You are on page 1of 15

International Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition

ISSN: 0963-7486 (Print) 1465-3478 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/iijf20

A comparison of two low-calorie sweeteners and


sugar in dark chocolate on sensory attributes and
emotional conceptualisations

Sofie Lagast , Hans De Steur , Joachim J. Schouteten & Xavier Gellynck

To cite this article: Sofie Lagast , Hans De Steur , Joachim J. Schouteten & Xavier Gellynck
(2017): A comparison of two low-calorie sweeteners and sugar in dark chocolate on sensory
attributes and emotional conceptualisations, International Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition,
DOI: 10.1080/09637486.2017.1362689

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09637486.2017.1362689

View supplementary material

Published online: 14 Aug 2017.

Submit your article to this journal

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=iijf20

Download by: [Australian Catholic University] Date: 15 August 2017, At: 17:10
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCES AND NUTRITION, 2017
https://doi.org/10.1080/09637486.2017.1362689

RESEARCH ARTICLE

A comparison of two low-calorie sweeteners and sugar in dark chocolate on


sensory attributes and emotional conceptualisations
Sofie Lagast , Hans De Steur , Joachim J. Schouteten and Xavier Gellynck
SensoLab, Department of Agricultural Economics, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Reducing sugar consumption is an important aspect in the prevention of and fight against obes- Received 27 February 2017
ity. A broader understanding of consumers’ perceptions of low-calorie sweeteners is needed. This Revised 20 July 2017
study examined two low-calorie sweeteners, tagatose and stevia, in comparison to sugar in dark Accepted 24 July 2017
chocolate. A total of 219 consumers participated in this study and rated overall liking and sensory
attributes. Participants also listed their emotional conceptualisations upon consumption and were
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

KEYWORDS
assessed on emotional eating behaviour and health and taste attitudes. The chocolate with taga- Dark chocolate; sugar;
tose was perceived as more similar to the chocolate with sugar than with stevia on overall liking, tagatose; stevia; emotion;
texture, bitterness, duration of aftertaste and intensity of aftertaste. Furthermore, chocolate with sensory
sugar and chocolate with tagatose both elicited positive emotional conceptualisations whereas
chocolate with stevia elicited negative emotional conceptualisations. In conclusion, dark choc-
olate with tagatose did not significantly differ from sugar in overall liking, most sensory attributes
and emotional conceptualisation.

Introduction for certain consumer groups such as patients with dia-


betes, people who want to decrease caloric intake and
Reducing consumption of sugar levels in the world’s
children (Goyal and Goyal 2010).
population is a key to improving public health by pre-
However, low-calorie sweeteners will only be
venting and tackling obesity. In 2015, the World
Health Organisation launched a directive to limit accepted by a broad base of consumers if their sensory
sugar intake to 25 g per day (World Health attributes are positively evaluated. Sensory liking
Organization 2015). This has led to an increased remains the main driver for preference and food
awareness among consumers about the risks of high choice (Hellemann and Tuorila 1991; Hetherington
sugar intake and to a more prominent role of low-cal- and Macdiarmid 1995; de Graaf et al. 2005).
orie sweeteners in the market (Goyal and Goyal 2010; Therefore, it is important that the products with a
Ghosh and Sudha 2012). lower calorie content resemble the original product on
The replacement of sugars with low-calorie sweet- sensory attributes (Zorn et al. 2014). Unfortunately,
eners is a way to lower sugar intake and to manage some low-calorie sweeteners elicit undesirable sensory
body weight (Bellisle and Drewnowski 2007; Ludwig qualities such as unpleasant aftertaste, bitterness,
2009; Anderson et al. 2012; Gardner et al. 2012; metallic taste or astringency (Fujimaru et al. 2012)
Drewnowski and Rehm 2014). People who consume which can be linked to a lower consumer acceptance
low-calorie sweeteners tend to have higher health eat- (Zhao and Tepper 2007). Stevia, for example, elicits
ing index scores and tend to be more physically active lower liking scores in mango nectar, grape nectar,
as well (Drewnowski and Rehm 2014). The combin- skimmed chocolate milk and chocolate compared to
ation of consumption of low-calorie sweeteners with sugar (de Melo et al. 2009; Shah et al. 2010; Cadena
healthier diets and with more physical activity is even et al. 2013; Voorpostel et al. 2014; Li et al. 2015) and
more effective in reducing and controlling body shows non-sweet off flavours (bitterness and black
weight (Bellisle et al. 2001; Bleich et al. 2014). Low- liquorice) with high stevia levels (Prakash et al. 2008).
calorie sweeteners can thus contribute to promoting Tagatose, on the other hand, has been shown to have
healthier public nutrition and are particularly helpful similar physical and sensory characteristics as sugar

CONTACT Sofie Lagast sofie.lagast@ugent.be Department of Agricultural Economics, Ghent University, Ghent 9000, Belgium
Supplemental data for this article can be accessed here.
ß 2017 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2 S. LAGAST ET AL.

and to elicit sweet flavour without undesirable qual- Emotions associated with chocolate consumption have
ities in aqueous solutions (Fujimaru et al. 2012). been both positive and negative. Macht and Dettmer
Not only sensory acceptance is important, it is also (2006) demonstrated that women experience both joy
essential to measure beyond acceptance by assessing a and guilt after consuming chocolate. Joy was elicited
broader insight into consumers’ food product experi- by the sensory pleasure of eating chocolate while guilt
ence (Thomson et al. 2010; Cardello et al. 2012). In appeared to be induced by negative thoughts associ-
consumer research, the role of emotion in behaviour ated with it (like the unwanted effect on body weight).
has been increasingly acknowledged (Johnson and In attempts to address consumers’ demands to reduce
Stewart 2005). Recent studies show that consumers’ sugar intake through chocolate, sugar is increasingly
emotional conceptualisations towards food products substituted by sweeteners. Yet, for long-term con-
can add additional information beyond overall accept- sumption of low-calorie chocolate, there is a need to
ance (King and Meiselman 2010; Thomson et al. 2010; examine consumer-acceptance of low-calorie sweet-
Cardello et al. 2012; Ng et al. 2013; Coleman et al. eners (Li et al. 2015).
2014; Spinelli et al. 2014; Gutjar et al. 2015; In this study, consumers’ sensory evaluation and
Schouteten et al. 2015) and even significantly improve emotional conceptualisations upon consumption of
food choice prediction (Dalenberg et al. 2014). dark chocolates with sugar and two low-calorie
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

To assess the emotions elicited by food, most stud- sweeteners (tagatose and stevia) are investigated. The
ies use a self-reported method. The most commonly study aims to compare two low-calorie sweeteners
used method is a questionnaire format with a list of in relation to sugar in dark chocolate (1) by exam-
emotional terms (emotional lexicon) that can be ining the overall liking and sensory attributes of the
checked (e.g. Check-all-that-apply, CATA) or rated. chocolates, (2) by investigating which emotional
The emotional lexicon typically holds terms that can conceptualisations consumers associate with the
be classified as positive or negative (Desmet and chocolates and (3) by looking at how consumers’
Schifferstein 2008; Schifferstein and Desmet 2010; emotional eating behaviour and health and taste
Schouteten et al. 2015) and can be either standardised attitudes are related to acceptability and emotional
or consumer-generated. Standardised emotional lexi- conceptualisations.
cons such as the EsSense ProfileV questionnaire have
R

been developed by King and Meiselman (2010).


Consumer-generated lexicons are product-specific and Materials and methods
have already been applied to a wide range of foods,
Participants
such as chocolate (Thomson et al. 2010), hazelnut
spreads (Spinelli et al. 2014), fruit salads (Manzocco Participants were recruited at the university campus
et al. 2013) and cheese (Schouteten et al. 2015). by poster advertisement and were also recruited from
Recently, also non-self-reported (implicit) measure- a database containing volunteers for sensory and con-
ments of emotions have gained attention. Researchers sumer research of Ghent University. In total, 219 con-
have used psychophysiological response tracking, such sumers participated voluntarily and completed the
as facial expressions, skin conductance, heart rate or questionnaire anonymously via EyeQuestion version
finger temperature of consumers, to access implicit 3.12.0 software (Logic8 BV, Elst, The Netherlands).
emotions (de Wijk et al. 2012, 2014; Pentus et al. Testing took place in the sensory facilities of Ghent
2014; Leitch et al. 2015). University and participants evaluated the samples in
Some studies have even aimed to attribute distinct sensory booths. Because this study focussed on the
emotions to sensory attributes. Robin et al. (2000) for sweeteners used in chocolate, i.e. through comparing
example asked participants to associate emotions (hap- two low-calorie sweeteners in relation to sugar in dark
piness, surprise, sadness, fear, disgust and anger) with chocolate, this study has recruited participants that
water solutions of basic flavours (sweet, sour, bitter consume all types of chocolate (white, milk and dark)
and salt). The sweet solution was mainly associated regardless of their chocolate preference. Yet, to be eli-
with happiness and surprise, the bitter one with anger gible for participation participants were required to
and disgust and the salty and sour solutions were like and consume all three types of chocolate. They
associated with all emotions. were barred from participation if they had any food
This study uses dark chocolate as a case. Chocolate allergies or if they disliked dark chocolate. This
is high in sugar content and has a hedonic appeal due screening was operatised through two questions, one
to its composition and sensory attributes (fat, sugar, for acceptability of chocolate and one for food
texture and aroma) (Bruinsma and Taren 1999). allergies.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCES AND NUTRITION 3

Samples information was visible in the chocolate pieces. A ran-


dom three-digit number was assigned to each sample
Three dark chocolates were selected for consumer
to reduce expectation errors (Moskowitz et al. 2012).
evaluation: chocolate sweetened with tagatose
Moreover, the chocolates were evaluated in a random
(Damhert dark, Belgium), chocolate sweetened with
stevia (Cavalier dark, Belgium) and chocolate with order to prevent first-order and carryover effects.
sugar (Jacques dark, Belgium). Based on previous sug-
gestions for emotional research by King et al. (2013), Procedure
the number of samples was limited to three. All choc-
Participants were invited to the sensory facilities of
olates were commercially available in Belgian super-
the university. They were told they would be evaluat-
markets and all contained a minimum cacao
ing three pieces of dark chocolate but no information
percentage of 50%. The chocolates with sweeteners
still contained naturally occurring sugars, respectively of sugar or sweetener content was given.
0.3 and 3.2 g of sugars per 100 g of chocolate for the Before starting the questionnaire participants
chocolate with tagatose and the chocolate with stevia. needed to complete two screening questions in order
Additionally, the chocolate with stevia contained to assess their eligibility for the study. The screening
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

erythritol which is a bulking agent that also suppresses criteria were based on their diet (liking and consump-
the bitter flavour of stevia. Table 1 gives an overview tion of chocolate) and food allergies (no allergy for
of the key characteristics of the chocolates. In chocolate, no allergy for nuts, no allergy or intolerance
Supplementary Table 1, a more detailed overview of for lactose and no allergies for other food products).
the nutritional values and the ingredient list for each The questionnaire consisted of three sections: (1)
chocolate can be found. attitude and behaviour (health and taste attitudes
A piece of approximately 3.33 g of each chocolate related to food, chocolate eating behaviour and emo-
was presented to the participants at the same time. All tional eating behaviour); (2) overall liking, sensory
chocolates had the same shape and no brand attributes and emotional conceptualisations of the
three selected chocolates and (3) socio-demographic
Table 1. Key characteristics of the examined chocolate prod- profile and diet restriction behaviour of the participant
ucts expressed per 100 g of chocolates. (diet to lose weight). The flow of the screening and
Cocoa Nutritional questionnaire is depicted in Figure 1.
Product (min %) value (kcal) Sugar (g) Polyols (g)
The first section of the questionnaire examined the
Chocolate þ sugar 50 520 46.9 0.0
Chocolate þ tagatose 54 457 0.3 3.6 participant’s attitudes and behaviour. To gain more
Chocolate þ stevia 55 432 3.2 8.7 information about the health and taste interests of the
Note: All values are expressed per 100 g of chocolate. participants, health and taste attitudes were measured

Screening criteria
• Chocolate consumption
• Food allergies

Attitude and behavior


• Health and taste attitudes related to food (HTAS)
• Chocolate eating behavior
• Emotional eating behavior (DEBQ-e)

Sensory attributes and emotional conceptualizations of the three selected chocolates


• Overall liking
• Emotional conceptualizations
• Sensory attributes

Socio-demographic questions and diet restriction behavior of the participant

Figure 1. Screening and questionnaire flow.


4 S. LAGAST ET AL.

by the Health and Taste Attitude Scale (HTAS), a vali- (currently on a diet to lose weight, on a diet to lose
dated questionnaire, developed by Roininen et al. weight during the last year or none of both).
(2001). This scale measures the importance of health
and taste in foods in the food choice process. It con-
Development of consumer-defined emotional
sists of three health sub-scales (general health interest,
lexicon
light product interest and natural product interest)
and three taste sub-scales (craving for sweet foods, A product-specific emotional lexicon was determined
using food as reward and pleasure). The 20 items on during preliminary research following a two-step
health and 18 items on taste had to be rated on a 7- approach suggested by Ng et al. (2013). First, a group
point Likert scale, ranging from “strongly disagree” (1) of 17 healthy and young adults (10 females, 7 males,
to “strongly agree” (7). 82% between 18 and 34 years) evaluated a list of emo-
Behaviour was measured by questions about choc- tional terms based on literature (Desmet and
olate eating behaviour and emotional eating behav- Schifferstein 2008; King and Meiselman 2010;
iour. Chocolate eating behaviour was examined with Thomson and Crocker 2013). Second, a final selection
questions based upon a focus group discussion, prior was made based on the frequency of the terms
selected (20%) and the ability of the terms to discrim-
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

research and literature review. The preference of type


of chocolates was measured using three categorical inate between food products (Ferrarini et al. 2010;
labels: white, milk and dark. The frequency of con- Thomson et al. 2010; De Pelsmaeker et al. 2013;
sumption was examined using five categorical labels Manzocco et al. 2013; Ng et al. 2013). Additionally, a
ranging from “daily” to “less than once in 2 weeks”. balance between positive and negative emotions was
Emotional eating behaviour was examined by the made to enhance a more natural balance between the
emotional eating scale of the validated Dutch Eating amount of positive and negative emotional conceptu-
alisation for food products (Desmet and Schifferstein
Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ-e) (Van Strien et al.
2008; King and Meiselman 2010; De Pelsmaeker et al.
1986). The DEBQ is a validated psychometric con-
2013). Based on these results, 24 emotional terms
struct which measures three types of eating behaviour:
comprised the actual list (Table 2).
emotional eating, external eating and restricted eating.
In the present study, only the 13 relevant items on
emotional eating were implemented on the question- Data analysis
naire as a 5-point scale (from “never” to “very often”).
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS
In the second part, participants received three
Statistics version 22 (SPSS, Chicago, IL). To assess the
pieces of dark chocolate at the same time, one of each
difference in overall liking and sensory attributes
type of chocolate. In a randomised order, participants among chocolate samples, repeated measures ANOVA
evaluated one piece of dark chocolate at a time. tests were performed. Bonferroni or Dunnett post-hoc
Participants were instructed to take a first bite of the tests were executed to look at differences between the
chocolate and (1) rate the overall liking using a 7- samples, in the case of homogeneity or non-homogen-
point bipolar scale (ranging from 1 ¼ extremely dislike eity of variance, respectively.
to 7 ¼ extremely like) and (2) select the applicable To compare emotional conceptualisations and dif-
emotional terms with the following instruction: ferent types of chocolates, a Cochran’s Q test was exe-
“Below you find a list of terms that describe emotion. cuted for each emotional term. Pairwise comparison
Using the list below, tick each word that describes between different chocolates for each emotional term
how you feel right now. Please tick all terms that are was achieved using the McNemar Test. As emotional
applicable”. This instruction was based upon previous
work of emotional profiling of food products (King
and Meiselman 2010). Next, participants were Table 2. Overview of the selected emotional terms.
instructed to take a second bite and were asked to rate Emotional terms
sensory attributes, namely texture and taste (sweetness, Feeling goodþ Activeþ Disappointment
Pleasedþ Joyfulþ Unpleasant surprise
bitterness, the intensity of aftertaste and duration of Happyþ Peacefulþ Bored
aftertaste) on a 5-point just-about-right (JAR) scale. Gladþ Stimulatingþ Dissatisfying
Satisfyingþ Pleasantþ Sad
The last part contained questions regarding the Interestingþ Enthusiasticþ Quiet
socio-demographic status of the respondents (gender Enjoymentþ Good-naturedþ Disgusted
Calmþ Anger Tame
and age (categorical) and weight and length (continu- þ,
means positive and negative emotional term.
ous)) and diet restriction behaviour to lose weight
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCES AND NUTRITION 5

conceptualisations were measured through a CATA being low emotional eaters, medium emotional eaters
question, correspondence analysis was used in order and high emotional eaters using the norm scales for
to make a graphical presentation. All emotional con- healthy populations taking gender and BMI into
ceptualisations were plotted together with the three account. The amount of participants in these groups
types of chocolate and overall liking was added as a was 17, 120 and 82, respectively. The same method-
supplementary category. ology was used as in Piqueras-Fiszman and Jaeger
Factor analysis (Maximum likelihood with Varimax (2014) to link emotional responses to emotional eat-
rotation) was performed on the items of the HTAS ing. The segmentation in three groups created an
(separately on the 20 health and 18 taste items) and uneven group of participants; however, the results
on the 13 items of the emotional eating scale of the were interpreted with caution in instances where the
DEBQ. Cronbach’s alpha was then used to test the number of participants was very unbalanced.
internal reliability of each factor. Means and standard
deviations of the factors of the health and taste attrib- Results
utes and emotional eating behaviour were calculated.
For differences in mean liking by socio-demograph- Socio-demographic profile
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

ics, eating behaviour and attitudes, mixed factorial Participants’ socio-demographics are displayed in
ANOVA tests were performed. Depending on the Table 3. The questionnaire was completed by 219 con-
homogeneity or non-homogeneity of the variance of sumers of which 92 men (42%) and 127 women
samples, respectively, Bonferroni or Dunnet post-hoc (58%). Of all participants, 68% were high chocolate
tests were used. Mean overall liking was used as a users (with an everyday or more than once a week
within-subjects variable and socio-demographics, eat- consumption of chocolate). Milk chocolate was the
ing behaviour and attitudes as a between-subjects vari- most consumed and preferred type of chocolate. The
able for each chocolate sample. main motivation to eat chocolate was craving, fol-
Finally, to link the emotional profile in response to lowed by taste. Half of the participants had a moder-
the consumption of the chocolates and the emotional ate emotional eating behaviour (54.8%) and high
eating behaviour, the participants were segmented into emotional eaters accounted for 37.4%. In this case, 82
three groups according to their DEBQ-e score. The participants were highly influenced by emotions in
split was performed by characterising participants as their eating behaviour (Table 3).

Table 3. Socio-demographics, chocolate consumption behaviour and emotional eating behaviour of participants.
Socio-demographics n % Chocolate consumption n %
Gender Frequency
Male 92 42 High users 147 68
Female 127 58 Medium users 45 20
Age Low users 27 12
18–25 years 128 58 Consumption
26–45 years 41 19 White 12 6
46þ years 50 23 Milk 127 58
BMI (mean ¼ 22.44, SD ¼ 3.12) Dark 80 36
Underweight (<18.5) 17 8 Preference
Normal weight (18.5–25) 158 74 White 27 12
Overweight (>25) 40 19 Milk 107 49
Currently on a diet to lose weight Dark 85 39
No 202 92 Motivation
Yes 17 8 Taste 134 42
On a diet to lose weight during last year Emotional 23 7
No 185 85 Craving 151 48
Yes 34 15 Habit 28 9
Reduce feelings of hunger 23 7
Else 12 4

Eating behaviour
Emotional eating
Low 17 7.8
Moderate 120 54.8
High 82 37.4
Participants could check more than one option for motivation (n ¼ 371). Eating behaviour was measured through 13 items of the Dutch eating behaviour
questionnaire (DEBQ-e) (Van Strien et al. 1986) on a 5-point Likert scale (1 ¼ never, 5 ¼ very often). Raw scale score was compared to norm scales accord-
ing to BMI and gender. These were categorised as follows: very low to low ¼ low, under average, average and above average ¼ moderate, high and very
high ¼ high.
6 S. LAGAST ET AL.

Table 4. Evaluation of overall liking and sensory attributes of chocolates with sugar or low-calorie sweeteners.
Regular Low-calorie sweeteners
Chocolate þ sugar Chocolate þ tagatose Chocolate þ stevia
Median Q1–Q3 Median Q1–Q3 Median Q1–Q3 F value p Value
Overall liking 5a 4–6 5a 3–6 3b 2–4 85.80 .000
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Sensory attributes
Texture 0.02a,b 0.57 0.06a 0.63 0.12b 0.66 6.72 .001
Taste
Sweetness 0.15a 0.73 0.16b 0.77 0.73c 0.87 100.10 .000
Bitterness 0.23a 0.71 0.10a 0.79 0.29b 1.05 33.60 .000
Intensity of aftertaste 0.05 0.68 0.10 0.75 0.07 1.07 2.91 .058
Duration of aftertaste 0.04a 0.71 0.05a 0.77 0.24b 0.94 10.53 .000
Repeated measures ANOVA with Bonferroni post-hoc tests, ab significantly different (p  .05). Overall liking measured on 7-point bipolar scale ranging from
1 (extremely dislike) to 7 (extremely like), sensory attributes measured on 5-point JAR scale ranging from 2 (e.g. not sweet enough) to 2 (e.g. to sweet).
significance p < .05. significance p < .01.
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

Overall liking and sensory attributes: a discriminating between sugar and low-calorie sweet-
comparison of chocolate with tagatose and ener stevia. Negative emotional conceptualisations are
chocolate with stevia in relation to chocolate with mostly associated with chocolate with stevia. In par-
sugar ticular, negatively loaded emotions, such as
“disappointment”, “dissatisfaction”, “unpleasant sur-
The overall liking differed significantly between the
selected chocolate samples (Table 4). Bonferroni post- prise” and “disgusted” are significantly more stated
hoc test showed a significantly lower overall liking for when consuming chocolate with stevia. The negative
chocolate with stevia compared to chocolate with taga- conceptualisation “unpleasant surprise” showed the
tose and compared to chocolate with sugar. No signifi- biggest difference between the low-calorie sweeteners
cant differences were found between chocolate with and the negative emotional conceptualisation
sugar and chocolate with tagatose. “disappointment” showed the biggest difference
Regarding the sensory attributes, chocolates with between sugar and low-calorie sweetener stevia.
low-calorie sweeteners tagatose and stevia differed sig- Values of the emotional conceptualisation can be
nificantly on texture, sweetness, bitterness and dur- found in Table 5.
ation of aftertaste. Yet, the chocolate with tagatose did A graphical presentation of the correspondence
not differ from the chocolate with sugar on texture, analysis of the emotional conceptualisations on two
bitterness and duration of aftertaste. The results show dimensions is shown in Figure 2. Chocolate with taga-
that chocolate with tagatose approximates the choc- tose and chocolate with sugar were situated on the
olate with sugar. The use of stevia leads to the largest side where positive emotional conceptualisations dom-
differences with chocolate with sugar. inate (left side). In contrast, chocolate with stevia was
situated on the side where negative emotional concep-
tualisations are situated (right side). To have a visual
Emotional conceptualisations: a comparison of representation of how liking is associated with the
chocolate with tagatose and chocolate with stevia emotional conceptualisations, overall liking was added
in relation to chocolate with sugar as a supplementary category. This did not affect the
Significant differences in the frequency of use among configuration of the emotional conceptualisation and
the different types of chocolate were found for 14 the orientation of the axes. On the right side of the
emotional terms, 10 positive and four negative. plot, negative overall liking is situated and on the left
Positive emotional conceptualisations are more associ- side, positive overall liking is displayed. Negative over-
ated with chocolate with sugar and chocolate with all liking was associated with negative emotional con-
tagatose. “Feeling good” and “pleased” has the highest ceptualisations as well as with chocolate with stevia.
values for chocolate with sugar and chocolate with Positive overall liking was associated with positive
tagatose. The positive emotional conceptualisation emotional conceptualisation as well as with chocolate
“feeling good” is more discriminating between the with tagatose or sugar. Mapping of the emotional
low-calorie sweeteners tagatose and stevia and the terms in a semantic space as proposed by Spinelli
positive emotional conceptualisation “pleased” is more et al. (2014) showed that sugar and tagatose were
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCES AND NUTRITION 7

Table 5. Comparison of emotional conceptualisations of chocolate with tagatose and stevia in relation to chocolate with sugar.
Significant differences in frequency of emotional conceptualisations (%) (n ¼ 219).
Regular Low-calorie sweetener Sub-sample differences
Chocolate þ sugar Chocolate þ tagatose Chocolate þ stevia
Cochran’s Q p Value
% % %
Positive Feeling good 47.49a 48.86a 30.14b 27.49 .000
Pleased 45.66a 40.18a 23.74b 32.84 .000
Calm 43.38 42.47 35.16 5.36 .069
Interesting 29.68a 27.85a 18.72b 11.81 .003
Pleasant 29.22a 26.03a 11.42b 30.88 .000
Satisfying 27.40a 19.63b 12.79c 19.72 .000
Happy 24.20a 26.48a 17.35b 10.31 .006
Glad 24.66a 25.57a 11.87b 24.46 .000
Peaceful 23.29 21.46 17.35 3.75 .154
Good-natured 18.72a 21.00a 11.87b 11.61 .003
Enthusiastic 14.16a 15.53a 6.85b 12.28 .002
Enjoyment 12.79 13.24 8.68 3.64 .162
Joyful 12.33 15.53 11.87 2.43 .297
Stimulating 10.05a 10.05a 2.74b 12.80 .002
Active 5.94a,b 9.59a 4.57b 6.93 .03
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

Negative Disappointment 8.22b 12.79b 34.25a 60.41 .000


Unpleasant surprise 10.05b 9.13b 31.96a 55.91 .000
Dissatisfying 8.22b 11.87b 27.85a 41.29 .000
Disgusted 5.48b 4.11b 15.98a 27.59 .000
Tame 9.59 12.79 14.16 4.16 .125
Quiet 16.44 13.24 13.24 2.33 .311
Bored 5.48 7.76 10.05 4.29 .117
Sad 3.20 3.65 5.02 1.44 .489
Anger 2.74 2.28 3.65 1.00 .607
p < .01. Repeated measures ANOVA, abSignificantly different (p  .05) Paired t-test. abcRow indicates which sample are significantly different from each
other on the emotional conceptualisation. Each chocolate sample with different letters is significantly different (p  .05). No letter in row indicates that
the emotional conceptualisation is not significantly different. Bold numbers refer to the highest significant percentage in rows.

situated together on the valence dimension (positive – Cronbach’s alpha. The Cronbach’s alpha for the fac-
negative), but appeared to be slightly different on the tors has a good reliability on the health scale (General
activation dimension (low-high arousal). Chocolate health interest: 0.831; Light product interest: 0.784;
with tagatose was situated at the high arousal (higher Natural product interest: 0.76), an acceptable reliability
activation) side of the dimension and chocolate with on the taste scale (Consuming food for pleasure:
sugar at the low arousal (lower activation) side. This 0.621; Using food as reward: 0.721; Craving for sweet
means that chocolate with sugar and chocolate with foods: 0.762) and a very high reliability of the emo-
tagatose were associated with different types of posi- tional eating scale (0.907). Table 6 gives an overview
tive emotional conceptualisations. Chocolate with of the means and standard deviations (SD) for the
sugar was linked to the emotional conceptualisations health and taste attitudes and emotional eating.
“pleased” and “pleasant”, whereas chocolate with taga- Participants considered “Pleasure” as the most import-
tose was related to “enthusiastic” and “joyful”. As ant attitude, as they have scored this the highest on
shown, valence positively correlated with liking, but the health and taste attitudes questionnaire, followed
activation was not that straightforwardly related to lik- by “General health interest” and “Using food as a
ing. For the chocolate with stevia, there was no differ- reward”.
ence on the arousal dimension, as it is situated at the To understand the role of sensory evaluation and
midline of the dimension. emotional conceptualisations when comparing the
chocolates, differences according to socio-demograph-
ics, eating behaviour and attitudes were analysed
Consumers’ emotional eating behaviour, health
(Table 7). For age, for example, overall liking of choc-
and taste attitudes related to acceptability and
olate with stevia differed significantly from the other
emotional conceptualisations: a comparison of
chocolate types for every age category. Thereby, the
chocolate with tagatose and chocolate with stevia
overall liking of participants older than 46 years was
in relation to chocolate with sugar
significantly lower than for other age categories for
The internal reliability of each factor of the health and chocolate with tagatose and chocolate with sugar. For
taste attitudes questionnaire (HTAS) and for emo- BMI category, preference and emotional eating behav-
tional eating of the DEBQ-e was calculated by use of iour, only non-significant differences were reported,
8 S. LAGAST ET AL.
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

Figure 2. Graphical presentation of a correspondence analysis of the emotional conceptualisations together with the three different
chocolate samples. Four quadrants (I–IV) are distinguished, based on two dimensions of semantic space (Arousal, Valence), as pro-
posed by Spinelli et al. (2014) (I: valence positive, arousal low; II: valence negative, arousal high; III: valence positive, arousal high;
IV: valence negative; arousal low). The filled labels represent the three different types of chocolate, the unfilled labels represent
emotional conceptualisations, “X” refers to overall liking score.

Table 6. Means and standard deviations (SD) for health and taste attitudes and emotional eating (n ¼ 219).
Questionnaire Factors Mean SD
Health and taste attitudes Pleasure 5.06 0.81
General health interest 4.42 0.92
Using food as reward 4.12 1.10
Craving for sweet foods 3.92 1.07
Interest in natural products 3.72 0.99
Interest in light products 3.34 0.95
Eating behaviour Emotional eating 2.49 0.69
Factors based on factor analysis (Maximum likelihood with Varimax rotation). Items about health and taste attributes were meas-
ured by the validated questionnaire Health and Taste Attitude Scale (HTAS), attitudes were rated on 7-point Likert scale ranging
from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Items about emotional eating behaviour were measured by the validated emotional
eating scale of the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ-e) on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often).

similar as for health and taste attitudes (the latter not chocolate with tagatose (Figure 3(b)) are similar and
presented here). differ from the spider plot for chocolate with stevia
The spider plots of Figure 3(a–c) represent the pro- (Figure 3(c)). For chocolate with sugar (Figure 3(a)),
portioned frequency for the three groups of partici- there was a significant difference between the three
pants when characterised as being low, moderate or groups of emotional eaters for the emotional terms:
high emotional eaters according to the DEBQ-e for “stimulating” and “anger”. For chocolate with tagatose
the three different chocolates. The spider plots for the (Figure 3(b)), the emotional terms “pleasant”, “glad”
chocolate with sugar (Figure 3(a)) and for the and “enthusiastic” were checked more by the high
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCES AND NUTRITION 9

Table 7. Differences in mean liking by socio-demographics and eating behaviour.


Overall liking
Regular Low-calorie sweeteners
Chocolate þ sugar Chocolate þ tagatose Chocolate þ stevia
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD F stat. p Value
Socio-demographics
Age category
18–25 years 4.85aA 1.40 4.68aA 1.33 3.02aB 1.37 4.48 .002
26–45 years 4.59abA 1.47 4.71aA 1.19 3.02aB 1.41
46þ years 4.26bA 1.54 3.82bAB 1.49 3.38aB 1.51
BMI category
Underweight (<18.5) 4.53 1.59 4.24 1.39 2.88 1.50 1.59 .178
Normal weight (18.5–25) 4.71 1.45 4.68 1.34 3.12 1.39
Overweight (>25) 4.65 1.42 3.88 1.40 3.05 1.41
Preference
White 4.93 1.33 4.52 1.28 2.96 1.45 1.21 .306
Milk 4.63 1.36 4.26 1.40 2.94 1.28
Dark 4.64 1.62 4.76 1.36 3.35 1.53
Eating behaviour
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

Emotional eating
Low 4.65 1.73 3.76 1.60 2.22 1.78 0.99 .412
Moderate 4.70 1.33 4.46 1.37 3.05 1.33
High 4.67 1.59 4.68 1.39 3.24 1.44
Note: Mixed factorial ANOVA with Bonferroni post-hoc between age categories and chocolates. Mean overall liking was used as a within-subjects variable
and age, BMI, preference and emotional eating as between-subjects variable for each chocolate sample.
Mean liking with different letters (ab) in same column and mean liking with different letters (AB) in same row are significantly different (p  .05). Only sig-
nificant differences in both rows and columns are presented. significance p < .05.

emotional eaters. The emotional term “sad” discrimi- (de Melo et al. 2009; Shah et al. 2010; Cadena et al.
nated between the three groups for chocolate with ste- 2013; Voorpostel et al. 2014). Overall liking of choc-
via (Figure 3(c)). In average, the high emotional eaters olate did not differ significantly between chocolate
selected a larger amount of emotional terms than the with sugar and chocolate with tagatose. These results
other emotional eaters (low and moderate) across all are in line with a study that shows tagatose to have
chocolates. comparable physical attributes to sugar when the
sugar is not completely substituted, as is the case in
the chocolate with tagatose of this study (0.3 g sugar
Discussion
remained in 100 g) (Taylor et al. 2008).
This study examined consumers’ sensory evaluation as The results showed a significant difference between
well as emotional conceptualisations upon consump- the two low-calorie sweeteners for four of the five sen-
tion of three types of dark chocolate: one with sugar sory attributes investigated in this study: texture, sweet
and two with low-calorie sweeteners: tagatose and ste- flavour, bitter flavour and duration of aftertaste. The
via. Three comparisons have been made: (1) a com- study did not find a significant difference for the
parison of the two low-calorie sweeteners in chocolate intensity of aftertaste. For texture, there was only a
in relation to sugar in chocolate on overall liking and significant difference between the two low-calorie
sensory attributes, (2) a comparison of emotional con- sweeteners and not between the low-calorie sweeteners
ceptualisations associated with the chocolates and (3) and sugar. Stevia has been shown to retain hardness
a comparison of consumers’ emotional eating behav- of chocolate under specific circumstances. Research of
iour and health and taste attitudes in relation to Shah et al. (2010) showed no substantial effect on the
acceptability and emotional conceptualisations. hardness of chocolate when sucrose was replaced by
Regarding the first comparison (1), the overall lik- stevia as sweetening agent only when inulin and poly-
ing and sensory attributes of the three types of choc- dextrose are used as bulking agents and only in choco-
olate, the results showed a significantly lower overall lates without inulin HPX. For a sweet flavour, both
liking for chocolate with stevia as low-calorie sweet- low-calorie sweeteners significantly differed from
ener compared to chocolate with sugar or with the chocolate with sugar but chocolate with tagatose was
other low-calorie sweetener, tagatose. Several studies significantly perceived as sweeter than chocolate with
have found similar lower levels of liking and accept- stevia. Just like in de Melo et al. (2009) chocolate
ance for stevia products, such as mango nectar, grape with sugar is the sweetest, followed by chocolate with
nectar and chocolate compared to sugar products tagatose and chocolate with stevia is the least sweet.
10 S. LAGAST ET AL.

chocolate is perceived as more similar to sugar than


the low-calorie sweetener stevia. This conclusion con-
firms previous research (Livesey and Brown 1996;
Shankar et al. 2013; Li et al. 2015). Chocolate with ste-
via differed from chocolate with sugar on overall lik-
ing, sweetness, bitterness and duration of aftertaste.
Chocolate with tagatose is similar to chocolate with
sugar on overall liking, texture, bitterness, duration of
aftertaste and intensity of aftertaste. Although both are
significantly different from sweetness, chocolate with
tagatose was closer to chocolate with sugar on sweet-
ness than chocolate with stevia.
Regarding the second comparison of the study (2),
the emotional conceptualisations consumers associate
with the types of chocolates, the results showed sig-
nificant differences between chocolate with tagatose
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

and chocolate with stevia. Chocolate with tagatose was


significantly more associated with positive emotional
conceptualisations like the results of chocolate with
sugar. Chocolate with stevia, on the other hand,
aroused mostly negative emotions.
Although the emotional conceptualisations are quite
similar, two differences between chocolate with taga-
tose and chocolate with sugar are worth mentioning.
First, the semantic space as proposed by Spinelli et al.
(2014) gives added information on consumer’s percep-
tions of chocolate with sugar and chocolate with taga-
tose. By mapping the results in the semantic space,
sugar and tagatose appeared to be slightly different on
the activation dimension (low-high arousal). Second,
Figure 3. Emotional conceptualisations (%) segmented accord- on the positive emotional term “satisfying”, chocolate
ing to emotional eating behaviour measured with DEBQ-e. with tagatose showed a significant difference with
Emotional terms with  are significantly different among the chocolate with sugar. Yet, chocolate with tagatose was
three groups at the significant level of 0.05 and 0.01. (a) still significantly more associated with the term
Chocolate with sugar, (b) chocolate with tagatose and (c) choc- “satisfying” than chocolate with stevia.
olate with stevia.
Whereas other research only showed comparable
physical attributes and sweetness of sugar and taga-
For bitter flavour, the use of stevia resulted in a more
tose (Livesey and Brown 1996; Shankar et al. 2013),
pronounced bitterness than tagatose which was similar
comparable to the first comparison in this study, the
to sugar. This corresponds with previous research
second comparison indicates that sugar and tagatose
showing an enhancing effect of stevia on non-sweet
also elicit comparable (positive) emotional conceptu-
off flavours such as bitterness (Prakash et al. 2008) alisations. Other recent studies have stressed the
and a weakening effect of sucrose on bitterness added and unique information of emotional
(Prawira and Barringer 2009). For the duration of an responses to food, which can give new information
aftertaste, the results of this study support the longer for product development (King and Meiselman 2010;
aftertaste for chocolate with stevia (de Melo et al. Thomson et al. 2010; Cardello et al. 2012; Gutjar
2009). Yet, for the intensity of aftertaste, like the pre- et al. 2015).
vious study of de Melo et al. (2009), there was no sig- The third comparison (3), examines how consum-
nificant difference in intensity of aftertaste between ers’ emotional behaviour and health and taste attitudes
tagatose and stevia, nor between the two low-calorie are related to acceptability and emotional conceptuali-
sweeteners and sugar. sations. Chocolate consumers were profiled based on
When concluding on the comparison of the sensory socio-demographics, consumer behaviour, eating
attributes, the low-calorie sweetener tagatose in behaviour and attitudes.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCES AND NUTRITION 11

For acceptability, the study showed that mean over- This method relies on participants consciously and
all liking for the chocolate with tagatose was lower for explicitly stating their experienced emotions. Although
the participants older than 46 years. Sensory percep- this is a commonly used method in food-elicited emo-
tion declines with age and stronger flavours are tion research, the emphasis is often put on what the
increasingly preferred (Murphy and Withee 1986; product is communicating to the consumer instead of
Schiffman and Warwick 1993; de Graaf et al. 1996; what the product is really doing to them (Thomson
Mojet et al. 2005). et al. 2010). Future research should also focus on the
For emotional conceptualisations, the emotional non-self-reported and implicit measurement of emo-
profile was linked to the consumption of the choco- tions by tracking psychophysiological responses, such
lates and the emotional eating behaviour of the partic- as facial expressions, skin conductance or brain activ-
ipants. The results showed that the group of high ity (K€oster and Mojet 2015). Facial expression analysis,
emotional eaters selected a larger amount of emotional for instance, may contribute to detecting rapid, uncon-
terms on average than the low and moderate emo- trollable emotional responses that influence liking and
tional eaters across all chocolates. This result is in line preference of products but that cannot be consciously
with previous research of Piqueras-Fiszman and Jaeger stated by participants. Both ways of measurements can
(2014) who have shown a higher frequency of emo- also be combined. One study combining explicit (con-
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

tional words checked by high emotional eaters for a joint analysis) and implicit (facial expression) meas-
chocolate brownie. The result is also in line with urements found that some designs of packages
Jaeger and Hedderley (2013) who showed the intensity generated happiness more than other designs (Pentus
of emotion varied among individual emotional traits. et al. 2014). Other studies found different results
These results hint at implementing emotional profiles between explicit and implicit measurements. A recent
for participants to avoid overrepresentation of high study on sweeteners in black tea found a differenti-
emotional eaters. ation between liked versus disliked sweeteners when
The first limitation of this study is that the study using verbal responses, but no differentiation when
used only blind sensory evaluation of the chocolates. analysing facial responses (Leitch et al. 2015; Mojet
As a result, the participants had no knowledge about et al. 2015). Although implicit measurements merit
the composition of the chocolates and had conse- attention, this study relied on explicit measurements
quently no idea of the reduced amount of sugar in the as a commonly used method in food-elicited emotion
chocolates. The study opted for blind evaluation to research.
avoid bias. The altered composition of the chocolates The preference and consumption of milk chocolate
could affect the expectations and consequently the in, respectively, 58% and 49% of the participants is
evaluation of the types of chocolate. Torres-Moreno the third limitation of this study. The dominance of
et al. (2012) confirmed that consumers had a signifi- those preferring milk chocolate over dark chocolate
cantly higher liking for a premium brand chocolate could be a confounding factor. Nevertheless, to be
compared to a standard brand in informed testing. selected, participants were asked whether they con-
However, in blind testing, there was no difference in sumed all three types of chocolate (white, milk and
the consumers liking of both chocolates. Varela et al. dark). Before the study started, they were informed
(2010) showed an influence of previous experiences, that the study would be on dark chocolate.
information on the label, the appearance and package Furthermore, the focus of the study was on the sweet-
on the sensory and hedonic expectations. Nutritional eners used in chocolate, rather than comparing dark
information also drives sensory evaluation into the versus other chocolates. As a consequence, participants
direction of expectations (Schouteten et al. 2015; who dislike dark chocolate were excluded, by which
Tuorila 2015). For example, the evaluation of fattiness one can assume that the effect of those preferring
of a chocolate bar with a reduced fat label was lower milk chocolate on the evaluation of dark chocolate is
in comparison to blind testing (K€ahk€ onen and Tuorila negligible. The results also showed no significant effect
1999) although overall evaluation did not to seem to of preference or consumption on overall liking of the
differ (Norton et al. 2013). As this study focussed on chocolates (Table 7).
low-calorie sweeteners to reduce sugar, knowledge
about the reduced amount of sugar could, in the same
Conclusion
way, affect the consumers’ perception.
A second limitation refers to the self-reported and This study looked at two low-calorie sweeteners, taga-
explicit measurement of emotional conceptualisations, tose and stevia, to reduce sugar in dark chocolate by
by asking participants to check the applicable emotion. making three comparisons. When comparing overall
12 S. LAGAST ET AL.

liking and sensory attributes, the low-calorie sweetener Cardello AV, Meiselman HL, Schutz HG, Craig C, Given Z,
tagatose in chocolate is perceived as more similar to Lesher LL, Eicher S. 2012. Measuring emotional responses
to foods and food names using questionnaires. Food Qual
sugar than the low-calorie sweetener stevia. The
Prefer. 24:243–250.
second comparison indicates that sugar and tagatose Coleman KL, Miah EM, Morris GA, Morris C. 2014. Impact
also elicit comparable (positive) emotional conceptual- of health claims in prebiotic-enriched breads on purchase
isations. The third comparison showed different liking intent, emotional response and product liking. Int J Food
of tagatose in different age groups with a lower liking Sci Nutr. 65:164–171.
Dalenberg JR, Gutjar S, Ter Horst GJ, De Graaf K, Renken
for participants older than 46 years. To reduce sugar
RJ, Jager G. 2014. Evoked emotions predict food choice.
intake without changing the sensory perception nor PLoS One. 9:e115388.
the emotional conceptualisation tagatose seems to be a de Graaf C, Kramer FM, Meiselman HL, Lesher LL, Baker-
promising low-calorie sweetener for dark chocolate as Fulco C, Hirsch ES, Warber J. 2005. Food acceptability in
this study showed it does not differ significantly from field studies with US army men and women: relationship
sugar in overall liking, most sensory attributes and with food intake and food choice after repeated expo-
sures. Appetite. 44:23–31.
emotional conceptualisation. de Graaf C, van Staveren W, Burema J. 1996.
Psychophysical and psychohedonic functions of four
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

common food flavours in elderly subjects. Chem Senses.


Disclosure statement 21:293–302.
The authors report no conflicts of interest. de Melo LLMM, Bolini HMA, Efraim P. 2009. Sensory pro-
file, acceptability, and their relationship for diabetic/
reduced calorie chocolates. Food Qual Prefer. 20:138–143.
Funding de Pelsmaeker S, Schouteten J, Gellynck X. 2013. The con-
sumption of flavored milk among a children population.
This study was funded independently from chocolate or The influence of beliefs and the association of brands
other industries. with emotions. Appetite. 71:279–286.
de Wijk RA, He W, Mensink MG, Verhoeven RH, De Graaf
C. 2014. ANS responses and facial expressions differenti-
ORCID ate between the taste of commercial breakfast drinks.
Sofie Lagast http://orcid.org/0000-0002-7912-9397 PLoS One. 9:e93823.
Hans De Steur http://orcid.org/0000-0003-1340-0882 de Wijk RA, Kooijman V, Verhoeven RH, Holthuysen NT,
Joachim J. Schouteten http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2030- de Graaf C. 2012. Autonomic nervous system responses
5608 on and facial expressions to the sight, smell, and taste of
Xavier Gellynck http://orcid.org/0000-0002-8908-3310 liked and disliked foods. Food Qual Prefer. 26:196–203.
Desmet PM, Schifferstein HN. 2008. Sources of positive and
negative emotions in food experience. Appetite.
50:290–301.
References Drewnowski A, Rehm CD. 2014. Consumption of low-cal-
orie sweeteners among US adults is associated with
Anderson GH, Foreyt J, Sigman-Grant M, Allison DB. 2012. higher Healthy Eating Index (HEI 2005) scores and more
The use of low-calorie sweeteners by adults: impact on physical activity. Nutrients. 6:4389–4403.
weight management. J Nutr. 142:1163S–1169S. Ferrarini R, Carbognin C, Casarotti E, Nicolis E, Nencini A,
Bellisle F, Altenburg de Assis M, Fieux B, Preziosi P, Galan Meneghini A. 2010. The emotional response to wine con-
P, Guy-Grand B, Hercberg S. 2001. Use of ‘light’ foods sumption. Food Qual Prefer. 21:720–725.
and drinks in French adults: biological, anthropometric Fujimaru T, Park JH, Lim J. 2012. Sensory characteristics
and nutritional correlates. J Hum Nutr Diet. 14:191–206. and relative sweetness of tagatose and other sweeteners. J
Bellisle F, Drewnowski A. 2007. Intense sweeteners, energy Food Sci. 77:S323–S328.
intake and the control of body weight. Eur J Clin Nutr. Gardner C, Wylie-Rosett J, Gidding SS, Steffen LM, Johnson
61:691–700. RK, Reader D, Lichtenstein AH. 2012. Nonnutritive sweet-
Bleich SN, Wolfson JA, Vine S, Wang YC. 2014. Diet-bever- eners: current use and health perspectives: a scientific
age consumption and caloric intake among US adults, statement from the American Heart Association and the
overall and by body weight. Am J Public Health. American Diabetes Association. Circulation. 126:509–519.
104:e72–e78. Ghosh S, Sudha M. 2012. A review on polyols: new frontiers
Bruinsma K, Taren DL. 1999. Chocolate: food or drug? J for health-based bakery products. Int J Food Sci Nutr.
Am Diet Assoc. 99:1249–1256. 63:372–379.
Cadena RS, Cruz AG, Netto RR, Castro WF, Faria JDaF, Goyal S, Goyal R. 2010. Stevia (Stevia rebaudiana) a bio-
Bolini HMA. 2013. Sensory profile and physicochemical sweetener: a review. Int J Food Sci Nutr. 61:1–10.
characteristics of mango nectar sweetened with high Gutjar S, De Graaf C, Kooijman V, De Wijk RA, Nys A,
intensity sweeteners throughout storage time. Food Res Ter Horst GJ, Jager G. 2015. The role of emotions in
Int. 54:1670–1679. food choice and liking. Food Res Int. 76:216–223.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCES AND NUTRITION 13

Hellemann U, Tuorila H. 1991. Pleasantness ratings and Norton J, Fryer P, Parkinson J. 2013. The effect of reduced-
consumption of open sandwiches with varying NaCl and fat labelling on chocolate expectations. Food Qual Prefer.
acid contents. Appetite. 17:229–238. 28:101–105.
Hetherington MM, Macdiarmid JI. 1995. Pleasure and Pentus K, Mehine T, Kuusik A. 2014. Considering emotions
excess: liking for and overconsumption of chocolate. in product package design through combining conjoint
Physiol Behav. 57:27–35. analysis with psycho physiological measurements.
Jaeger SR, Hedderley DI. 2013. Impact of individual differen- Procedia Soc Behav Sci. 148:280–290.
ces in emotional intensity and private body consciousness Piqueras-Fiszman B, Jaeger SR. 2014. The impact of evoked
on EsSense ProfileV responses. Food Qual Prefer. 27:54–62.
R
consumption contexts and appropriateness on emotion
Johnson AR, Stewart DW. 2005. A reappraisal of the role of responses. Food Qual Prefer. 32:277–288.
emotion in consumer behavior. Review of marketing Prakash I, Dubois G, Clos J, Wilkens K, Fosdick L. 2008.
research. Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing Limited. Development of rebiana, a natural, non-caloric sweetener.
K€ahk€onen P, Tuorila H. 1999. Consumer responses to Food Chem Toxicol. 46:S75–S82.
reduced and regular fat content in different products: Prawira M, Barringer S. 2009. Effects of conching time and
effects of gender, involvement and health concern. Food ingredients on preference of milk chocolate. J Food
Qual Prefer. 10:83–91. Process Preserv. 33:571–589.
King SC, Meiselman HL. 2010. Development of a method to Robin O, Rousmans S, Dittmar A, Vernet-Maury E. 2000.
measure consumer emotions associated with foods. Food Autonomic estimated basic emotions induced by primary
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017

Qual Prefer. 21:168–177. tastes. Eur J Clin Nutr. 54:S14–S15.


King SC, Meiselman HL, Carr BT. 2013. Measuring emo- Roininen K, Tuorila H, Zandstra E, De Graaf C, Vehkalahti
tions associated with foods: important elements of ques- K, Stubenitsky K, Mela DJ. 2001. Differences in health
tionnaire and test design. Food Qual Prefer. 28:8–16. and taste attitudes and reported behaviour among
K€oster EP, Mojet J. 2015. From mood to food and from Finnish, Dutch and British consumers: a cross-national
food to mood: a psychological perspective on the meas- validation of the Health and Taste Attitude Scales
urement of food-related emotions in consumer research. (HTAS). Appetite. 37:33–45.
Food Res Int. 76:180–191. Schifferstein HN, Desmet PM. 2010. Hedonic asymmetry in
Leitch K, Duncan S, O’Keefe S, Rudd R, Gallagher D. 2015. emotional responses to consumer products. Food Qual
Characterizing consumer emotional response to sweet- Prefer. 21:1100–1104.
eners using an emotion terminology questionnaire and Schiffman SS, Warwick ZS. 1993. Effect of flavor enhance-
facial expression analysis. Food Res Int. 76:283–292. ment of foods for the elderly on nutritional status: food
Li X, Lopetcharat K, Drake M. 2015. Parents’ and children’s intake, biochemical indices, and anthropometric meas-
acceptance of skim chocolate milks sweetened by monk ures. Physiol Behav. 53:395–402.
fruit and stevia leaf extracts. J Food Sci. 80:S1083–S1092. Schouteten JJ, De Steur H, De Pelsmaeker S, Lagast S, De
Livesey G, Brown J. 1996. D-tagatose is a bulk sweetener Bourdeaudhuij I, Gellynck X. 2015. Impact of health
with zero energy determined in rats. J Nutr. labels on flavor perception and emotional profiling: a
126:1601–1609. consumer study on cheese. Nutrients. 7:10251–10268.
Ludwig DS. 2009. Artificially sweetened beverages: cause for Shah AB, Jones GP, Vasiljevic T. 2010. Sucrose-free chocolate
concern. JAMA. 302:2477–2478. sweetened with Stevia rebaudiana extract and containing
Macht M, Dettmer D. 2006. Everyday mood and emotions different bulking agents – effects on physicochemical and
after eating a chocolate bar or an apple. Appetite. sensory properties. Int J Food Sci Tech. 45:1426–1435.
46:332–336. Shankar P, Ahuja S, Sriram K. 2013. Non-nutritive sweet-
Manzocco L, Rumignani A, Lagazio C. 2013. Emotional eners: review and update. Nutrition. 29:1293–1299.
response to fruit salads with different visual quality. Food Spinelli S, Masi C, Dinnella C, Zoboli GP, Monteleone E.
Qual Prefer. 28:17–22. 2014. How does it make you feel? A new approach to
Mojet J, Christ-Hazelhof E, Heidema J. 2005. Taste percep- measuring emotions in food product experience. Food
tion with age: pleasantness and its relationships with Qual Prefer. 37:109–122.
threshold sensitivity and supra-threshold intensity of five Taylor T, Fasina O, Bell L. 2008. Physical properties and
taste qualities. Food Qual Prefer. 16:413–423. consumer liking of cookies prepared by replacing sucrose
Mojet J, D€ urrschmid K, Danner L, J€ ochl M, Heini€ o R-L, with tagatose. J Food Sci. 73:S145–S151.
Holthuysen N, K€ oster E. 2015. Are implicit emotion Thomson DM, Crocker C. 2013. A data-driven classification
measurements evoked by food unrelated to liking? Food of feelings. Food Qual Prefer. 27:137–152.
Res Int. 76:224–232. Thomson DM, Crocker C, Marketo CG. 2010. Linking sen-
Moskowitz HR, Beckley JH, Resurreccion AV. 2012. Sensory sory characteristics to emotions: an example using dark
and consumer research in food product design and devel- chocolate. Food Qual Prefer. 21:1117–1125.
opment. Ames, IA: John Wiley & Sons. Torres-Moreno M, Tarrega A, Torrescasana E, Blanch C.
Murphy C, Withee J. 1986. Age-related differences in the 2012. Influence of label information on dark chocolate
pleasantness of chemosensory stimuli. Psychol Aging. acceptability. Appetite. 58:665–671.
1:312–318. Tuorila H. 2015. From sensory evaluation to sensory and
Ng M, Chaya C, Hort J. 2013. Beyond liking: comparing the consumer research of food: an autobiographical perspec-
measurement of emotional response using EsSense Profile tive. Food Qual Prefer. 40:255–262.
and consumer defined check-all-that-apply methodolo- Van Strien T, Frijters JE, Bergers G, Defares PB. 1986. The
gies. Food Qual Prefer. 28:193–205. Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for
14 S. LAGAST ET AL.

assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating World Health Organization. 2015. Guideline: sugars intake for
behavior. Int J Eat Disord. 5:295–315. adults and children. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Varela P, Ares G, Gimenez A, Gambaro A. 2010. Influence Zhao L, Tepper BJ. 2007. Perception and acceptance of
of brand information on consumers’ expectations and lik- selected high-intensity sweeteners and blends in model
ing of powdered drinks in central location tests. Food soft drinks by propylthiouracil (PROP) non-tasters and
Qual Prefer. 21:873–880. super-tasters. Food Qual Prefer. 18:531–540.
Voorpostel CR, Dutra Mbdl, Bolini HMA. 2014. Sensory Zorn S, Alcaire F, Vidal L, Gimenez A, Ares G. 2014.
profile and drivers of liking for grape nectar among Application of multiple-sip temporal dominance of sensa-
smoker and nonsmoker consumers. Food Sci Technol tions to the evaluation of sweeteners. Food Qual Prefer.
(Campinas). 34:164–173. 36:135–143.
Downloaded by [Australian Catholic University] at 17:10 15 August 2017