (1996). Effects of focus of attention on anxiety levels and social
affect on electromyographic activity over zygomaticus major and corrugator performance of individuals with social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
supercilii. Psychophysiology, 40, 776–785. 105, 61–69.
Levenson, R. W. (1983). Personality research and psychophysiology: General Woolfolk, R., & Richardson, F. (1978). Stress, sanity, and survival. New York: Monarch
considerations. Journal of Research in Personality, 17, 1–21. Books.

You might also like