You are on page 1of 8

Epilepsia, 53(12):2156–2163, 2012

doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03706.x

FULL-LENGTH ORIGINAL RESEARCH

Neurocognitive profiles in children with epilepsy


*Claudia L. Kernan, *yRobert Asarnow, *Prabha Siddarth, zSuresh Gurbani, xErin K. Lanphier,
{Raman Sankar, and *Rochelle Caplan

*Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, UCLA, Los Angeles, California, U.S.A.; yDepartment of Psychology,
UCLA, Los Angeles, California, U.S.A.; zDepartment of Neurology, Kaiser Permanente, Anaheim, California, U.S.A.;
xDepartment of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, U.S.A.; and {Department of Pediatrics, UCLA,
Los Angeles, California, U.S.A.

Key Findings: When compared to controls, children with


SUMMARY
CPS showed a mild generalized cognitive deficit, whereas
Purpose: The presence of specific neurocognitive deficits children with CAE did not. When we controlled for intelli-
may help explain why school achievement and psychoso- gent quotient (IQ), both epilepsy groups showed poorer
cial functioning are often worse in children with epilepsy performance relative to controls in the domain of verbal
than would be predicted by their global intellectual func- memory. When the epilepsy groups were compared to one
tioning. This study compared children with two forms of another, the CPS group performed significantly poorer
epilepsy: localization-related epilepsy with complex par- than the CAE group on a test of generalized cognitive func-
tial seizures (CPS) and childhood absence epilepsy (CAE), tioning. However, in the specific domains of executive func-
to determine whether they display distinct neurocogni- tioning, verbal memory, and visual memory the epilepsy
tive profiles. groups did not differ when compared to one another.
Methods: Fifty-one children with CPS, 31 children with Significance: Neurocognitive deficits present in the
CAE, and 51 controls underwent neuropsychological test- context of grossly intact global intellectual functioning
ing assessing verbal memory, visual memory, and execu- highlight the importance of neuropsychological screening
tive functioning. Groups were compared in these in both children with CPS and children with CAE.
cognitive domains. Within-group analyses were also con- KEY WORDS: Childhood absence epilepsy, Complex
ducted to examine seizure-related factors that may be partial seizures, Neuropsychology, Memory, Executive
related to neuropsychological test performance. functions, Child.

Although a significant proportion of children with severe Past research has indicated that children with epilepsy
forms of epilepsy score below average on intelligence tests, display specific cognitive weaknesses, with a number of
the majority of children with epilepsy have intellectual func- studies demonstrating deficits in the domains of memory
tioning within the average range (Berg et al., 2008). Despite and executive functioning (Jambaque et al., 1993; Bailet &
their mostly intact global intellectual functioning, however, Turk, 2000; Lassonde et al., 2000; Pavone et al., 2001;
children with epilepsy as a group are at high risk for poor Culhane-Shelburne et al., 2002; Nolan et al., 2004; Henkin
academic functioning (Sillanpaa, 2004; Aldenkamp et al., et al., 2005; Hoie et al., 2005; Borden et al., 2006; Hermann
2005; Fastenau et al., 2008) and negative psychosocial et al., 2006; Hommet et al., 2006; Jocic-Jakubi & Jovic,
outcomes (Wirrell et al., 1997; Sillanpaa et al., 1998). 2006; MacAllister & Schaffer, 2007; Parrish et al., 2007;
Researchers have therefore highlighted the importance of Seidenberg et al., 2007; Hermann et al., 2008; Pulsipher
neuropsychological testing in children with epilepsy, positing et al., 2009). However, the question of whether children
that specific cognitive deficits in domains other than general with different types of seizures or epilepsy syndromes show
intelligence may help explain why school achievement and distinct cognitive profiles has yet to be resolved in the litera-
psychosocial functioning are often worse than would be pre- ture. Most studies to date looking at cognitive functioning in
dicted by their intelligence scores alone (Nolan et al., 2004; children with epilepsy have compared children with epi-
Jocic-Jakubi & Jovic, 2006; Fastenau et al., 2008). lepsy to a healthy control group or standard scores. The epi-
lepsy groups in these studies have been heterogeneous,
Accepted August 16, 2012; Early View publication November 5, 2012. limiting the conclusions that can be drawn in terms of delin-
Address correspondence to Claudia L. Kernan, Semel Institute for Neu-
roscience and Human Behavior, 760 Westwood Plaza, Los Angeles, CA
eating distinct cognitive profiles. Although several recent
90024, U.S.A. E-mail: ckernan@mednet.ucla.edu studies have directly compared children with different types
Wiley Periodicals, Inc. of seizures and epilepsy syndromes, findings have been
ª 2012 International League Against Epilepsy mixed with regard to whether there are differences among

2156
2157
Neurocognitive Profiles in Pediatric Epilepsy

epilepsy groups. In addition, children have been grouped studies of neurocognitive functioning in children with epi-
together using different criteria in terms of type of seizure lepsy, a healthy control group, and groups with comparable
and epilepsy syndrome, making comparisons across studies demographic variables. If patterns specific to epilepsy
difficult. group and clinical seizure variables can be identified, this
For example, some studies have compared children with could aid in the early identification of children at risk of
generalized seizures to children with focal epilepsies and developing cognitive problems and in tailoring interven-
found that children with focal epilepsies perform poorer on tions that address their specific needs.
tests of memory (Jambaque et al., 1993; Nolan et al., 2004).
Other studies comparing children with localization-related
epilepsies to children with generalized seizures have not
Methods
found significant differences in memory functioning (Wil- Subjects
liams et al., 1998, 2001; Hermann et al., 2006). In the The current study included 51 children with CPS, 31 chil-
domain of executive functioning, some studies comparing dren with CAE, and 51 controls, aged 6–16 years with IQ
children with localization-related epilepsy to children with scores between 70 and 130. Table 1 presents demographic
idiopathic generalized epilepsy have found no or small dif- features of the sample and the recruitment sites of the sub-
ferences between the groups (Hermann et al., 2006; Parrish jects. We determined socioeconomic status (SES) using the
et al., 2007). Similarly, a study comparing children with Hollingshead 2 factor index (Hollingshead, 1973), based on
absence seizures to children with complex partial seizures parental occupational and educational status. As summa-
found no differences among the groups in executive func- rized in Table 1, there were significantly more Caucasian
tioning (Williams et al., 1998). In contrast, a study compar- subjects in the CPS group than in the CAE and control
ing children with juvenile myoclonic epilepsy (JME) to groups (v2 = 6.5, p < 0.05). There were no significant dif-
children with benign childhood epilepsy with centrotempo- ferences in the demographic variables of the CPS, CAE, and
ral spikes (BCETS) found children with JME to have signifi- control groups.
cant executive dysfunction, whereas children with BCECTS The primary study inclusion criterion for each epilepsy
did not show the same pattern (Pulsipher et al., 2009). subject was that he/she had a diagnosis of epilepsy (CPS or
In addition to seizure type and epilepsy syndrome, a num- CAE) and at least one seizure in the year prior to participa-
ber of other seizure-related variables have been identified as tion in the study. At each site a pediatric neurologist made
contributing to variability in cognitive outcomes for chil- the diagnosis of CPS or CAE based on clinical history and
dren with epilepsy. Age at onset of epilepsy has been shown electroencephalography (EEG) findings, according to the
to predict later cognitive ability, with younger age of onset International Classification of Epilepsy (Commission,
associated with poorer cognitive outcomes (Jambaque 1989). Children with a clinical history of CPS but no EEG
et al., 1993; Schoenfeld et al., 1999; Pavone et al., 2001; evidence of epileptic activity were also included in this
Nolan et al., 2004; Hoie et al., 2006; Jocic-Jakubi & Jovic, study. All CAE patients had EEG evidence of 3 Hz spike
2006). Other disease-related factors such as longer duration and wave in addition to absence seizures induced by hyper-
of illness (Nolan et al., 2004), greater numbers of seizures ventilation. We excluded patients with a mixed seizure dis-
(Hoie et al., 2006; Jocic-Jakubi & Jovic, 2006; Fastenau order, previous epilepsy surgery, atypical spike and wave
et al., 2009), and treatment with one or more antiepileptic complexes, juvenile myoclonic epilepsy, generalized tonic–
drugs (AEDs) (Schoenfeld et al., 1999; Nolan et al., 2004; clonic seizures, a neurologic illness other than epilepsy,
Fastenau et al., 2009) have also been associated with
increased cognitive difficulties.
Table 1. Demographics
The purpose of the present study was to compare neuro-
cognitive functioning in children with localization-related Diagnosis
epilepsy with complex partial seizures (CPS), usually Variables Control CPS CAE
thought to involve cognitive impairment (particularly mem- n 51 53 31
ory), and children with childhood absence epilepsy (CAE), Male (%) 51 49 45
often regarded as a benign disorder without cognitive or Female (%) 49 51 55
other comorbidities. Children with general cognitive func- Chronological age (year) (SD) 10 (3) 10 (3) 9 (2)
Ethnicity (%)
tioning in the average range were compared on neuropsy- Caucasiana 39 62 39
chological measures of verbal memory, visual memory, and Non-Caucasian 61 38 61
executive functioning. We also investigated the effects of Socioeconomic status (%)
clinical variables on neurocognitive functioning in this pop- High 29 33 26
ulation, including age at seizure onset, duration of illness, Low 71 67 74
seizure frequency, number of AEDs, as well as lateralization CPS, complex partial seizures; CAE, childhood absence epilepsy.
a
Significantly more Caucasian subjects in CPS group than in CAE and
and localization of epileptic activity on EEG. The current Control group, p < 0.05.
study included a larger sample size than in most previous
Epilepsia, 53(12):2156–2163, 2012
doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03706.x
2158
C. L. Kernan et al.

chronic medical illness, imaging evidence for structural and diagnosis of each CAE subject from the different
brain abnormalities, a metabolic disorder, a hearing disor- recruitment sites. If he did not concur with the diagnosis
der, mental retardation based on school/classroom place- or EEG findings, the child was not included in the study.
ment, and bilingual speakers of American English who did The parents and children’s medical records provided
not attend English speaking schools or speak English at information on seizure-related variables, including seizure
home. frequency, current AEDs, age of onset, and illness duration
We recruited 42% of the CPS and 33% of the CAE (Table 2). EEG recordings done around the time of diagno-
subjects from tertiary centers (i.e., UCLA- and USC-based sis indicated left, right, bilateral, and no lateralization in
clinics) and 58% of the CPS and 67% of the CAE from 28%, 20%, 28%, and 24% of the CPS subjects, respectively.
community services (i.e., Los Angeles and Anaheim The localization of epileptic activity was temporal in 36%,
Kaiser Permanente, the Los Angeles and San Diego Chap- frontotemporal in 30%, and not localized in 34% of the CPS
ters of the Epilepsy Foundation of America, and private subjects. Thirteen of the CPS children had secondary gener-
practices). There were no differences between the CPS alization.
group and the CAE group in terms of recruitment site To include children from a wide range of ethnic and
(Table 2). UCLA institutional review board (IRB)– socioeconomic status backgrounds similar to that of the
approved recruitment flyers were available for parents of CPS and CAE group, we recruited the control subjects from
children with CPS and CAE at each recruitment site. Par- four public and two private schools in the Los Angeles com-
ents who decided to enter their children into the study munity. The study coordinator screened potential partici-
contacted the study coordinator who provided information pants for neurological, psychiatric, language, and hearing
about the study and used a UCLA IRB–approved tele- disorders through a telephone conversation with a parent.
phone script to determine if the children met the study’s We excluded children with diagnoses of these disorders in
inclusionary but none of the exclusionary criteria. The the past from the study.
study coordinator also contacted the child’s pediatric neu-
rologist to confirm the child’s diagnosis and to rule out Procedures
exclusionary criteria. One UCLA pediatric neurology This study was conducted in accordance with the policies
investigator (W.D.S.) reviewed the history, EEG records, of the University of California, Los Angeles Human Sub-
jects Protection Committees. Informed assents and consents
were obtained from all subjects and their parents, respec-
Table 2. Recruitment location and seizure tively.
characteristics
Diagnosis
Neuropsychological assessment
All subjects were administered neuropsychological tests
Variables CPS CAE
by individuals trained in standardized testing procedures.
n 53 31 For safety reasons, testers were not blind to whether the sub-
Recruitment location (%)
Tertiary 42 33
jects had epilepsy, but testers were blind to the epilepsy
Community 58 67 group (CPS or CAE) and all other seizure variables. The
Seizure frequency (log) (SD)a,b 3 (2) 6 (3) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition
Antiepileptic drugs (%) (WISC-III) was administered to children to obtain a mea-
None 2 6 sure of global intellectual functioning (Wechsler, 1991).
Monotherapy 75 68
Polytherapy 23 26
The Full Scale IQ score was generated from each test. Exec-
Age of onset (year) (SD) 6 (3) 6 (2) utive functioning was assessed with subtests from the
Duration of epilepsy (year) (SD) 4 (2) 3 (2) Stroop Color-Word Test (Stroop Color Naming, Stroop
Seizure lateralization (%) Interference) (Golden, 1975), the Wisconsin Card Sorting
Left 28 N/A Test (Total Errors, Perseverative Errors)(Heaton et al.,
Right 20 N/A
Bilateral 28 N/A
1993), and the Test of Memory and Learning (TOMAL;
No lateralization 24 N/A Digits Forward, Digits Backwards) (Reynolds & Bigler,
Seizure localization (%) 1994). Verbal memory was assessed with subtests from the
Temporal 36 N/A California Verbal Learning Test-Children’s Version
Frontotemporal 30 N/A (CVLT-C; CVLT-C Total, Long-Delay Free Recall, Reten-
Not localized 34 N/A
Secondary generalization (%) 25 0
tion, and Discriminability) (Delis et al., 1994), the TOMAL
(Memory for Stories, Memory for Stories-Delayed), and the
N/A, not applicable.
a
CAE significantly different than CPS, p < 0.0001. Doors and People Test (Auditory Name Recall, Long-
b
Log-transformed seizure frequency was used in the analyses because the Delayed Auditory Name Recall, Visual Name Recall, and
seizure frequency ranged from 1–50,000 seizures per year in the CAE Auditory Retention) (Baddeley et al., 1994). Visual mem-
subjects.
ory was assessed with subtests from the Doors People Test
Epilepsia, 53(12):2156–2163, 2012
doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03706.x
2159
Neurocognitive Profiles in Pediatric Epilepsy

(Door Recall, Shapes Recall, Long Delayed Shapes Recall,


and Visual Retention).
Results
Comparisons between the control and epilepsy groups
Data analysis Table 3 summarizes the means and standard deviations
A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was for each of the three groups on the global intellectual func-
used to compare the CPS, CAE, and control groups in the tioning, executive functioning, verbal memory, and visual
domain of verbal memory. Standardized scores were used memory variables. There were significant differences
for this analysis. Because standardized test scores were not among the groups in terms of global intellectual function-
uniformly available for all neurocognitive tests within the ing. Specifically, the ANCOVA comparing the CPS, CAE,
executive functioning and visual memory domains (i.e., and control groups, with age as a covariate revealed signifi-
Stroop, and Doors and People tests), multivariate analyses cant group differences: F2,135 = 10.29, p £ 0.0001, with the
of covariance (MANCOVAs) using raw scores, controlling control group and the CAE group scoring significantly
for age, were used to compare the CPS, CAE, and control higher than the CPS group (mean difference = )13, 95% CI
groups on neurocognitive tests in these domains. For all [)17 to )8] and mean difference = )7, 95% CI [)11 to
MANOVAs and MANCOVAs, subjects missing data were )0.2]), respectively).
excluded. Significant MANOVA and MANCOVA effects For the executive functioning tests, the MANCOVA
were subsequently analyzed using univariate analysis of comparing the CAE, CPS, and control group scores, with
variance (ANOVA) and post hoc comparisons when appro- age as a covariate, yielded a significant effect of group:
priate. To determine whether demographic variables con- F10,218 = 2.24, p < 0.05. Univariate analyses yielded signif-
founded results, we analyzed the relationship between icant group differences for Stroop Color Naming Time:
gender, SES, and ethnicity on neurocognitive tests scores F3,117 = 5.72, p < 0.01; and WCST Total Errors:
using separate ANOVAs. Gender, SES, and ethnicity did F3,117 = 3.98, p < 0.05. Post hoc tests revealed that the CPS
not significantly influence test scores for any of the three group performed more poorly than the controls on Stroop
groups, and were therefore not controlled for in the analyses Color Naming Time (mean difference = 16, 95% CI [7–
described above. 24]) and WCST Total Errors (mean difference = 13, 95%
Within the domain of executive functioning, Persevera- CI [5–19]). The CAE group performed more poorly than the
tive Errors on the WSCT were not included in the data anal- controls on WCST Total Errors only (mean difference = 8,
yses, because an error in data recording during test 95% CI [)1 to 17]).
administration resulted in loss of data for this variable. In In the domain of verbal memory, the MANOVA compar-
the domain of verbal memory, a separate MANCOVA using ing the groups on the CVLT and TOMAL measures yielded
raw scores controlling for age was conducted comparing the a significant effect for group: F10,252 = 3.86, p < 0.0001.
groups on verbal memory subtests from the Doors and Univariate analyses yielded significant group differences
People test, because significantly fewer subjects (N = 48) for CVLT Total: F2,133 = 6.63, p < 0.01; CVLT Long Delay
completed this test. This was because the Doors and People Free Recall: F2,133 = 8.7, p < 0.01; CVLT Discriminability:
test was added to our battery later in the study. Doors and F2,133 = 4, p < 0.05; TOMAL Memory for Stories:
People completers did not differ from the rest of the sample F2,133 = 8.93, p < 0.01; and TOMAL Memory for Stories
in terms of demographics. Delayed: F2,133 = 15.43, p < 0.0001. Post hoc tests revealed
In addition, to determine whether neuropsychological that the CPS group performed more poorly than the controls
testing discriminated between the epilepsy and control on CVLT Total: mean difference = )8, 95% CI ()11 to )5);
groups above and beyond what would be predicted by their CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall: mean difference = )0.9,
general intellectual functioning, the between-group analy- 95% CI ()1 to )0.5); CVLT Discriminability: mean differ-
ses described above were also conducted controlling for IQ. ence = )0.7, 95% CI ()1 to )0.3); TOMAL Memory for
For only those cognitive domains on which the groups Stories: mean difference = )2, 95% CI ()3 to )1); and
differed significantly, within-group analyses were per- TOMAL Memory for Stories Delayed: mean differ-
formed to explore the association of seizure-related vari- ence = )3, 95% CI ()4 to )2). The CAE group performed
ables (age of onset, log seizure frequency, lateralization more poorly than the controls on CVLT Total: mean differ-
and localization, and number of AEDs) and neurocogni- ence = )5, 95% CI ()9 to )1); TOMAL Memory for Stories:
tive domains. Specifically, for the executive functioning mean difference = )2, 95% CI ()3 to )1); and TOMAL
and verbal memory domains, within-group MANCOVAs Memory for Stories Delayed: mean difference = )2, 95% CI
were conducted with the neurocognitive test scores as the ()4 to )1). For the Doors and People verbal memory tests,
dependent variables and the seizure variables as predic- the MANCOVA did not yield a significant effect.
tors, controlling for age. Significant MANCOVA effects In the domain of visual memory, MANCOVA comparing
were subsequently analyzed using univariate analysis of the groups on the Doors and People measures yielded a sig-
variance (ANOVA) and post hoc comparisons when nificant effect for group: F8,82 = 1.83, p < 0.05; however,
appropriate. univariate analyses did not yield significant effects.

Epilepsia, 53(12):2156–2163, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03706.x
2160
C. L. Kernan et al.

Table 3. Neurocognitive performances in control, CPS, and CAE groupsa


CPS CAE
Control
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) 95% CI Mean (SD) 95% CI
Full Scale IQb***c* 107 (12) 94 (15) )17 to )8 101 (16) )11 to 0.2
Attention and executive functioning
Stroop Color Namingb* 53 (15) 69 (30) 7 to 24 65 (23) 4 to 20
Stroop Interference 43 (30) 37 (29) )14 to 2 50 (35) )6 to 50
WCST Total Errorsb*d* 30 (21) 43 (24) 5 to 19 38 (24) )1 to 17
Digits Forward 39 (17) 34 (21) )11 to 1 28 (12) )16 to )7
Digits Backward 23 (13) 16 (15) )10 to )2 18 (13) )9 to 0.4
Verbal memory
CVLT Total (T score)b**d* 56 (9) 48 (11) )11 to )5 51 (11) )9 to )1
CVLT Long-Delay Free (z-score)b* 1 (1) )0.4 (1) )1 to 0 0.03 (1) )1 to )0.1
Discriminability (z-score)b* 1 (1) )0.1 (1) )1 to 0 0.2 (1) )1 to )0.01
TOMAL Memory for Stories 12 (3) 10 (3) )3 to )1 10 (3) )3 to )1
(standard score)b**d**
TOMAL Memory for Stories- Delayed 11 (3) 8 (3) )4 to )2 9 (3) )4 to )1
(standard score)b**d**
Verbal Memory (Doors and People)
Auditory Name Recall 23 (7) 19 (8) )7 to 0.1 20 (7) )7 to 1
Long-Delay Auditory Name Recall 9 (3) 6 (4) )4 to )1 8 (3) )2 to 1
Visual Name Recall 16 (6) 13 (7) )7 to 0.2 17 (5) )2 to 3
E–F Difference (Auditory Retention) 1 (2) 3 (2) 0.1 to 2 1 (2) )1 to 1
Visual memory
Door Recall 17 (4) 15 (5) )5 to )0.3 18 (3) )1 to 2
Shapes Recall 32 (5) 26 (9) )10 to )1 31 (4) )4 to 2
Long Delayed Shapes 11 (2) 10 (2) )2 to 0.1 11 (1) )0.4 to 1
G–H Difference (Visual Retention) 0.2 (2) 1 (2) )1 to 2 0.4 (1) )1 to 1
*p < 0.05.
**p < 0.01.
***p < 0.0001.
a
Raw scores are reported unless otherwise indicated. Lower scores indicate better performance for Stroop Color Naming, Stroop Interference, and WCST
Total Errors. For all other tests, higher scores indicate better performance.
b
Significant difference between CPS and controls.
c
Significant difference between CPS and CAE.
d
Significant difference between CAE and controls.

Comparisons between the control and epilepsy groups, Memory for Stories (adjusted means = 10.12 and 11.80)
controlling for IQ and TOMAL Memory for Stories Delayed (adjusted
For the executive functioning tests, the MANCOVA means = 9.07 and 10.95).
comparing the CAE, CPS, and control group scores, with
age and IQ as a covariates, did not yield a significant effect: Relationship between neurocognitive performance and
F10,236 = 1.01, p = 0.44. For the Doors and People tests, seizure-related variables
neither visual memory nor verbal memory MANCOVA In the executive functioning domain, for the CPS group,
yielded a significant effect: F8,88 = 1.83, p = 0.08, and the within-group MANCOVA exploring the association
F8,104 = 1.07, p = 0.39, respectively. between the seizure-related variable and the tests of neuro-
In the domain of verbal memory, the MANCOVA com- cognitive functioning did not yield a significant relationship
paring the groups on the CVLT and TOMAL measures, con- between seizure-related variables and tests of neurocogni-
trolling for IQ, yielded a significant effect of group tive functioning. For the CAE group, there was a significant
(F10,272 = 2.34, p = 0.05). Univariate analyses yielded sig- main effect for age of seizure onset (F5,20 = 7.19,
nificant group differences for CVLT Long Delay Free p < 0.001). Post hoc t-tests revealed that younger age of
Recall: F3,144 = 4.39, p < 0.05; TOMAL Memory for onset was associated with poorer performance on Digits
Stories: F3,144 = 5.40, p < 0.01; and TOMAL Memory for Forward (t28 = )3.34, p < 0.05).
Stories Delayed: F3,144 = 7.10, p < 0.01. Post hoc tests In the domain of verbal memory, for the CPS group, the
revealed that the CPS group performed more poorly than the within-group MANCOVA exploring the association
controls on CVLT Long Delay Free Recall (adjusted between the seizure-related variables and the tests of neuro-
means = )0.26 and 0.39) and TOMAL Memory for Stories cognitive functioning in the verbal domain where there were
Delayed (adjusted means = 9.37 and 10.95). The CAE group differences (CVLT and TOMAL) yielded a signifi-
group performed more poorly than the controls on TOMAL cant main effect for seizure frequency (F5,40 = 2.84,

Epilepsia, 53(12):2156–2163, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03706.x
2161
Neurocognitive Profiles in Pediatric Epilepsy

p < 0.03). Post hoc t-tests were performed and revealed that memory problems, with intact verbal memory skills (Pav-
greater seizure frequency was associated with poorer perfor- one et al., 2001; Nolan et al., 2004). These inconsistencies
mance on CVLT Total (t48 = )2.06, p < 0.05) and TOMAL in findings may be partially due to small sample sizes and
Memory for Stories Delayed Recall (t48 = )2.30, p < 0.05). subsequent lack of power to detect differences. Varying
For the CAE group, the MANCOVA did not yield a signifi- degrees of seizure control in the study populations and dif-
cant relationship between the seizure related variables and ferences across studies in instruments used to assess verbal
neurocognitive performance on the CVLT and TOMAL. and visual memory may also partially account for these dis-
AEDs, lateralization, and localization were not related to crepant findings. More research is needed to better charac-
neurocognitive functioning. terize the factors that account for within-group variability in
cognitive functioning among children with CAE. Nonethe-
less, these findings, along with studies showing language
Discussion impairments (Caplan et al., 2009) in children with CAE
The present study is one of few that compare neurocogni- suggest that CAE is a syndrome that affects multiple cogni-
tive functioning in relatively large samples of children with tive functions.
different types of epilepsy with comparable demographic The literature is somewhat inconclusive regarding the
variables. When compared to each other, the CPS and CAE relationship between seizure variables and cognitive func-
group differed significantly in generalized cognitive func- tioning in children with epilepsy, with some studies finding
tioning, but not in the specific cognitive domains of execu- relationships and others not (Schoenfeld et al., 1999;
tive functioning, verbal memory, and visual memory. Williams et al., 2001; Nolan et al., 2004; Hermann et al.,
Compared to controls, children with CPS showed a mild 2006; Jocic-Jakubi & Jovic, 2006; Fastenau et al., 2009;
generalized cognitive deficit reflected by a mean Full Scale Austin & Fastenau, 2010; see Jambaque et al., 1993). In our
IQ of 94 (SD = 15), whereas children with CAE did not sample, seizure frequency was associated with cognitive
(mean Full Scale IQ = 102, SD = 16). Relative to controls, test performance in the CPS group, but not in the CAE
both groups showed poorer performances in the domains of group. Younger age of onset was associated with poorer per-
executive functioning and verbal memory, with the CPS formance on cognitive tests for the CAE group, but not the
group showing poorer across a greater range of tests. This is CPS group. It may be that the relationship between age of
consistent with a study of children with CPS, which found a seizure onset and frequency and neurocognitive perfor-
pattern of relatively diffuse and generalized cognitive dys- mance differs depending on type of epilepsy and involved
function in children with CPS (Schoenfeld et al., 1999). brain regions. However, the literature does not adequately
To determine whether neuropsychological testing could address this question, and further exploration of the mecha-
help identify specific neurocognitive weaknesses in chil- nisms underlying the association between these factors and
dren with CPS and CAE above and beyond what would be neurocognitive functioning or lack thereof is needed. In par-
predicted by their general intellectual functioning, we also ticular, large prospective studies initiated close after the
compared our study groups controlling for IQ. After control- onset of epilepsy will be useful in characterizing the course
ling for IQ, no differences were found between the epilepsy of neuropsychological functioning over time and in deter-
groups and the controls in the domain of executive function- mining the relative contributions of seizures variables on
ing. However, both the CPS and CAE groups demonstrated neuropsychological impairment (Hermann et al., 2006;
mild verbal memory deficits that were not associated with Fastenau et al., 2009; Austin & Fastenau, 2010).
deficits in generalized cognitive functioning. Specifically, Along with the cross-sectional design of this study and
controlling for IQ, the CPS group performed poorer than the the lack of power to detect differences in the visual memory
controls on delayed verbal memory tasks (memory for word domain, other limitations should be noted. The study was
lists and memory for stories), and the CAE performing not population-based, which limits the generalizability of
poorer than the control group on story memory tasks (both the findings. Although the control group was well-matched
immediate and delayed). The present study is among the to the epilepsy group on demographic variables, the control
few to document cognitive impairments in children with group performed better than average on many of the neuro-
CAE. CAE has historically been considered a ‘‘benign’’ dis- psychological tests. In addition, information on lateraliza-
order, with little effect on neurocognitive functioning. How- tion and localization of the EEG findings of the CPS group
ever, increasing evidence suggests that this may not be the was limited because it was based on reports of EEG record-
case. In a prospective study with 39 children with general- ings done at the time of the diagnosis of epilepsy.
ized absence seizures, children with absence seizures per- Despite its limitations, the present study demonstrates
formed more poorly on tests of attention/executive that although children with CPS and CAE may look similar
functioning/construction than controls (Fastenau et al., in terms of their academic and behavioral problems (Caplan
2009). Although we found intact nonverbal memory skills et al., 2004, 2008; Fastenau et al., 2008; Caplan et al.,
with mild verbal memory deficits in children with CAE, two 2009), the cognitive underpinnings of their functional defi-
other studies found the reverse pattern of mild nonverbal cits may be distinct, with the CPS group showing more
Epilepsia, 53(12):2156–2163, 2012
doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03706.x
2162
C. L. Kernan et al.

diffuse cognitive impairment. In addition, the present study Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. (1994) California verbal learn-
ing test- children’s version. The Psychological Corporation, San Anto-
demonstrated verbal memory deficits in children with CAE nio, TX.
and CPS in the context of intact global intellectual function- Fastenau P, Shen J, Dunn DW, Austin JK. (2008) Academic underachieve-
ing. Although the deficits were mild in magnitude for many ment among children with epilepsy: proportion exceeding psychomet-
ric criteria for learning disability and associated risk factors. J Learn
of the children studied, it is important to note that, overall, a Disabil 41:195–197.
disproportionate number of children with CPS and CAE fell Fastenau PS, Johnson CS, Perkins SM, Byars AW. (2009) Neuropsycholog-
more than one standard deviation below controls, even ical status at seizure onset in children: risk factors for early cognitive
deficits. Neurology 73:526.
when controlling for IQ. There is clearly a subset of children Golden CJ. (1975) A group version of the stroop color and word test. J Pers
who are at risk for academic and functional difficulties due Assess 39:386.
to memory problems. More research is needed to determine Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G. (1993) Wisconsin
card sorting test manual: revised and expanded. Psychological Assess-
whether these memory difficulties may be present in the ment Resources Inc, Odessa, FL.
nonverbal domain, as the present study was underpowered Henkin Y, Sadeh M, Kivity S, Shabtai E, Kishon-Rabin L, Gadoth N.
to detect visual memory differences. Because neuropsycho- (2005) Cognitive function in idiopathic generalized epilepsy of child-
hood. Dev Med Child Neurol 47:126–132.
logical testing is costly in terms of both time and financial Hermann B, Jones J, Sheth R, Dow C, Koehn M, Seidenberg M. (2006)
resources, findings may be useful for designing more Children with new-onset epilepsy: neuropsychological status and brain
targeted memory screenings of children with epilepsy. structure. Brain 129:2609–2619.
Hermann BP, Jones JE, Sheth R, Koehn M, Becker T, Fine J, Allen CA,
Seidenberg M. (2008) Growing up with epilepsy: a two year investiga-
Acknowledgments tion of cognitive development in children with new onset epilepsy.
Epilepsia 49:1847–1858.
Hoie B, Mykletun A, Sommerfelt K, Bjornffis H, Skeidsvoll H, Waaler PE.
This study was supported by grants NS32070 (RC) and MH 67187 (RC).
(2005) Seizure-related factors and non-verbal intelligence in children
with epilepsy: a population-based study from Western Norway. Seizure
14:223–231.
Disclosure Hoie B, Mykletun A, Waaler PE, Skeidsvoll H, Sommerfelt K. (2006)
Executive functions and seizure-related factors in children with epi-
None of the authors has any conflicts of interest to disclose. We confirm lepsy in western Norway. Dev Med Child Neurol 48:519–525.
that we have read the Journal’s position on issues involved in ethical publi- Hollingshead A. (1973) Medical sociology: a brief review. Milbank Mem
cation and affirm that this report is consistent with those guidelines. Fund Q Health Soc 51:531–542.
Hommet C, Sauerwein HC, De Toffol B, Lassonde M. (2006) Idiopathic
epileptic syndromes and cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:85–96.
References Jambaque I, Dellatolas G, Dulac O, Ponsot G, Signoret J-L. (1993) Verbal
and visual memory impairment in childrem with epilepsy. Neuropsych-
Aldenkamp AP, Weber B, Overweg-Plandsoen WCG, Reijs R, van Mil S. ologia 31:1321–1337.
(2005) Educational underachievement in children with epilepsy: a Jocic-Jakubi B, Jovic NJ. (2006) Verbal memory impairment in children
model to predict the effects of epilepsy on educational achievement. with focal epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 9:432–439.
J Child Neurol 20:175–180. Lassonde M, Sauerwein HC, Jambaqu I, Smith ML, Helmstaedter C.
Austin JK, Fastenau PS. (2010) Are seizure variables related to cognitive (2000) Neuropsychology of childhood epilepsy: pre- and postsurgical
and behavior problems? Dev Med Child Neurol 52:5–6. assessment. Epileptic Disord 2:3–13.
Baddeley A, Emslie H, Nimmo-Smith I. (1994) Doors and people: a test of MacAllister WS, Schaffer SG. (2007) Neuropsychological deficits in child-
visual and verbal recall and recognition. Thames Valley Test Com- hood epilepsy syndromes. Neuropsychol Rev 17:427–444.
pany, Bury St. Edmunds, UK. Nolan MA, Redoblado MA, Lah S, Sabaz M, Lawson JA, Cunningham
Bailet LL, Turk WR. (2000) The impact of childhood epilepsy on neuro- AM, Bleasel AF, Bye AME. (2004) Memory function in childhood epi-
cognitive and behavioral performance: a prospective longitudinal study. lepsy syndromes. J Paediatr Child Health 40:20–27.
Epilepsia 41:426–431. Parrish J, Geary E, Jones J, Seth R, Hermann B, Seidenberg M. (2007)
Berg AT, Langfitt JT, Testa FM, Levy SR, DiMario F, Westerveld M, Executive functioning in childhood epilepsy: parent-report and cogni-
Kulas J. (2008) Global cognitive function in children with epilepsy: a tive assessment. Dev Med Child Neurol 49:412–416.
community-based study. Epilepsia 49:608–614. Pavone P, Bianchini R, Trifiletti RR, Incorpora G, Pavone A, Parano E.
Borden KA, Burns TG, O’Leary SD. (2006) A comparison of children with (2001) Neuropsychological assessment in children with absence epi-
epilepsy to an age-and IQ-matched control group on the Children’s lepsy. Neurology 56:1047–1051.
Memory Scale. Child Neuropsychol 12:165–172. Pulsipher DT, Seidenberg M, Guidotti L, Tuchscherer VN, Morton J, Sheth
Caplan R, Siddarth P, Gurbani S, Ott D, Sankar R, Shields WD. (2004) Psy- RD, Hermann B. (2009) Thalamofrontal circuitry and executive dys-
chopathology and pediatric complex partial seizures: seizure-related, function in recent-onset juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. Epilepsia
cognitive, and linguistic variables. Epilepsia 45:1273–1281. 50:1210–1219.
Caplan R, Siddarth P, Stahl L, Lanphier E, Vona P, Gurbani S, Koh S, San- Reynolds CR, Bigler ED. (1994) Test of memory and learning. PRO-ED,
kar R, Shields WD. (2008) Childhood absence epilepsy: behavioral, Austin, TX.
cognitive, and linguistic comorbidities. Epilepsia 49:1838–1846. Schoenfeld J, Seidenberg M, Woodard A, Hecox K, Inglese C, Mack K,
Caplan R, Siddarth P, Vona P, Stahl L, Bailey C, Gurbani S, Sankar R, Hermann B. (1999) Neuropsychological and behavioral status of chil-
Shields WD. (2009) Language in pediatric epilepsy. Epilepsia dren with complex partial seizures. Dev Med Child Neurol 41:724–731.
50:2397–2407. Seidenberg M, Pulsipher D, Hermann B. (2007) Cognitive progression in
Commission on Classification and Terminology of the International League epilepsy. Neuropsychol Rev 17:445–454.
Against Epilepsy. (1989) Proposal for revised clinical and electro- Sillanpaa M. (2004) Learning disability: occurrence and long-term conse-
encephalographic classification of epileptic seizures. Epilepsia 30: quences in childhood-onset epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 5:937–944.
389–399. Sillanpaa M, Jalava M, Kaleva O, Shinnar S. (1998) Long-term prognosis
Culhane-Shelburne K, Chapieski L, Hiscock M, Glaze D. (2002) Executive of seizures with onset in childhood. N Engl J Med 338:1715–1722.
functions in children with frontal and temporal lobe epilepsy. J Int Neu- Wechsler D. (1991) Wechsler intelligence scale for children, 3rd edn. The
ropsychol Soc 8:623–632. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX.

Epilepsia, 53(12):2156–2163, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03706.x
2163
Neurocognitive Profiles in Pediatric Epilepsy

Williams J, Griebel ML, Dykman RA. (1998) Neuropsychological patterns Wirrell EC, Camfield CS, Camfield PR, Dooley JM, Gordon KE, Smith B.
in pediatric epilepsy. Seizure 7:223–228. (1997) Long-term psychosocial outcome in typical absence epilepsy:
Williams J, Phillips T, Griebel ML, Sharp GB, Lange B, Edgar T, Simpson sometimes a wolf in sheeps’ clothing. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med
P. (2001) Patterns of memory performance in children with controlled 151:152–158.
epilepsy on the CVLT-C. Child Neuropsychol 7:15–20.

Epilepsia, 53(12):2156–2163, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03706.x

You might also like