You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/21654325

Proneness to Shame, Proneness to Guilt, and Psychopathology

Article  in  Journal of Abnormal Psychology · September 1992


DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.101.3.469 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

430 2,952

3 authors, including:

June P Tangney
George Mason University
114 PUBLICATIONS   10,902 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by June P Tangney on 23 May 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1992, Vol. 101, No. 3, 469-478 0021-843X/92/S3.00

Proneness to Shame, Proneness to Guilt, and Psychopathology


June Price Tangney, Patricia Wagner, and Richard Gramzow
George Mason University

The links between shame and guilt and psychopathology were examined. In 2 studies, 245 and 234
undergraduates completed the Self-Conscious Affect and Attribution Inventory, the Symptom
Checklist 90, the Beck Depression Inventory, the State-Trait Anxiety Scale, and the Attributional
Style Questionnaire. Results failed to support Lewis's (1971) notion that shame and guilt are differ-
entially related to unique symptom clusters. Shame-proneness was strongly related to psychological
maladjustment in general. Guilt-proneness was only moderately related to psychopathology; corre-
lations were ascribable entirely to the shared variance between shame and guilt. Although clearly
related to a depressogenic attributional style, shame accounted for substantial variance in depres-
sion, above and beyond attributional style.

Psychologists have long speculated about the links between is some specific action (or failure to act). There is remorse or
shame and guilt and various forms of psychopathology. From regret over the "bad thing that was done" and a sense of tension
the early writings of Freud (1896/1953), psychoanalytic theo- that often serves as a motivation for reparative action. The ten-
rists have emphasized the role of superego functions in the sion, remorse, and regret engendered by guilt can be quite un-
formation of symptoms. Shame and guilt have also figured comfortable, particularly when reparation is blocked for one
prominently in more recent conceptualizations of a number of reason or another. Nonetheless, the experience of shame is far
disorders (Kohut, 1971; Lewis, 1971,1987b; Nathanson, 1987). more painful and devastating.
The clinical and empirical literature, however, is inconsistent In shame, the object of concern is the entire self. The "bad
with regard to the specific links between psychological symp- thing" is experienced as a reflection of a "bad self," and the
toms and these moral affective processes. Part of the confusion entire self is painfully scrutinized and negatively evaluated.
is attributable to the fact that many psychologists either have With this painful scrutiny of the self is a corresponding sense of
failed to make a clear distinction between the experiences of shrinking, of being small, and of being worthless and power-
shame and guilt or have adopted theoretical perspectives that less. Shame also involves a sense of exposure. In introducing the
focus on one emotion to the exclusion of the other. notion of an "internalized other," Lewis (1971) extended the
A growing theoretical and empirical literature underlines definition of shame beyond an affective reaction to public dis-
important differences in the phenomenology of shame and approval. But there is typically the imagery of an explicit or
guilt (DeRivera, 1977; Erikson, 1950; Lewis, 1971; Lindsay- implicit disapproving other. Even when alone, the disapproving
Hartz, 1984; Tangney, 1989b; Weiner, 1985; Wicker, Payne, & self imagines how the self (as disapproved object) might look to
Morgan, 1983). Both shame and guilt involve negative affect, another person. Thus it is not surprising that the ashamed per-
but the focus of the negative affect differs, leading to distinct
son often wants to hide from others or to sink into the floor and
phenomenological experiences. In guilt, the object of concern disappear.
The most comprehensive discussion of shame and guilt in
psychopathology can be found in analytically oriented the-
This research was supported by Grant IR15HD25506 from the Na-
tional Institute for Child Health and Human Development and by a ories. These theories, however, vary considerably in their em-
Faculty Research Grant from George Mason University. phasis on shame or guilt. Freud (1909/1955,1917/1957,1924/
The authors thank Carey Fletcher, Elly Bordeaux, Joe Constantin, 1961), for example, devoted far more attention to the patho-
Laura Flicker, William Harman, Donna Marschall, James Maxfield, genic implications of guilt than to those of shame, particularly
Julie Morig, Yvette Nageotte, Gary Russell, Provie Rydstrom, Gordon in connection with obsessional neuroses, masochism, and de-
Shaw, and Chris Smart for their assistance with the larger studies from pression. He hypothesized that guilt, arising from unacceptable
which this report was drawn. Susanne Denham, James Maddux, John impulses, unleashes a variety of self-punitive processes that re-
Riskind, and two anonymous reviewers provided invaluable com-
sult in manifest psychopathology. Shame played a much less
ments and suggestions on an earlier draft.
Portions of this article were presented at the annual meeting of the central role in early psychoanalytic thinking. Freud (1905/
American Psychological Society, Dallas, June 1990. 1953) largely relegated shame to the status of a reaction forma-
Correspondence concerning this article (including requests for cop- tion against sexually exhibitionistic impulses. Some theorists
ies of the Self-Conscious Affect and Attribution Inventory or the Test have suggested that Freud's relative neglect of shame may have
of Self-Conscious Affect) should be addressed to June Price Tangney, been attributable in part to his staunch adherence to a conflict-
Department of Psychology, George Mason University, Fairfax, Vir- defense model of psychological functioning and his attendant
ginia 22030. failure to distinguish between the ego and the self (Lewis,
469
470 J. TANGNEY, P. WAGNER, AND R. GRAMZOW

1987a; Miller, 1985; A. P. Morrison, 1989). In any event, in regard to the cognitive components of these affective states, the
developing a theory that focused almost exclusively on guilt, distinction between guilt and shame bears some resemblance
Freud may have misinterpreted his patients1 experiences with to Janoff-Bulman's (1979) distinction between behavioral and
shame. Lewis (1971) argued that much of Freud's case material characterological self-blame. To the extent that characterologi-
and associated formulations can be reconceptualized in terms cal self-blame (the tendency to make internal, global, and stable
of shame. attributions for negative events) has been theoretically and em-
With the emergence of self psychology, shame gained a more pirically linked to depression (for a review, see Robins, 1988),
prominent place in dynamic conceptualizations of psychopa- the attributional literature is consistent with Lewis's notion that
thology. Psychoanalytically oriented clinicians and theorists there may be a special link between proneness to shame and
have identified shame as a key component of such disorders as depression.
depression, bipolar illness, schizophrenia, and narcissism (Ko- In spite of the rather rich theoretical literature pertaining to
hut, 1971; A. P. Morrison, 1989; N. K. Morrison, 1987; Nathan- shame and guilt and psychopathology, there is little direct em-
son, 1987). Although these theories vary in the particular func- pirical evidence linking the two affective processes to various
tional role assigned to shame (in some, shame is viewed as a aspects of psychological maladjustment. The paucity of empiri-
cause of the disorder; in others, shame is viewed as a concomi- cal research is attributable in part to difficulties in the measure-
tant of defects in the self system), these more recent formula- ment of shame and guilt. The earliest guilt scales (e.g., Buss &
tions share a common focus on shame and a corresponding Durkee, 1957; Mosher, 1966) were developed without regard to
de-emphasis of the pathogenic aspects of guilt. the distinction between shame and guilt. As a consequence,
Helen Block Lewis is one of the few psychologists who has these measures tend to assess both shame and guilt experiences
presented an integrated theoretical account of the differential and thus are of little use in distinguishing between the psycho-
roles of shame and guilt in psychopathology. In her seminal pathological correlates of guilt and those of shame.
work Shame and Guilt in Neurosis, Lewis (1971) noted that More recently, researchers have attempted to develop mea-
there are individual differences in the degree to which people sures for assessing and differentiating these affective experi-
experience shame or guilt across a range of situations. More- ences. Smith (1972) reported that severely depressed patients'
over, Lewis (1971, 1987b) hypothesized that these individual descriptions of their earliest memories contained more shame
differences in affective style (shame-proneness vs. guilt-prone- themes than guilt themes. This finding, however, was not repli-
ness) are differentially related to the formation of psychological cated in a subsequent similar study by Crouppen (1977), and
symptoms. Specifically, Lewis postulated that shame-prone Hoblitzelle (1987) raised concerns regarding the validity of the
persons are vulnerable primarily to affective disorders (particu- Early Memories Test as an index of shame-proneness and guilt-
larly depression), whereas guilt-prone persons are more vulnera- proneness. Using the Personal Feelings Questionnaire (a brief
ble to thought-related disorders (e.g., paranoia, obsessive-com- self-report measure), Harder and Lewis (1987) found that both
pulsive syndromes).' shame- and guilt-proneness were associated with depression,
Lewis's formulation represents an interesting integration of anxiety, hostility, and low self-esteem. Using a more extended
psychodynamic and cognitive principles. She believes that indi- adjective checklist, Hoblitzelle (1987) found evidence of a link
vidual differences in cognitive style (i.e., field dependence vs. between depression and shame-proneness in two independent
field independence) lead to contrasting modes of superego studies. The relation between depression and guilt-proneness,
functioning (i.e., shame-proneness and guilt-proneness), and to- however, was less consistent across the two studies, perhaps
gether these cognitive and affective styles set the stage for differ- owing to problems with Hoblitzelle's guilt scale. There is some
ential symptom formation. The global, less differentiated self question, for example, about the use of the adjective checklist
of the field-dependent person is particularly vulnerable to the approach for assessing guilt-proneness (Tangney, 1990). Al-
global, less differentiated experience of shame and, ultimately, though adjectives may vary subtly in terms of shame and guilt,
to the global experience of depression. In contrast, the more adjective checklists essentially pose respondents with a shame-
clearly differentiated self of the field-independent person is par- related task. Respondents are asked to make global ratings
ticularly vulnerable to the experience of guilt (which involves a about the self (or, in the case of the Personal Feelings Question-
differentiation between self and behavior) and also to obsessive naire, global ratings of the self's general affective experience)
and paranoid symptoms involving vigilance of the "field" sepa- rather than ratings of affective responses to circumscribed be-
rate from the self.
There are some parallels between Lewis's (1971) conceptual-
ization and more recent attributional models of depression (e.g., ' It is interesting to note that Lewis's (1971) predictions are nearly in
Abramson, Seligman & Teasdale, 1978; Beck, 1983). Although direct opposition to those of Erikson (1950). In a brief discussion of
the attributional literature contains few explicit references to shame and guilt and psychopathology, Erikson suggested that shame-
the constructs of shame and guilt, these experiences involve related problems emerging from Stage II (autonomy vs. shame and
doubt) are likely to be associated with paranoid ideation, compulsive
both affective and cognitive components, the latter of which
behaviors, a defiant shamelessness, rage toward the self, or a combina-
can be conceptualized in attributional terms (Hoblitzelle, 1987; tion of these characteristics. In contrast, he suggested that guilt-related
Lewis, 1987b). Shame, which focuses on the entire self, can be problems emerging from Stage III (initiative vs. guilt) are likely to be
viewed as an affective state stemming from internal, global, associated with anxiety, denial, psychosomatic symptoms, jealousy,
and stable attributions; guilt, which focuses on specific behav- grandiosity, and self-righteous anger toward others. Unfortunately, Er-
iors, can be viewed as an affective state arising from internal, ikson did not fully develop his ideas on this topic in the 1950 volume,
specific, and presumably less stable attributions. Thus, with nor did he elaborate in his later works.
SHAME, GUILT, AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 471

haviors embedded in local contexts. Thus the structure of such from subsequent sessions. The following were among the measures
global adjective checklists seem more appropriate for the as- completed by participants.
sessment of shame. 1. The SCAAI (Tangney, Burggraf, Hamme, & Domingos, 1988) is
In our two studies, we used the Self-Conscious Affect and designed to assess characteristic affective, cognitive, and behavioral
Attribution Inventory (SCAAI), a scenario-based measure of responses associated with shame, guilt, externalization of blame, de-
tachment/unconcern, and pride, as described in the theoretical and
affective style, to examine the degrees to which shame-prone- phenomenological literature (e.g., Lewis, 1971; Lindsay-Hartz, 1984;
ness and guilt-proneness differentially relate to a range of psy- Weiner, 1985; Wicker et al., 1983). The SCAAI is a paper-and-pencil
chological symptoms. In Study 2, we also used the Test of Self- measure composed of 13 brief scenarios that college students are likely
Conscious Affect (TOSCA), a revised version of the SCAAI to encounter in daily life. The 10 negative scenarios are followed, in
constructed from subject-generated scenarios and responses. random order, by responses indicating shame, guilt, externalization,
Because shame-proneness and guilt-proneness theoretically and detachment/unconcern. The three positive scenarios are followed
have cognitive features in common with explanatory style, we by responses indicating shame, guilt, externalization, alpha pride
examined the relation of shame-proneness and guilt-proneness (pride in self), and beta pride (pride in behavior). The measure is not
to attributional style. We also evaluated the degree to which forced choice in nature; subjects rate, on a 5-point scale, the likelihood
these affective style variables contribute to the understanding of responding in each of the manners indicated. Relevant items are
of depression above and beyond the variance accounted for by summed across scenarios to create indices of shame-proneness, guilt-
proneness, externalization, detachment/unconcern, alpha pride, and
purely attributional factors.
beta pride. Thus a common set of scenarios is used to elicit shame and
guilt responses.
Method In previous reports, we have presented data supporting the reliabil-
ity and construct validity of the central scales of the SCAAI (Tangney,
Participants 1990, 1991; Tangney et al., 1988). Shame, guilt, externalization, and
detachment/unconcern subscales demonstrated satisfactory reliabil-
In Study 1, 245 undergraduates attending a large east coast state ity, as indicated by analyses of their internal consistency and test-retest
university received credit toward a course requirement in return for stability. With regard to validity, the interrelation among SCAAI sub-
their participation. The ages of students ranged from 18 to 55 (M = scales and their relation to other measures of shame and guilt indicate
21.1). Seventy-one percent were female. Seventy-seven percent were that the SCAAI yields related but functionally distinct indices of
White, 5% were Black, 13% were Asian, and 6% belonged to other shame and guilt in a way that other measures have not (Tangney, 1990).
races. Thirty-nine percent reported their primary religious affiliation Moreover, across several independent studies, shame has been consis-
in childhood as Catholic, 32% as Protestant, 4% as Jewish, and 13% as tently linked to low self-esteem; a tendency to externalize blame; a
other; 12% had no affiliation. seething, bitter, resentful kind of anger; an impaired capacity for em-
In Study 2, 234 undergraduates attending a large east coast state pathy; and dysfunctional family relationships (Burggraf & Tangney,
university received credit toward a course requirement in return for 1989; Hamme, 1991; Tangney, 1991; Tangney, Wagner, Fletcher, &
their participation. The ages of the students ranged from 17 to 35 (Af= Gramzow, 1992). Guilt, on the other hand, has been consistently posi-
19.5). Seventy-two percent were female. Eighty-three percent were tively related to interpersonal empathy and negatively related to exter-
White, 6% were Black, 7% were Asian, and 4% belonged to other races. nalization of blame, a detached/unconcerned attitude toward negative
Thirty-seven percent reported their primary religious affiliation in interpersonal events, resentment toward others, and a hostile sense of
childhood as Catholic, 23% as Protestant, 4% as Jewish, and 21% as humor, particularly with regard to the unique variance in guilt
other; 16% had no affiliation. (Gessner & Tangney, 1990; Tangney, 1990,1991; Tangney et al., 1992).
In Study 1, the mean SCAAI shame score was 35.5 (SD = 7.98), and
the mean SCAAI guilt score was 47.2 (SD = 6.20). Estimates of inter-
Measures and Procedures nal consistency (Cronbach's alpha) for the Study 1 SCAAI shame and
guilt subscales were .75 and .65, respectively. In Study 2, the mean
The data reported here were collected as part of two larger investiga- SCAAI shame score was 36.4 (SD = 7.78), and the mean SCAAI guilt
tions of the personality correlates of proneness to shame and guilt. score was 47.0 (SD = 6.19). Estimates of internal consistency for the
Students in Study 1 participated in two sessions, and students in Study Study 2 SCAAI shame and guilt subscales were .78 and .70, respec-
2 participated in four sessions. Sessions lasted from 30 to 60 min and tively.
were conducted on separate days. At the beginning of the studies, in- 2. The TOSCA (Tangney, Wagner, & Gramzow, 1989) was modeled
formed consent forms describing the general nature of the procedures after the SCAAI. The TOSCA also consists of a series of brief scenarios
were distributed. Participants were informed that the investigation (10 negative and 5 positive) and associated responses, yielding indices
was "a psychological study to learn more about emotions, perceptions, of shame, guilt, externalization, detachment/unconcern, alpha pride,
and styles of thinking among normal, young adults." The voluntary, and beta pride. This entirely new set of scenarios was drawn from
confidential, and anonymous nature of the study was emphasized, and written accounts of personal shame, guilt, and pride experiences pro-
students were asked not to write their names on any of the question- vided by a sample of several hundred college students and adults not in
naires. college. The new responses were drawn from a much larger pool of
Questionnaires were coded with unique identification numbers in affective, cognitive, and behavioral responses provided by a second
advance. At the end of each session, participants were asked to write on sample of noncollege adults. The TOSCA has several advantages over
a Post-It Note a unique and easily remembered pseudonym; these notes the original SCAAI. First, the items were subject generated rather than
were affixed to questionnaires for the subsequent session. Upon re- experimenter generated, thereby enhancing the ecological validity of
turning, participants were asked to recall their pseudonyms, and the the measure. Second, the items are appropriate for adults of all ages,
note was removed, leaving questionnaires indexed only by identifica- not just specifically college students. Third, in terms of reliability and
tion numbers. It was thus possible to link questionnaires across ses- validity, our preliminary analyses indicate that the TOSCA is equiva-
sions while preserving participants' anonymity. Only one participant, lent to, and in some respects more psychometrically sound than, the
from Study 2, was unable to recall his pseudonym and was excused SCAAI.
472 J. TANGNEY, P. WAGNER, AND R. GRAMZOW

The TOSCA was administered to participants in Study 2 only. The shame-proneness, as measured by both the SCAAI and the
mean TOSCA shame score was 44.3 (SD = 8.43), and the mean TOSCA.
TOSCA guilt score was 58.3 (SD= 5.91). Estimates of internal consis- Guilt-proneness, on the other hand, was only moderately re-
tency (Cronbach's alpha) for the TOSCA shame and guilt subscales lated to psychopathology, and part-correlational analyses re-
were .76 and .66, respectively.2
vealed that these moderate correlations resulted entirely from
3. The Symptom Checklist 90 (SCL-90; Derogatis, Lipman, & Covi,
1973) is a list of 90 symptoms, each rated on a 5-point scale to indicate the shared variance between shame and guilt. Across numerous
absence or intensity. The SCL-90 is a widely used clinical rating scale studies, we have found a substantial positive correlation be-
that is appropriate for psychiatric outpatients as well as for screening tween shame-proneness and guilt-proneness, as assessed by the
nonclinical populations. The checklist yields nine clinical subscales: SCAAI (Tangney, 1990). This covariation between measures of
Somatization, Obsessive-Compulsive, Interpersonal Sensitivity (as- shame and guilt no doubt reflects the fact that shame and guilt
sessing feelings of personal inadequacy or inferiority), Depression, have a number of features in common (e.g., both are dysphoric
Anxiety, Hostility, Phobic Anxiety, Paranoid Ideation, and Psychotic- affective processes that involve internal attributions) and that
ism. An extensive body of research supports the reliability and validity these affective processes can be experienced in tandem (Lewis,
of these scales (e.g., Derogatis, 1989; Derogatis & Cleary, 1977; Deroga- 1971). In our Studies 1 and 2, SCAAI shame and guilt subscales
tis, Rickels, & Rock, 1976). In our studies, internal consistency (Cron-
were correlated at .46 and .63, respectively,3 and TOSCA shame
bach's alpha) of the SCL-90 scales ranged from .66 to .89 (M= .82).
and guilt subscales were correlated at .45. To further refine our
4. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, 1972) is a widely
used, well-validated measure of depression appropriate for both clini- analyses, we conducted part correlations, factoring out shame
cal and nonclinical populations. The measure is a list of 21 symptoms, from guilt and vice versa. The part correlations showed an even
each of which are rated on a 4-point scale to indicate intensity. Esti- clearer pattern of results. All indices of psychopathology, with
mates of internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha) were .86 and .88 in the exception of Somatization in Study 1, were positively and
Studies 1 and 2, respectively. significantly related to shame residuals. In contrast, the mea-
5. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, sures of psychological maladjustment were negligibly, and in
& Lushene, 1970) is a widely used, well-validated measure of anxiety. It some cases negatively, related to guilt residuals.4
is composed of 30 items (15 state items and 15 trait items), each rated on One question that arises in the interpretation of null results
a 4-point scale. Spielberger et al. (1970) presented considerable data involving part correlational analyses is whether there remains
supporting the reliability and validity of the measure. In our Study 1,
in the residual variable any meaningful variance beyond mea-
estimates of internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha) for the State Anxi-
ety and Trait Anxiety scales were .94 and .90, respectively. In Study 2, surement error—that is, whether in partialing out the variance
estimates of internal consistency for the State Anxiety and Trait Anxi- shared with shame we have effectively partialed out all the reli-
ety scales were .94 and .92, respectively. able and valid variance of guilt. This consideration is impor-
6. The Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ; Seligman, Abram- tant, particularly because our measure of guilt-proneness ap-
son, Semmel, & von Baeyer, 1979) consists of 12 hypothetical situa- pears somewhat (although not substantially) less reliable than
tions (6 positive and 6 negative). Respondents provide a likely cause for our measure of shame-proneness (Tangney, 1990). Results in-
each event and then rate, on a 7-point scale, each cause on three dimen-
sions: internal-external, stable-unstable, and global-specific. The rat-
ings are summed across negative and positive situations separately to 2
yield six scales of attributional style: Internality, Stability, and Global- The Study 2 sample was initially randomly split, and item analyses
ity for positive and negative events each. Although the individual scales were conducted on one half so that we could select the final 15 TOSCA
of the ASQ have generally modest reliabilities (e.g., Peterson et al., scenarios from a larger pool of scenarios. Thus the reliability estimates
1982), a number of studies have supported the validity and empirical reported here were based on analyses involving only the other half of
utility of the ASQ (see reviews by Peterson & Seligman, 1984; Robins, the sample. (Item analyses involved only the TOSCA items; none of the
1988). In our studies, internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha) of the other measures used in the study were considered in the preliminary
ASQ scales ranged from .25 to .67 (M= .52). Internal attributions for analyses of the TOSCA.)
3
negative events had, by far, the lowest reliabilities; reliabilities for the The correlation between the SCAAI shame and guilt measures in
other five attributional scales were more substantial. Study 2 was substantially higher than expected. In five previous stud-
Study 1 participants completed the SCA Al, the BDI, and the SCL-90 ies in which the SCAAI was used, shame-guilt correlations have
in Session 1 and the ASQ and the STAI in Session 2. Study 2 partici- ranged from .43 to .48. An inspection of the joint distribution for Study
pants completed the TOSCA in Session 1; the SCAAI, the ASQ, the 2 revealed 5 outliers. These 5 participants had responded rather idio-
BDI, and the SCL-90 in Session 2; and the STAI in Session 3. syncratically to the SCAAI, primarily making use of the extreme ends
of the 5-point rating scale. Apart from these 5 outliers, the Study 2
sample offers substantial nonshared shame and guilt variance, as ob-
served in previous samples.
Results 4
Results of; tests for dependent correlations indicated that in 28 of
the 36 pairs of bivariate correlations, the magnitude of association
Shame-Proneness, Guih-Proneness, and Psychopathology involving shame was significantly different from that involving guilt.
In 35 of the 36 pairs, the shame correlation was higher than the guilt
Table 1 contains the correlations of proneness to shame and
correlation—a pattern highly unlikely to be observed by chance, as
proneness to guilt with indices of psychopathology from Study indicated by a sign test (z = 5.50, p < .001). Similarly, / tests for indepen-
1 and Study 2. Correlations for both the SCAAI and the TOSCA dent correlations indicated that in 29 of the 36 pairs of part correla-
in Study 2 are presented. In both studies, the tendency to experi- tions, the correlates of the shame residuals were significantly different
ence shame across a range of situations was strongly related to from those of the guilt residuals. Again, in 35 of the 36 pairs, the part
psychological maladjustment in general. All 12 indices of psy- correlation involving shame was higher than the part correlation in-
chopathology were positively and significantly correlated with volving guilt.
SHAME, GUILT, AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 473

Table 1
Relation of Shame-Proneness and Guilt-Proneness to Indices of Psychopathology
Bivariate correlations Part correlations: Residuals

Index Shame Guilt Shame Guilt

Symptom Checklist 90
Somatization
Study 1 (SCAAI) .14* .16* .07 .11
Sutdy 2 (SCAAI) .26*** .18** .18** .03
Study 2 (TOSCA) _27**« .11 .25***" -.02
Obsessive-Compulsive
Study 1 (SCAAI) .31*** .19** .25***" .05
Study 2 (SCAAI) .38***" .21*** .32***" -.03
Study 2 (TOSCA) .38***" .22*** .31***" .05
Psychoticism b
Study 1 (SCAAI) .25***' .11 22*** -.01
Study 2 (SCAAI) .39***" .23*** ;32***b -.03
Study 2 (TOSCA) .34***" .15* 31***b -.01
Paranoid Ideation
Study 1 (SCAAI) .23*** .14* .19** .04
Study 2 (SCAAI) .40***" .17* .38***b -.11
Study 2 (TOSCA) .35***' .08 .35***" -.09
Hostility-Anger
Study 1 (SCAAI) .13* .03 .13* -.03
Study 2 (SCAAI) .19*** .01 .23***" -.14*
Study 2 (TOSCA) .20**" -.05 24***b -.15**
Interpersonal Sensitivity
Study 1 (SCAAI) .42***" .22*** .36***" .03
Study 2 (SCAAI) .56***' .29*** 49***b -.08
Study 2 (TOSCA) .46***" .21*** 42***b -.00
Anxiety
Study 1 (SCAAI) .24*** .18** .18** .08
Study 2 (SCAAI) .36***' .25*** .26***" .03
Study 2 (TOSCA) .34***' .19** 2g***b .04
Phobic Anxiety
Study 1 (SCAAI) .27*** .16** .21*** .05
Study 2 (SCAAI) .32***a .18** .26***" -.03
Study 2 (TOSCA) .25***' .01 27**#b -.11*
Depression
Study 1 (SCAAI) .32*** .24*** .23*** .10
Study 2 (SCAAI) .41***' .22*** .36***b -.05
Study 2 (TOSCA) .43***" .15* .41***" -.06

Other measures
Beck Depression
Study 1 (SCAAI) .34***' .15* .31***" -.01
Study 2 (SCAAI) .47***' .24*** .41***" -.07
Study 2 (TOSCA) .51***" .19** 47***b -.05
Trait Anxiety
Study 1 (SCAAI) .45***' .21*** .40***" .00
Study 2 (SCAAI) .52***' .22** .50***b -.15*
Study 2 (TOSCA) .53***" .17* .51***" -.09
State Anxiety
Study 1 (SCAAI) .38***' .21** 32***b .03
b
Study 2 (SCAAI) .36***' .09 39*** -.17*
Study 2 (TOSCA) .41***' .10 ;41***b -.10

Note. SCAAI = Self-Conscious Affect and Attribution Inventory; TOSCA = Test of Self-Conscious
Affect. Study 1 ns ranged from 228 to 243; Study 2 ns ranged from 206 to 225 and from 211 to 230 for
analyses involving the SCAAI and TOSCA, respectively.
a
t tests for dependent correlations indicate that the shame correlation is significantly higher than the guilt
correlation at at least p < .05. b / tests for independent correlations indicate that the shame part correla-
tion is significantly higher than the guilt part correlation at at least p < .05.
*p<.05. **p<.01. ***p< .001
474 J. TANGNEY, P. WAGNER, AND R. GRAMZOW

volving measures of other constructs, however, indicate that Table 2


this is not the case. In previous studies with the SCAAI, the Relation of Attributional Style to Shame-
TOSCA, or both, guilt residuals (the unique variance in guilt) Proneness and Guilt-Proneness
have shown consistent positive correlations with interpersonal
empathy and with a variety of constructive responses to anger Bivariate Part correlations:
correlations Residuals
(Tangney, 1991; Tangney et al, 1991) and consistent negative
correlations with externalization of blame, resentment toward Attributional style Shame Guilt Shame Guilt
others, a hostile/aggressive sense of humor, and a detached/un-
concerned attitude in response to negative events (Gessner & Positive events
Tangney, 1990; Tangney, 1990; Tangney et al, 1992). Thus the Internality
negligible correlations between indices of psychopathology and Study 1 (SCAAI) -.14* -.08 -.12 -.01
guilt residuals are not simply artifacts of a restriction of valid Study 2 (SCAAI) -.30*** — 22** -.21** -.04
Study 2 (TOSCA) -.21*** -.11 -.18** -.02
variance in the residual guilt variables. Stability
In summary, our findings failed to support Lewis's (1971) Study 1 (SCAAI) -.17* -.16* -.11 -.08
notion that shame and guilt are differentially related to unique Study 2 (SCAAI) -.26*** -.01 -.32*** .19*'
symptom clusters. Rather, shame-proneness was associated Study 2 (TOSCA) -.19** .08 -.25*** .19*'
with a wide variety of pathological features; guilt-proneness, Globality
Study 1 (SCAAI) .00 .08 -.04 .09
independent of shame, appears to have few implications for Study 2 (SCAAI) -.16* .02 -.23*** .16*
psychological maladjustment. Study 2 (TOSCA) -.10 .08 -.15* .15*
Negative events
Shame-Proneness, Guilt-Proneness, and Attributional
Style Internality
Study 1 (SCAAI) .16* .11 .13 .04
In recent years, cognitive theories have dominated the clini- Study 2 (SCAAI) .29*** .21** .21** .04
Study 2 (TOSCA) .29*** .16* .24*** .03
cal literature pertaining to depression and anxiety (e.g., Abram- Stability
son et al., 1978; Beck, 1983). One question that inevitably arises Study 1 (SCAAI) .15* .01 .16* -.07
when shame and guilt are considered is the degree to which Study 2 (SCAAI) .21** .08 .20** -.07
these constructs (or measures of these constructs) simply reflect Study 2 (TOSCA) .17** .05 .17** -.03
individual differences in explanatory style. As noted earlier, the Globality
Study 1 (SCAAI) .24*** .19** .18** .08
cognitive components of shame and guilt can be conceptual- Study 2 (SCAAI) .40*** .23*** .33*** -.03
ized in attributional terms. Shame, which focuses on the entire Study 2 (TOSCA) .35*** .17** .31*** .01
self, involves internal, global, and stable attributions, whereas
guilt, which focuses on specific behaviors, involves internal, Note. SCAAI = Self-Conscious Affect and Attribution Inventory;
TOSCA = Test of Self-Conscious Affect. Study 1 «s ranged from 227 to
specific, and presumably unstable attributions. In view of the 228; Study 2 ns ranged from 218 to 224 and from 223 to 229 for analy-
observed links between shame and psychopathology, particu- ses involving the SCAAI and TOSCA, respectively.
larly depression and anxiety, it is important to examine whether */7<.05. **/7<.01. ***/7<.001.

these findings are simply a reflection of participants' attribu-


tional style. If so, a consideration of shame and guilt may be
largely superfluous to the understanding of psychological mal- the shared variance with shame. Guilt residuals were unrelated
adjustment. Individual differences in cognitive style may pro- to dimensions of attributional style for negative events. There
vide a more parsimonious (and mainstream) framework for in- were, however, indications in Study 2 (involving both the
terpreting these results. SCAAI and the TOSCA) that the unique variance in guilt may
To answer this question, we first directly examined the rela- be associated with stable and global attributions for positive
tion of shame-proneness and guilt-proneness to attributional events. (The pattern of results was relatively independent of the
style as measured by the ASQ (see Table 2). The results across differential reliabilities of the ASQ subscales. For example, in-
studies clearly indicate that shame-proneness is linked to a de- ternal, not stable, attributions for negative events showed the
pressogenic attributional style. Proneness to shame (and the lowest reliabilities.)
unique variance in shame) was positively correlated with the
tendency to make internal, stable, and global attributions for Attributional Style and Affective Style as Predictors of
negative events and negatively correlated with internal, stable, Depression
and (to a lesser extent) global attributions for positive events.
The attributional style correlates of guilt-proneness were less Across both studies, shame-proneness was clearly related to a
consistent and did not reflect the expected pattern of results— depressogenic attributional style. Does a consideration of self-
that is, correlations with internal but unstable and specific at- conscious affective style contribute to the understanding of de-
tributions for negative events. With regard to the bivariate pression, above and beyond the variance accounted for by attri-
correlations, guilt was positively associated with global attribu- butional style? To answer this question, we conducted a series
tions for negative events in both studies, as well as with internal of multiple regression analyses to predict BDI and SCL-90 de-
attributions for negative events in Study 2. Part correlational pression scores from attributional style and affective style vari-
analyses revealed that these associations resulted entirely from ables. In doing so, we used hierarchical regression procedures,
SHAME, GUILT, AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 475

forcing in attributional style dimensions first, to provide the inclusion of the affective component of shame (e.g., once attri-
most conservative test of the predictive utility of shame-prone- butional factors were controlled for) essentially doubled the
ness and guilt-proneness. proportion of variance predicted in depression.
Tables 3 and 4 show that in both Studies 1 and 2, attributional
style accounted for a significant portion of the variance in de- Discussion
pression (for the BDI, 9% and 13%, respectively; for SCL-90
depression, 7% and 14%, respectively). Of the six attributional In a series of previous studies (Tangney, 1989a, 1991; Tang-
dimensions, internal attributions for negative events was a sig- ney et al., 1992), we presented evidence suggesting that shame-
nificant predictor of both BDI and SCL-90 depression scores in prone persons have rather pervasive difficulties in interper-
Study 1, and unstable attributions for positive events was also sonal relationships. Shame-proneness was associated with im-
significantly predictive of Study 1 BDI scores. In Study 2, global paired empathic abilities, a tendency to externalize blame, and
attributions for negative events was a significant predictor of frequent bouts of anger and hostility. The results of our current
both BDI and SCL-90 scores, and internal attributions for nega- studies extend our earlier findings regarding the maladaptive
tive events was also significantly predictive of Study 2 BDI nature of shame from the interpersonal to the intrapersonal
scores. realm.
Shame-proneness and guilt-proneness were forced into the Shame-prone persons appear vulnerable to a host of psycho-
regression equations after attributional style variables. In each logical symptoms. Across two independent studies, the second
case, the change in R2 associated with these affective variables of which involved two measures of shame, proneness to shame
was substantial. In the prediction of BDI scores, SCAAI shame was consistently related to a range of symptom clusters. Further-
and guilt subscales accounted for an additional 8% and 12% of more, although shame-proneness was clearly related to a de-
the variance in depression in Studies 1 and 2, respectively. In pressogenic attributional style, our findings indicate that the
Study 2, TOSCA shame and guilt subscales accounted for an link between shame and depression is not solely due to attribu-
additional 15% of the variance in BDI scores. In the prediction tional factors. Shame-proneness accounted for a substantial
of SCL-90 depression scores, SCAAI shame and guilt subscales portion of variance in depression, above and beyond the vari-
accounted for an additional 9% of the variance in depression in ance accounted for by attributional style.
both Studies 1 and 2. In Study 2, TOSCA shame and guilt sub- Our studies were correlational in nature and thus cannot
scales accounted for an additional 10% of the variance in SCL- directly address the causal links between shame-proneness and
90 depression scores. In all cases, the change in R2 resulted psychological maladjustment. It seems likely, however, that the
almost entirely from individual differences in shame-prone- relation between affective style and psychopathology is com-
ness. Guilt-proneness emerged as a significant predictor of de- plex. First, shame is a painfully devastating experience that, at
pression only in the Study 1 analysis involving the SCL-90. least temporarily, leads to a crippling of adaptive self-functions.
These findings clearly indicate that the link between shame Thus the tendency to experience shame across a range of situa-
and depression is not solely due to attributional style. In fact, tions may render people more vulnerable to a variety of psycho-

Table 3
Hierarchical Regression Analyses Predicting Depression (BDI) From Attributional
Style Variables, Shame-Proneness, and Guilt-Proneness
BDI scores
Study 1: SCAAI Study 2: SCAAI Study 2: TOSCA
2 2
Variable 0 R change /3 R change 0 R2 change
Step 1 .09** .13*** .13***
Positive events
Internality .04 -.07 -.08
Stability -.23* -.09 -.09
Globality .07 .05 .04
Negative events
Internality .20** .16* .16*
Stability .05 .02 .01
Globality .02 .21** .21**
Step 2 .08*** .12*** .15***
Guilt-proneness .01 -.11 -.05
Shame-proneness .29*** .45*** .45***
Total .17*** .25*** .29***
Note. BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; SCAAI = Self-Conscious Affect and Attribution Inventory;
TOSCA = Test of Self-Conscious Affect. Study 1 /i = 227; Study 2 ns = 216 and 221 for analyses involving
the SCAAI and TOSCA, respectively. The regression coefficients are standardized betas.
*p<.05. **/)<.01 ***;?<.001.
476 J. TANGNEY, P. WAGNER, AND R. GRAMZOW

Table 4
Hierarchical Regression Analyses Predicting Depression (SCL-90) From Attributional
Style Variables, Shame-Proneness, and Guilt-Proneness
SCL-90 Depression scores

Study 1 : SCAAI Study 2: SCAAI Study 2: TOSCA

Variable 0 R2 change /3 R2 change & R2 change

Step 1 .07* .14*** .14***


Positive events
Internality .07 -.03 -.03
Stability -.13 -.07 -.07
Globality .02 -.02 -.02
Negative events
Internality 22** .13 .13
Stability -.00 -.02 -.02
Globality .09 29*** .28***
Step 2 .09*** .09*** .10***
Guilt-proneness .14* -.09* -.08
Shame-proneness .24** .39*** .38***
Total .16*** .23*** .24***

Note. SCL-90 = Symptom Checklist-90; SCAAI = Self-Conscious Affect and Attribution Inventory;
TOSCA = Test of Self-Conscious Affect. Study t n = 226; Study 2 ns = 213 and 218 for analyses involving
the SCAAI and TOSCA, respectively. The regression coefficients are standardized betas.
*p<.Q5. **p<.0\. ***p<.001.

logical disorders because of repeated disruptions in self-func- gize, or in some way make amends for the bad thing that was
tioning. Second, shame-prone persons may also be prone to done—and then move on to the business of everyday life.
feelings of hopelessness, which have been cited as a proximal The results address not only the links between shame and
cause of hopelessness depression and possibly other types of guilt and psychopathology but also the links between shame
disorders (Abramson, Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989). Abramson et al. and guilt and attributional style. Lewis (1987b) and Hoblitzelle
(1989) noted that internal, stable, and global attributions for (1987) noted conceptual parallels between attributional dimen-
negative events and associated inferred negative characteristics sions and the cognitive components of shame and guilt. Our
about the self (each components of the shame experience) are findings underscore the fact that shame and guilt are not
key contributory causes of hopelessness and hopelessness de- merely new affective labels for old cognitive styles. Rather, the
pression. Third, the recognition and experience of psychologi- constructs of shame and guilt are related to but distinct from
cal symptoms may place people at higher risk for further bouts attributional style factors. As predicted, shame-proneness was
of shame. People may feel ashamed of the psychological symp- associated with a depressogenic attributional style, but shame
toms themselves. Finally, both shame-proneness and various makes a substantial and unique contribution to the under-
forms of psychopathology likely share common etiological standing of depression, beyond the contribution of attribu-
roots. An examination of family processes in relation to affec- tional factors.
tive style and psychological adjustment, particularly within a Guilt, on the other hand, was less clearly linked to the pre-
longitudinal framework, is an important direction for future dicted pattern of internal, unstable, and specific attributions.
research. Why was the expected pattern of attributional correlates evi-
The negligible relation between guilt and psychopathology dent for shame but not for guilt? One possibility is that internal,
may, at first glance, appear surprising. Freud (1909/1955), for unstable, and specific attributions for negative events can result
example, emphasized the self-punitive aspects of guilt, which in a broader range of reactions, guilt being only one such re-
can lead to rather pervasive psychological symptoms. The guilt sponse. In acknowledging one's role in a negative event, but
portrayed in much of the clinical case literature, however, is an conceptualizing it as fairly transient and specific to the particu-
unusual form of guilt, quite distinct from that experienced in lar situation, a person may experience guilt (e.g., the person may
the normal range. It is typically an insoluble guilt (i.e., the pa- earnestly accept responsibility and experience the tension, re-
tient for whatever reason sees no means for making reparation, gret, and remorse over the "bad thing that was done"). But
apart from rather extreme forms of intrapsychic self-punish- because of the specific and unstable nature of the cause, a per-
ment), and it is typically guilt fused with shame. As noted ear- son might instead downplay the importance of his or her role
lier, shame itself presents an insoluble dilemma because at issue (e.g., "Well, I did it, but it was only one time, and it didn't affect
is a malignant self, not a malignant behavior. Thus the opportu- much anyway"). As a result, the person may experience little
nities for change are generally quite limited. The shame-free negative affect of any kind. In contrast, internal, stable, and
guilt assessed in our studies, on the other hand, is more likely to global attributions for negative events may afford fewer affec-
afford a range of adaptive resolutions. One can confess, apolo- tive reactions. The cognition that"/ did it, I always do it, and it
SHAME, GUILT, AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 477

affects a great dear may more consistently lead to a shamelike MMPI: A step in the validation of a new self-report scale. British
affective reaction, as well as to feelings of hopelessness, depres- Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 280-289.
sion, and so forth. Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: W W Norton.
We conducted our studies with nonclinical samples of univer- Freud, S. (1953). Further remarks on the defence neuro-psychosis. In J.
sity undergraduates, and although considerable variance was Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of 'the complete psycho-
logical works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 3, pp. 157-185). London: Ho-
observed in the indices of psychopathology, there remains the
garth. (Original work published 1896)
question of whether a different pattern of results would emerge Freud, S. (1953). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. In J. Strachey
in a study of the links between affective style and psychopathol- (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological
ogy in a clinical population. This possibility warrants investiga- works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 7, pp. 153-243). London: Hogarth.
tion for at least two reasons. First, as noted earlier, the nature of (Original work published 1905)
guilt itself may be qualitatively different in the clinical range, Freud, S. (1955). Notes upon a case of obsessional neurosis. In J. Stra-
perhaps resulting in a different pattern of relations between chey (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychologi-
guilt and psychopathology. Second, in nonclinical samples, psy- cal works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 10, pp. 155-318). London: Ho-
chological symptoms may not be clustered into well-defined garth. (Original work published 1909)
syndromes; rather, reported symptoms may simply reflect a Freud, S. (1957). Mourning and melancholia. In J. Strachey (Ed. &
kind of general negative affectivity (Watson & Clark, 1984). Trans), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of
Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14, pp. 243-258). London: Hogarth. (Original
Thus although we found no evidence to support Lewis's (1971)
work published 1917)
hypothesis that shame and guilt are differentially related to
Freud, S. (1961). The economic problem of masochism. In J. Strachey
unique symptom clusters, the possibility that such a pattern (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological
may emerge in clinical studies remains. works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 19, pp. 159-170). London: Hogarth.
In spite of these limitations, our results linking shame to (Original work published 1924)
psychological maladjustment in general suggest that a consider- Gessner, T. L., & Tangney, J. P. (1990, March). Personality and adjust-
ation of shame-related issues may be useful in the treatment of a ment correlates of wit and witticism. Poster presented at the annual
number of psychological problems. meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Philadelphia.
Hamme, H. (1991). Family correlates of proneness to shame and prone-
ness to guilt. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Bryn Mawr Col-
References lege.
Harder, D. W, & Lewis, S. J. (1987). The assessment of shame and guilt.
Abramson, L. Y, Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness In J. N. Butcher & C. D. Spielberger (Eds), Advances in personality
depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Re- assessment (Vol. 6, pp. 89-114). Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.
view, 96, 358-372. Hoblitzelle, W (1987). Attempts to measure and differentiate shame
Abramson, L. Y, Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned and guilt: The relation between shame and depression. In H. B.
helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Ab- Lewis (Ed), The role of shame in symptom formation (pp. 207-235).
normal Psychology, 87, 49-74. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Beck, A. T. (1972). Measuring depression: The depression inventory. In Janoff-Bulman, R. (1979). Characterological versus behavioral self-
T. A. Williams, M. M. Katz, & J. A. Shields (Eds.), Recent advances in blame: Inquiries into depression and rape. Journal of Personality and
the psychobiology of the depressive illnesses (pp. 299-302). Washing- Social Psychology, 37, 1798-1809.
ton, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self New York: International
Beck, A. T. (1983). Cognitive therapy of depression: New perspectives. Universities Press.
In P. Clayton & J. Barrett (Eds.), Treatment of depression: Old contro- Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York: Interna-
versies and new approaches (pp. 265-290). New York: Raven Press. tional Universities Press.
Burggraf, S. A., & Tangney, J. P. (1989, June). Proneness to shame, pron- Lewis, H. B. (1987a). Introduction: Shame—the "sleeper" in psychopa-
eness to guilt, and self-concept. Poster presented at the annual meet- thology. In H. B. Lewis (Ed), The role of shame in symptom forma-
ing of the American Psychological Society, Alexandria, VA. tion (pp. 1-28). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Buss, A. H., & Durkee, A. (1957). An inventory for assessing different Lewis, H. B. (1987b). The role of shame in depression over the life span.
kinds of hostility in clinical situations. Journal of Consulting Psychol- In H. B. Lewis (Ed), The role of shame in symptom formation (pp.
ogy, 21, 343-348. 29-50). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Crouppen, G. A. (1977). Field dependence-independence in depres- Lindsay-Hartz, J. (1984). Contrasting experiences of shame and guilt.
sive and "normal" males as an indicator of relative proneness to American Behavioral Scientist, 27, 689-704.
shame or guilt and ego-functioning. Dissertation Abstracts Interna- Miller, S. (1985). The shame experience. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tional, 37, 4669B-4670B. (University Microfilms No. 77-6292) Morrison, A. P. (1989). Shame: The underside of narcissism. Hillsdale,
DeRivera, J. (1977). A structural theory of emotion. New York: Interna- NJ: Analytic Press.
tional Universities Press. Morrison, N. K. (1987). The role of shame in schizophrenia. In H. B.
Derogatis, L. R. (1989). Description and bibliography for the SCL-90-R Lewis (Ed), The role of shame in symptom formation (pp. 51-87).
and other instruments of the psychopathology rating scale series. Rid- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
erwood, MD: Clinical Psychometric Research, Inc. Mosher, D. L. (1966). The development and multitrait-multimethod
Derogatis, L. R., & Cleary, P (1977). Confirmation of the dimensional matrix analysis of three measures of three aspects of guilt. Journal of
structure of the SCL-90: A study in construct validation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 30, 25-29.
Clinical Psychology, 33, 981-989. Nathanson, D. L. (1987). A timetable for shame. In D. L. Nathanson
Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., & Covi, L. (1973). SCL-90: An outpa- (Ed), The many faces of shame (pp. 1-63). New York: Guilford.
tient psychiatric rating scale—Preliminary report. Psychopharma- Peterson, C, & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk
cology Bulletin, 9, 13-28. factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review,
Derogatis, L. R., Rickels, K., & Rock, A. (1976). The SCL-90 and the
478 J. TANGNEY, P. WAGNER, AND R. GRAMZOW

Peterson, C, Semmel, A., von Baeyer, C, Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, Tangney, J. P. (1991). Moral affect: The good, the bad, and the ugly.
G. I., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). The Attributional Style Question- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 598-607.
naire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6, 287-299. Tangney, J. P., Burggraf, S. A., Hamme, H., & Domingos, B. (1988,
Robins, C. J. (1988). Attributions and depression: Why is the literature March). Assessing individual differences in proneness to shame and
so inconsistent? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, guilt: The Self-Conscious Affect and Attribution Inventory. Poster pre-
880-889. sented at the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Associa-
Seligman, M. E. P., Abramson, L. Y, Semmel, A., & von Baeyer, C. tion, Buffalo, NY
(1979). Depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychol- Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., & Gramzow, R. (1989). The Test of Self-
ogy, 88, 242-247. Conscious Affect. Fairfax, VA: George Mason University.
Smith, R. L. (1972). The relative proneness to shame or guilt as an Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., Fletcher, C., & Gramzow, R. (1992). Shamed
indicator of defensive style. Dissertation Abstracts International, 33, into anger? The relation of shame and guilt to anger and self-re-
2823B. (University Microfilms No. 72-3258) ported aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62,
Spielberger, C. D, Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for 669-675.
the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychol- Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., Gavlas, J., Hansbarger, A., Marschall, D., &
ogists Press. Gramzow, R. (1991). Constructive vs. destructive responses to anger:
Tangney, J. P. (1989a, April). Shame-proneness, guilt-proneness, and The moderating role of shame and guilt across the lifespan. Manu-
interpersonal processes. In J. P. Tangney (Chair), Self-conscious script in preparation.
emotions and social behavior. Symposium conducted at the annual Watson, D, & Clark, L. A. (1984) Negative affectivity: The disposition
meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 96,
City. 465-490.
Tangney, J. P. (1989b, August). A quantitative assessment of phenomena- Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation
logical differences between shame and guilt. Poster presented at the and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548-573.
annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New Wicker, F. W, Payne, G. C., & Morgan, R. D. (1983). Participant de-
scriptions of guilt and shame. Motivation and Emotion, 7, 25-39.
Orleans.
Tangney, J. P. (1990). Assessing individual differences in proneness to
shame and guilt: Development of the Self-Conscious Affect and At- Received August 20,1990
tribution Inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, Revision received November 21,1991
102-111. Accepted November 21,1991 •

View publication stats

You might also like