You are on page 1of 10

Biological Psychology 85 (2010) 258–267

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Biological Psychology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho

Developmental aspects of language lateralization in delta, theta, alpha and beta


EEG bands
Chiara Spironelli a , Alessandro Angrilli a,b,∗
a
University of Padova, Department of General Psychology, 35131 Padova, Italy
b
CNR Institute of Neuroscience, 35121 Padova, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study aimed to clarify the functional role of several EEG bands across age by analyzing language hemi-
Received 12 February 2010 spheric lateralization in three linguistic tasks. Twenty-eight children, 22 young adults and 20 middle-aged
Accepted 16 July 2010 participants were administered the same sample of written words and normalized amplitudes of ␦, ϑ,
Available online 24 July 2010
␣ and ␤ bands were analyzed. Only young adults showed rightwards lateralization in ␦ frequency band.
Task-dependent right-lateralization marked adults’ ϑ and ␣ distributions, whereas only linguistic vs.
Keywords:
non-linguistic differences were found in children. Concerning the high-beta band, all groups showed
Spectral analysis
left-lateralized linguistic effects. In adults slow rhythms represent indices of active inhibition of task-
Aging
Development
irrelevant regions, but in children they marked incomplete automated linguistic processing. The high-beta
Hemispheric lateralization band revealed task-relevant left hemisphere linguistic activity in all groups, therefore representing the
Phonological processing most reliable EEG marker of language hemispheric dominance in children and adults.
Semantics © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction the insular cortex). Indeed, results obtained by studies on lan-


guage development have suggested that brain plastic mechanisms
Language is a feature which typically distinguishes humans involved in linguistic learning are different from those recruited
from animals and is associated with specific hemispheric lateral- later for automated, skilled language. For example, one 10-year
ization (Crow, 2004). Indeed, the functional lateralization of the longitudinal study (Stiles et al., 2002) on children suffering from
human brain appears necessary for the full realization of the lin- pre- or peri-natal focal brain injury showed that linguistic learn-
guistic potential (Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985; Hiscock, 1998; ing includes a cortical network which becomes redundant once
Luria, 1973; Pulvermüller, 2001). Results achieved by functional the learning is complete—a process which largely takes place in
Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), Positron Emission Tomogra- the early phases of language development. Thus, the achievement
phy (PET) or functional Trans-Cranial Doppler (fTCD) imaging have of the typical left hemisphere specialization for linguistic func-
clearly demonstrated that the most steady laterality effects are reli- tioning can only be attained once early critical stages of learning
ably located over the left inferior frontal areas (e.g., Benson et al., are completed. Concerning written language, Bakker (1979, 2006)
1999; Démonet et al., 1994; Deppe et al., 2000; Frith et al., 1991; carried out a series of ERP studies on word reading in young read-
Liotti et al., 1994; Petersen et al., 1988; Rutten et al., 2002; Xiang ers with limited linguistic competence, and suggested that early
et al., 2010; Zatorre et al., 1992). However, in their review Fiez phases of learning to read are mediated by a strategy which mainly
and Petersen (1998) identified a complex neural network which involves the right hemisphere. When children gradually develop
selectively involves not only specific regions within the left hemi- more complex writing skills, reading strategies change accordingly:
sphere (e.g., the ventral occipito-temporal cortex, the fusiform and advanced readers show a shift from the right to the typical left
lingual gyri, and the frontal operculum, among others), but also dominance (Bakker, 1979). Thus, speech and comprehension are
some important areas distributed in both hemispheres (e.g., the complete in infants and children (Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2010;
anterior/posterior portions of the superior temporal gyrus, the dor- Friederici et al., 2007), as both these skills start to develop since
sal/ventral portions of the postcentral gyrus, the basal ganglia and the first year of age, whereas the capacity for writing and read-
ing at this age is significantly delayed and not yet fully developed.
Indeed, the study of Grossi et al. (2001) which aimed at analyzing
the developmental aspects of language lateralization during writ-
∗ Corresponding author at: University of Padova, Department of General Psychol-
ten word reading in different age groups (ranging from 7 to 23
ogy, via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy.
years), showed that the left lateralization of the Contingent Neg-
Tel.: +39 049 8276692; fax: +39 049 8276600.
E-mail address: alessandro.angrilli@unipd.it (A. Angrilli).
ative Variation (CNV), a slow wave elicited by a S1–S2 paradigm,

0301-0511/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.07.011
C. Spironelli, A. Angrilli / Biological Psychology 85 (2010) 258–267 259

increased linearly with age until 19–20 years. In a previous study EEG bands, there is a rhythm which can reflect the specialized
on the developmental dynamics of the N150—recognition potential left hemisphere linguistic networks necessary for an efficient, yet
(Spironelli and Angrilli, 2009a), we found a lack of left hemisphere not perfect, reading, in school-age children. A related scientific
linguistic dominance in children also in the early automatic phases question arises regarding the possible degradation of left linguistic
of word recognition. networks due to normal aging. Therefore, based on our previously
Despite their low spatial resolution, electroencephalography validated paradigm (Penolazzi et al., 2008; Spironelli and Angrilli,
and magnetoencephalography provide very accurate temporal res- 2006, 2009b; Spironelli et al., 2006, 2008), we carried out a spec-
olution and allow the identification of the cortical activity which tral FFT analysis on the same sample of 10-year-old children, young
can be time- or phase-locked to a stimulus or task, for example by adults, and middle-aged participants used in our previous work on
means of evoked potential analysis. Another approach consists of early ERP components (Spironelli and Angrilli, 2009a). The analysis
the analysis of oscillatory characteristics of the signal by means, for of a large number of EEG bands allowed us to clarify the func-
example, of time-frequency methods. Thus, it is possible to describe tional role of low and high frequency bands in different age groups
EEG in terms of its frequency components – which range from 1 using the same experimental paradigm and tasks. Indeed, children’s
to 100 Hz – over two main functional ranges, that is low frequency frontal lobes are not mature and fully myelinizated yet, and this
bands (delta: 0.5–4 Hz; theta: 4–8 Hz; alpha: 8–13 Hz), ascribed the would be detected by slow frequency bands. In addition, left frontal
broad functional role of cortical inhibition to slow rhythms (e.g., areas are important organizers of the whole language (Bookheimer,
Adrian and Matthews, 1934; Pfurtscheller et al., 1996), and high 2002, Hagoort, 2005) and these are expected to be less lateral-
frequency bands (beta: 13–30 Hz; gamma: 30–100 Hz), associated ized in children. In detail, we expected: (1) in accordance with
with cortical activation and neural networks integration (e.g., Ray the maturational lag hypothesis (e.g., Harmony et al., 1990), that
and Cole, 1985; Steriade, 2000). Recent studies have, however, sug- children show higher delta amplitude than adults, particularly in
gested a re-evaluation of the functional interpretation of EEG bands. the left anterior regions involved in linguistic processing and inte-
For example, von Stein and Sarnthein (2000) have claimed that, gration (Bookheimer, 2002; Hagoort, 2005; Xiang et al., 2010); (2)
with the exception of the delta rhythm, all other bands mark differ- that children have greater left hemispheric theta and alpha ampli-
ent levels of cortical, top-down integration. Palva and Palva (2007) tude compared with adults, since linguistic processes underlying
came to the same conclusions, and suggested that alpha, beta and written word reading are not entirely automated in 10-year-old
gamma bands are complementary and coexisting rhythms which readers, and the adopted paradigm is suited to load the verbal
are necessary to the unification process of high level cognitive working memory (Angrilli et al., 2000; Klimesch, 1999; Spironelli
operations associated to cognitive demands. Whereas the func- and Angrilli, 2006; Spironelli et al., 2006, 2008); (3) that both adult
tional meaning of high frequency bands, such as beta and gamma groups exhibit higher levels of theta and alpha rhythms in right
rhythms, is unanimously accepted, depending on the experimen- vs. left hemisphere, especially during the phonological processing
tal paradigm, as a marker of cognitive processing, integration and which requires an active inhibition of contralateral, non-linguistic
learning (e.g., Jensen et al., 2007; Miltner et al., 1999; Tallon- regions (Klimesch et al., 2007; Spironelli and Angrilli, 2006); (4) that
Baudry and Bertrand, 1999; Weiss and Rappelsberger, 1998), low adults have overall greater beta band percentage than children, and
frequency bands, such as theta and alpha, have been recently asso- show greater left vs. right hemisphere lateralization especially in
ciated with either working memory engagement (Bastiaansen et the Phonological task.
al., 2002; Scheeringa et al., 2009; for a review, see Klimesch, 1999)
2. Methods
or active inhibition of task-irrelevant cortical regions (for a review,
see Klimesch et al., 2007). Concerning the delta frequency, this slow 2.1. Participants
rhythm is typically observed in the deepest stages of sleep and is
strongly reduced during adults’ awake states (e.g., Steriade, 2000). Twenty-eight children (14 girls; mean age: 10.01 years, SD: ±0.18 years), 22
young adults (11 females; mean age: 22.59 years, SD: ±3.67 years) and 20 middle-
In addition, when it appears in wakefulness, the delta activity is
aged participants (7 females; mean age: 59.10 years, SD: ±7.11 years) entered the
considered a marker of brain damage or pathological conditions experiment. All subjects were native Italians fully right-handed (on average above
resulting from neurological damage or psychiatric disease (e.g., 90% according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; Oldfield, 1971), and had
Babiloni et al., 2006; De Jongh et al., 2001; Penolazzi et al., 2008; normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None of the participants had been treated
Spironelli and Angrilli, 2009b; Tanaka et al., 1998; Vieth et al., 2001; for any neurological or psychiatric disorder, nor were they under pharmacological
treatment at the time of the experimental session.
Wienbruch et al., 2003).
All adults and children’s parents gave their written informed consent to the
A few studies have investigated EEG bands across age, with study, according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The experimental procedures were
a developmental approach. Indeed, the progressive increase in approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology, University of Padova.
beta power in individuals aged from 7 to 60 years was associ-
2.2. Stimuli, tasks, and procedure
ated with the parallel decrease in alpha and theta power (Anokhin
et al., 1996). Age dependent fine-grained differences have been The stimuli consisted of bi- or trisyllabic Italian content words, selected from
found for theta activity, in children ranging from 6 to 11 years of a frequency dictionary of 5000 written Italian words (Bortolini et al., 1972). Words
age (Yordanova and Kolev, 1997) and, for the EEG alpha band, in were visually presented in pairs on a 17 computer monitor, one at a time with an
young adults compared with middle-aged participants at frontal intertrial interval of 3 s. The first word (W1) remained on the screen 1.5 s for children
and 1 s for both adult groups. After a fixed interstimulus interval of 2 s, the second
sites (Kolev et al., 2002). However, to our knowledge, no EEG word (W2) was presented until the participant responded by pressing a keyboard
research has been carried out on functional and developmental button, but for an interval no longer than 5 s. Word pairs were administered in three
aspects of linguistic hemispheric specialization for written word separate blocks corresponding to three tasks, in which the same words were pre-
processing. Thus, the present study aimed at investigating the elec- sented as W1, but in a different randomized order. In the Phonological task, upon
W2 target presentation, participants had to decide whether word pairs rhymed; in
trophysiological correlates of reading mechanisms in school-age
the Semantic task, whether target word W2 was of the same semantic category as
children, and the changes associated with a progressive reading W1; in the Orthographic task (used as control), whether word pairs were written in
skill development in young and middle-aged adults. Indeed, it is the same case (see Spironelli et al., 2006, 2008, for details of the whole experimen-
not clear whether competent 10-year-old readers have developed tal procedure). Therefore, both Phonological and Semantic tasks required different
a full left hemisphere specialization for written words and, at the levels of linguistic processing, whereas the Orthographic control task requested a
simple visuo-perceptual word matching, since the linguistic access was not neces-
same time, whether middle-aged adults maintain the same left sary. To perform motor responses, participants used their left index or middle finger
hemisphere linguistic dominance with respect to younger adults. for pressing two keyboard buttons corresponding to match-mismatch conditions.
A second, important aim was to investigate whether, among all Each task included 80 trials/word pairs. In all tasks, 50% matches were randomly
260 C. Spironelli, A. Angrilli / Biological Psychology 85 (2010) 258–267

interspersed with 50% mismatches. The task order was randomly varied across par-
ticipants. Since we used the same sample of words as W1 in all tasks, we can expect
that any statistical differences found between tasks (including the interaction of task
with the other factors) are due only to the specific processes engaged by the task
itself and not to possible confounding factors differing between samples of stimuli, a
problem that often raises in experimental designs using different samples of stimuli
for different tasks. In addition, we administered one task at a time: this experimen-
tal strategy stimulated participants to process the first word (W1) depending on
the specific linguistic process required by the task and to keep the critical linguistic
features in the working memory until the second word (W2) appeared. In support
of this choice, we have demonstrated in a number of past studies with both ERPs
and EEG bands that both W1 and ISI elicit linguistic task-related reliable (and cross-
linguistic) effects (Angrilli et al., 2000; Mondini et al., 2008; Spironelli and Angrilli,
2006, 2007, 2009a, 2009b; Spironelli et al., 2006, 2008, 2010, in press).
At the beginning of the experiment, all participants were administered a direct
Digit Span (DS) test in order to verify the extent of their verbal short-term memory.

2.3. Data acquisition and analysis

EEG was recorded by 38 tin electrodes; 31 mounted on an elastic cap (Elec-


troCap) according to the International 10–20 system (Oostenveld and Praamstra,
2001). The other 7 electrodes were applied below each eye (Io1, Io2), on the two
external canthi (F9, F10), nasion (Nz) and the mastoids (M1, M2). All cortical sites
Fig. 1. Contribution of each EEG band, ranging from low-delta to high-gamma
were on-line referred to Cz as this site is located at the topographical center of the
frequencies, across the normalized EEG spectrum (0.98–100 Hz) in children
scalp and is not used for analysis of lateralization. Data were off-line re-referenced
(left column), young adults (central column) and middle-aged participants
to the average reference: this solution allowed us to analyze, in addition to all
(right column). ␦ = delta rhythm; ϑ = theta rhythm; ␣ = alpha rhythm; ␤1 = low-
other important linguistic sites, the mastoid-temporal lobe activity. Data were
beta rhythm; ␤2 = high-beta rhythm; ␤3 = beta/gamma rhythm; ␥1 = low-gamma
stored using the acquire software NeuroScan 4.1 version (NeuroScan Labs, Sterling,
rhythm; ␥2 = high-gamma rhythm.
USA). Amplitude resolution was 0.168 ␮V/bin; the bandwidth ranged from DC to
100 Hz (6 dB/octave). The sampling rate was set at 500 Hz and impedance was kept
below 5 K.
Behavioral measures collected from each subject included error rates (ERs) and
response times (RTs) to the second stimulus. EEG was continuously recorded in DC the 0.98–100 Hz range). Depending on the rationale of our paradigm, which was
mode and stored for later analysis. After the completion of the data collection, the aimed to measure language lateralization, the percentage-normalization allowed
EEG signal was corrected for blinks and eye movement artifacts (according to Ille us to compare the same scalp locations in all samples and, at the same time, to
et al., 2002) by BESA software (Brain Electrical Source Analysis, 5.1 version). Each quantify the relative contribution of each EEG band with respect to total spectral
EEG trial epoch was divided into four 1024-ms time intervals, and thus included power (therefore as a percentage) in different age groups (Fig. 1): a strategic choice
512 samples with 0.98 Hz FFT resolution. Each interval corresponds to a different useful for measuring laterality phenomena in a developmental perspective. To avoid
word processing phase required by the task: 1024 ms before W1 onset (Baseline); the 50 Hz noise artifact, the frequency lines corresponding to the interval between
1024 ms after W1 onset (Word 1, W1); the first second after W1 offset, i.e., from 47.9 and 51.8 Hz (three samples) were eliminated. The delta band3 (1–4 Hz, effective
1500 to 2524 ms for children and from 1000 to 2024 ms for both adult groups (Ini- ␦ range: 0.98–3.91 Hz), theta band (4–8 Hz, effective ϑ range: 4.88–7.82 Hz), alpha
tial Inter Stimulus Interval, iISI); the last second after W1 offset (before W2 onset), band (8–13 Hz, effective ␣ range: 8.79–12.72 Hz), and beta band (divided in low-beta
i.e., from 2476 to 3500 ms for children and from 1976 to 3000 ms for both adult or ␤1: 13–20 Hz, effective ␤1 range: 13.68–20.52 Hz; as well as the high-beta band
groups (Terminal ISI, tISI). These time windows were selected on the basis of previ- or ␤2: 21–28 Hz, effective ␤2 range: 21.49–28.33 Hz), were analyzed. The gamma
ous studies (Penolazzi et al., 2008; Spironelli et al., 2006, 2008). Given the constraint band (divided into low-gamma or ␥1: 35–50 Hz, effective ␥1 range: 36.15–47.87 Hz;
of the Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) to use 2n samples, the width of each interval and high-gamma or ␥2: 50–65 Hz, effective ␥2 range: 51.78–65.46 Hz) and other
was necessarily forced to 512 samples, corresponding to a 1024-ms interval. The ISI high frequency bands (i.e., 80–100 Hz, effective range: 80.11–99.65 Hz) completed
therefore contained a very small overlap (48 ms, <5%) between iISI and tISI. This par- the whole EEG spectrum (Fig. 1), but were not included in the present statistical
tial overlap was a consequence of the decision not to change the main parameters of analysis, which considers only conventional EEG spectral bands. As in our previous
our validated paradigm on language lateralization (a solution which allowed us to study, for statistical purposes, electrodes were clustered into four groups/regions
analyze, in prior research, also evoked potentials). However, the possible problem of interest with two spatial factors consisting of two levels each: antero-posterior
related to the small overlap among time intervals is further limited by the win- asymmetry and laterality (Spironelli et al., 2006). Each quadrant therefore com-
dowing which cancels out the contribution of the extreme portions of the intervals. prised the averaged amplitude of four electrodes: anterior left (AL: F3, FC3, F7, FT7),
Artifact rejection was performed on each epoch, with both amplitude and deriva- anterior right (AR: F4, FC4, F8, FT8), posterior left (PL: P3, P7, M1, O1), and pos-
tive (with respect to time) thresholds1 (150 ␮V and 100 ␮V/ms, respectively). The terior right (PR: P4, P8, M2, O2). Since preliminary analyses carried out for each
remaining epochs were then visually inspected for any residual artifacts. On aver- EEG band revealed no effects susceptible to the time course, we decided to col-
age, 15% of the trials were rejected for children, 7% for young adults and 12% for lapse the three time intervals following the first word onset (i.e., W1, iISI and tISI
middle-aged participants, evenly distributed across tasks. For each participant, and intervals) by computing their mean amplitude. This solution was aimed to increase
for each interval of interest, the FFT was averaged across those epochs which, after the statistical power and improve data presentation making result interpretation
windowing with a tapered cosine, were free of residual artifacts (i.e., on average easier.
68 epochs for children, 74 epochs for young adults and 70 epochs for middle-aged The analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was carried out on the dependent variable
participants). The last step was the percentage-normalization of EEG amplitudes Digit Span including the between-subjects factor Age (three levels: Children vs.
within each electrode: this normalization is a scaling procedure which allows to Young vs. Middle-aged). With regard to behavioral measures (mean ERs and RTs),
compare subjects with very different total spectral power, and was calculated as ANOVA included the between-subjects factor Age (three levels: Children vs. Young
the contribution of each band to the whole 0.98–100 Hz spectral range,2 expressed vs. Middle-aged) and the within-subjects factor Task (three levels: Orthographic vs.
as a percentage (see also Fig. 1; in detail, normalization was computed as the sum of Phonological vs. Semantic).
spectral points of the selected band divided by the sum of all spectral points within On the EEG data separated ANOVAs were carried out for each EEG band
including, again, the factor Age (three levels: Children vs. Young vs. Middle-aged)
and four within-subjects factors: Task (three levels: Orthographic vs. Phonolog-
ical vs. Semantic), Interval (two levels: Baseline vs. Word processing), Region
1
Using BESA software, artifact rejection includes an Amplitude threshold cri- (two levels: Anterior vs. Posterior) and Laterality (two levels: Left vs. Right
terion, that rejects each trial within which the difference between the maximum hemisphere).
and the minimum amplitude is exceeded, and a Derivative threshold criterion, that Post-hoc comparisons were performed using the Tukey HSD test (p < 0.05), and
rejects every trial within which the largest amplitude difference between two adja- the Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied when necessary, that is when vari-
cent time samples is exceeded. ables with more than two levels were involved.
2
Preliminary analyses revealed a Gaussian distribution in all groups (mean,
median, skewness and kurtosis have been controlled for each EEG band) and the
percentage-normalization procedure of EEG bands did not affect the normal dis-
tribution of EEG data. Of course, this transformation makes each EEG rhythm not
3
completely independent from the others; however all data are described and dis- For each EEG band, we indicated the typical frequency range and the “effective
cussed, thus allowing a comprehensive view of all bands. range”, that is the real width of each EEG band.
C. Spironelli, A. Angrilli / Biological Psychology 85 (2010) 258–267 261

Table 1
Behavioral analysis computed on response times (RTs, in milliseconds) and error rates (ERs) for each age group.

Analysis Response times (RTs, in ms) Error rates (ERs)

Task Orth. Phon. Sem. Orth. Phon. Sem.

Children 1497.19 (SD: 424.3) 1530.25 (SD: 401.5) 2041.91 (SD: 471.6) 6.74% (SD: 4.89) 3.13% (SD: 2.53) 10.89% (SD: 4.46)
Young 750.34 (SD: 183.5) 798.12 (SD: 165.2) 968.85 (SD: 196.3) 2.67% (SD: 3.64) 1.36% (SD: 1.33) 4.66% (SD: 3.07)
Middle-aged 874.38 (SD: 228.9) 1021.38 (SD: 300.6) 1121.94 (SD: 236.2) 2.94% (SD: 2.34) 3.25% (SD: 3.54) 4.56% (SD: 2.00)

Orth., Orthographic task; Phon., Phonological task; Sem., Semantic task.

3. Results both Orthographic and Semantic tasks (all p < 0.001; all Cohen’s
d ≥ 0.93, resulting in large effects), but not during the Phonolog-
3.1. Behavioral results ical task (Table 1). Adult groups did not differ in ERs. With regard
to within-group differences, children’s ERs were greater during
In order to control for confounding factors, we carried Semantic and Orthographic tasks (all p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 2.14 and
out separate ANOVAs for Gender, Handedness and Educa- 0.93, very large effects) compared with phonological processing,
tion level. Groups showed no differences in sex distribu- whereas only young adults exhibited greater ERs on the Seman-
tion (F(2,67) = 0.63, p = 0.53) or percentage of right-handedness tic vs. the Phonological task (p < 0.01; Cohen’s d = 1.39, very large
(F(2,67) = 1.31, p = 0.27). As expected, the two-way Age by Edu- effect; Table 1).
cation interaction (F(2,67) = 130.60, p < 0.001) revealed the greater
education level in both adult groups (13 and 11.7 years for Young 3.2. FFT spectral analysis results
and Middle-aged participants, respectively) compared with Chil-
dren (4.5 years; p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 8.68 and 2.96, both very large Fig. 1 shows an overall view of all normalized EEG bands
effects). (0.5–100 Hz) in the three age groups. Low frequency rhythms –
Direct Digit Span (DS) performance was also expected to i.e., delta, theta and alpha bands – decreased with increasing age,
vary across groups. Interestingly, the Age variable (F(2,67) = 12.94, whereas high frequency rhythms – i.e., beta and gamma bands –
p < 0.001) showed greater DS in young adults (7.00) than in either showed the reversed pattern, with increasing amplitude across age.
children (5.43; p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 1.33, very large effect) or Indeed, low frequency bands had larger weight in children, their
middle-aged participants (5.95; p < 0.01; Cohen’s d = 0.93, large percentage making up about 50% of the whole EEG spectrum (Fig. 1,
effect). left column), whereas high frequency bands had a strong influ-
Performance analyses showed slower RTs in children (mean: ence in both adult samples, particularly in the middle-aged group
1690 ms) than in the two adult groups (p < 0.001; means: 839 in which the overall percentage exceeded 70% of the whole EEG
and 1006 ms, Cohen’s d = 2.15 and 1.65, both very large effects, spectrum (Fig. 1, right column).
for young and middle-aged participants, respectively: Age main
effect: F(2,67) = 58.34, p < 0.001). Moreover and regardless of age 3.3. Delta band analysis
group, RTs on the Semantic task (1377 ms) were longer than
those on the Phonological one (1116 ms; Cohen’s d = 0.54, medium Overall, ANOVA carried out on the normalized ␦ band ampli-
effect) which, in turn, were longer than RTs on the Orthographic tude showed a significant main effect of the between-subjects
task (1041 ms; Cohen’s d = 0.15, small effect; Task main effect: factor Age (F(2,67) = 93.74, p < 0.001): children exhibited greater
F(2,134) = 85.40, p < 0.001, GG ε = 0.93). The significant two-way ␦ amplitude (i.e., 17.75%) than both adult groups (all p < 0.001;
Age by Task interaction (F(4,134) = 14.02, p < 0.001, GG ε = 0.93) Cohen’s d ≥ 1.77, resulting in very large effects), and young adults,
revealed that, independent of task, children were slower when in turn, had greater ␦ levels than middle-aged participants (i.e.,
compared with both adult groups (all p < 0.001; all Cohen’s d ≥ 1.45, 12.32% and 8.25%, respectively; p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 1.32, very
resulting in very large effects; Table 1). But, in turn, the middle- large effect). In addition, regardless of age group, higher ␦ ampli-
aged group showed slower RTs than young participants on both tude was reached in the word processing (13.23%) compared with
Phonological (p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.93, large effect) and Seman- the baseline interval (12.31%; Interval main effect: F(1,67) = 175.22,
tic tasks (p < 0.05; Cohen’s d = 0.71, medium effect), whereas they p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.18, small effect). Delta amplitude was
did not differ during the Orthographic task (Table 1). With regard greater on posterior (13.71%) than anterior sites (11.83%; region
to within-group differences, children’s RTs were slower during main effect: F(1,67) = 46.80, p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.38, small effect)
Semantic than either Phonological or Orthographic tasks (p < 0.001; and within the right rather than the left hemisphere (12.90%
Cohen’s d = 1.17 and 1.21, both very large effects), whereas young vs. 12.64%; Laterality main effect: F(1,67) = 4.05, p < 0.05; Cohen’s
adults exhibited slower RTs for the Semantic with respect to the d = 0.04, negligible effect). However, the two-way Age by Laterality
Orthographic task (p < 0.01; Cohen’s d = 1.15, very large effect). interaction (F(2,67) = 11.80, p < 0.001) revealed specific patterns of
ER analysis showed both Age (F(2,67) = 18.19, p < 0.001) and Task ␦ band (Fig. 2, first row). Groups exhibited decreasing ␦ amplitude
(F(2,134) = 40.26, p < 0.001, GG ε = 0.98) main effects. Children made with increasing age, but children and adults also exhibited different
more errors (6.9%) as compared to young adults (2.9%, p < 0.001; patterns of lateralization. Indeed, children and middle-aged partici-
Cohen’s d = 0.92, large effect) and middle-aged participants (3.6%, pants showed bilateral ␦ distribution, whereas higher ␦ levels were
p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.77, large effect). Concerning the main effect found in right vs. left hemisphere of young adults (p < 0.01).
of task, the Semantic processing revealed higher ERs compared
with the other two tasks (all p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 1.19 and 0.61, 3.4. Theta band analysis
very large and medium effect, for Phonological and Semantic tasks,
respectively). In addition, the Orthographic task displayed more ANOVA carried out on the normalized ϑ rhythm showed
errors than did the Phonological one (p < 0.01; Cohen’s d = 0.49, a significant main effect of the between-subjects factor Age
medium effect). The significant two-way Age by Task interac- (F(2,67) = 30.76, p < 0.001): children exhibited greater ϑ ampli-
tion (F(4,134) = 9.16, p < 0.001, GG ε = 0.98) revealed that children tude (11.66%) than both young adult and middle-aged participants
made more errors compared with the two adult groups during (8.29% vs. 6.87%, respectively; p < 0.001; Cohen’s d ≥ 0.58, resulting
262 C. Spironelli, A. Angrilli / Biological Psychology 85 (2010) 258–267

Fig. 2. Spline maps of normalized delta (␦, first row), alpha (␣, second row) and high-beta (␤2, last row) EEG bands, expressed as percentage of spectral contribution with
respect to 0.98–100 Hz range in children, young adults and middle-aged participants for Orthographic (Orth.), Phonological (Phon.) and Semantic (Sem.) tasks.

in medium effects). Like in the ␦ band analysis and independent cortical regions, in line with the already mentioned Age main
of the age group, the ϑ amplitude was greater in posterior (9.94%) effect.
than in anterior sites (7.94%; Region main effect: F(1,67) = 101.13, Concerning laterality patterns, children had bilateral ϑ distribu-
p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.62, medium effect). In addition, the four- tion during the Orthographic task and posterior left-lateralized ϑ
way Age by Task by Region by Laterality interaction (F(4,134) = 3.34, activity during both linguistic tasks (i.e., the Phonological and the
p < 0.01, GG ε = 0.96) revealed specific, task-dependent patterns of Semantic ones; all p < 0.001, Fig. 3b). During all tasks, young adults
ϑ lateralization in each group (Fig. 3). Overall, children showed exhibited significant right-lateralized ϑ amplitude in anterior (with
greater ϑ amplitude than did both adult groups, regardless of the exception of the Semantic one; all p < 0.001, Fig. 3a) and pos-
terior regions (Orthographic and Phonological: p < 0.05, Semantic:
p < 0.001, Fig. 3b). Middle-aged participants revealed larger right
than left ϑ amplitude in anterior sites in both Orthographic and
Semantic tasks (all p < 0.001, Fig. 3a), but bilateral ϑ activity in the
Phonological one as well as over posterior sites in all tasks (Fig. 3b).

3.5. Alpha band analysis

ANOVA carried out on the normalized ␣ band showed signifi-


cant main effects in all within-subjects factors. Thus, independent
of Age, the Semantic task revealed greater ␣ amplitude than the
Orthographic one (10.33% vs. 10.05%, respectively; p < 0.05; Cohen’s
d = 0.07, negligible effect), whereas no differences were found for
the Phonological one (10.24%; Task main effect: F(2,134) = 3.88,
p < 0.05, GG ε = 0.99). The word processing interval displayed lower
␣ amplitude (10.06%) with respect to the baseline (10.35%; Interval
main effect: F(1,67) = 21.63, p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.08, negligible
effect), and the posterior regions had higher ␣ amplitude than the
anterior ones (12.05% vs. 8.36%, respectively; Region main effect:
F(1,67) = 262.78, p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 1.25, very large effect). Sig-
nificant overall greater ␣ amplitude was found in right vs. left
hemisphere (10.35% vs. 10.06%, respectively; Laterality main effect:
F(1,67) = 6.49, p < 0.01; Cohen’s d = 0.07, negligible effect). Similarly
to ϑ band results, the four-way Age by Task by Region by Laterality
interaction (F(4,134) = 3.42, p < 0.01, GG ε = 0.98) revealed specific
task-dependent patterns of ␣ lateralization in each group (Fig. 2,
second row).
Unlike ␦ and ϑ rhythms, ␣ amplitude was higher in children than
in both adult groups only in anterior sites (all p < 0.001) and young
adults, in turn, had greater ␣ amplitude than middle-aged partic-
ipants (all p < 0.001; Fig. 4a). Instead, in posterior locations, young
adults showed greater ␣ levels than children and middle-aged
Fig. 3. Theta (ϑ) EEG band: significant four-way Age by Task by Region by Lateral- participants, particularly during Phonological and Semantic tasks
ity interaction. Mean ϑ amplitude is depicted for children (white squares), young
(all p < 0.001 for left and right hemisphere, respectively; Fig. 4b).
adults (gray triangles) and middle-aged participants (black circles). LH = left hemi-
sphere; RH = right hemisphere. Asterisks indicate significant between hemisphere Children had higher ␣ amplitude than middle-aged participants
differences (Tukey HSD, *p < 0.05). over both posterior sites during the Orthographic task, but they
C. Spironelli, A. Angrilli / Biological Psychology 85 (2010) 258–267 263

Fig. 4. Alpha (␣) EEG band: significant four-way Age by Task by Region by Lateral- Fig. 5. Low-beta (␤1) EEG band: significant four-way Age by Task by Region by Later-
ity interaction. Mean ␣ amplitude is depicted for children (white squares), young ality interaction. Mean ␤1 amplitude is depicted for children (white squares), young
adults (gray triangles) and middle-aged participants (black circles). LH = left hemi- adults (gray triangles) and middle-aged participants (black circles). LH = left hemi-
sphere; RH = right hemisphere. Asterisks indicate significant between hemisphere sphere; RH = right hemisphere. Asterisks indicate significant between hemisphere
differences (Tukey HSD, *p < 0.05). differences (Tukey HSD, *p < 0.05).

showed higher ␣ in left posterior locations during phonological


and semantic processing (all p < 0.001; Fig. 4b). Concerning lat- 10.79%, respectively; Region main effect: F(1,67) = 83.05, p < 0.001;
erality patterns, children had bilateral ␣ distribution during the Cohen’s d = 0.62, medium effect), and significantly greater ␤1
Orthographic task and posterior left-lateralized ␣ amplitude dur- amplitude was found in right vs. left hemisphere (11.65% vs. 11.45%,
ing both linguistic tasks (i.e., the Phonological (p < 0.01) and the respectively; Laterality main effect: F(1,67) = 6.44, p < 0.01; Cohen’s
Semantic ones (p < 0.001), Fig. 4b), a result similar to that of the d = 0.08, negligible effect). Likewise in ϑ and ␣ band analyses, the
ϑ band analysis. Young adults, meanwhile, exhibited significant four-way Age by Task by Region by Laterality interaction was sig-
right-lateralized ␣ amplitude in anterior and posterior regions nificant (F(4,134) = 3.62, p < 0.01, GG ε = 0.96; Fig. 5).
during the Phonological task (all p < 0.01, Fig. 4), and showed ante- Similar to the ␣ band pattern, the ␤1 amplitude was higher
rior right-lateralization during the visuo-perceptual processing in children than in both adult groups only over anterior sites (all
(i.e., the Orthographic task; p < 0.001, Fig. 4a) and posterior right- p < 0.001) but, unlike the ␣ band, middle-aged participants had
lateralization during the semantic processing (p < 0.001, Fig. 4b). greater ␤1 levels with respect to young adults (all p < 0.001, Fig. 5a).
By way of contrast, middle-aged participants revealed anterior ␣ In posterior locations, the middle-aged group showed greater ␤1
right-lateralization (Orthographic and Semantic tasks: p < 0.001, levels as compared to young adult participants (all p < 0.001),
Phonological task: p < 0.01, Fig. 4a) and posterior bilateral distri- which, in turn, exhibited higher ␤1 amplitude than did children
bution in all tasks. (all p < 0.001, with the exception of left locations for the seman-
tic processing, in correspondence with which no group differences
3.6. Low-beta (ˇ1) band analysis were found; Fig. 5b). Concerning the hemispheric lateralization
across tasks, groups revealed a pattern very similar to that found
ANOVA carried out on the normalized ␤1 band showed sig- for the ␣ band analysis. Thus, children had bilateral ␤1 distribu-
nificant effects in all within-subjects factors, revealing patterns tion during the Orthographic task and posterior left-lateralized
of activation similar to the ␣ band analysis, namely greater ␤1 ␤1 amplitude during both linguistic tasks (i.e., the Phonological
amplitude was found during the Semantic (11.66%) and the Phono- (p < 0.01) and the Semantic ones (p < 0.001), Fig. 5b). The young
logical (11.59%) vs. the Orthographic task (11.40%; p < 0.001 and adult sample exhibited significant right vs. left ␤1 amplitude in
p < 0.01, respectively; Task main effect: F(2,134) = 80174, p < 0.001, anterior regions during both Orthographic and Phonological tasks
GG ε = 0.94; Cohen’s d ≥ 0.11, resulting in negligible effects). The (all p < 0.001, Fig. 5a) and in posterior regions during the Seman-
baseline interval revealed higher ␤1 amplitude than the word pro- tic one (p < 0.05, Fig. 5b). Middle-aged participants showed greater
cessing one (11.71% vs. 11.39%, respectively; Interval main effect: right vs. left anterior ␤1 amplitude (p < 0.001 during Orthographic
F(1,67) = 82.17, p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.14, negligible effect). Poste- and Semantic tasks, p < 0.05 during the Phonological one, Fig. 5a)
rior regions had higher ␤1 activity than anterior ones (12.31% vs. and posterior bilateral ␤1 distribution in all tasks (Fig. 5b).
264 C. Spironelli, A. Angrilli / Biological Psychology 85 (2010) 258–267

␤2 distribution (Fig. 6a). In addition, both Phonological and Seman-


tic tasks showed greater left ␤2 amplitude in comparison with
the Orthographic one (baseline: p < 0.05 and p < 0.001, respectively;
word processing interval: all p < 0.001), whereas only the Semantic
task displayed significant greater ␤2 amplitude in right sites (all
p < 0.001; Fig. 6a). Over posterior locations (Fig. 6b) the Semantic
task exhibited greater left ␤2 amplitude in the baseline interval
(p < 0.01), but the other two tasks were characterized by bilateral
␤2 distribution, regardless of intervals. In addition, during the base-
line interval, the ␤2 amplitude was higher in left locations during
the Phonological and the Semantic with respect to the Orthographic
task (p < 0.01 and p < 0.001, respectively), and in both hemispheres
(left: p < 0.01; right: p < 0.001) during the word processing interval.

4. Discussion

The present study aimed at demonstrating that age-related


skilled reading, achieved from continuous interaction with written
word, plays a role in the development of the typical left lateral-
ization of linguistic networks. Based on our previously validated
paradigm (Penolazzi et al., 2008; Spironelli and Angrilli, 2006,
2009b; Spironelli et al., 2006, 2008), this experiment compared
three different age groups in order to investigate the functional lin-
guistic role of low and high frequency bands across age within the
linguistic framework, by using the same experimental paradigm
and tasks.
Overall, children’s longer RTs (compared with adult groups)
suggest that our 10-year-old participants have not completely
reached the full development of their linguistic skills – at least
for the reading process – although their reading speed, measured
Fig. 6. High-beta (␤2) EEG band: significant four-way Task by Interval by Region
using several standardized tests, fell into the range proper for
by Laterality interaction. Mean ␤2 amplitude reached by all groups collapsed is
depicted for Orthographic (gray squares on gray lines), Phonological (white cir- their age (Spironelli et al., 2010). However, children’s performance
cles on thick black lines) and Semantic tasks (white diamonds on thin black lines). shows, on average, low levels of error rates, particularly for the
LH = left hemisphere; RH = right hemisphere. Asterisks indicate significant between Phonological task: in line with this, no group differences were
hemisphere differences (Tukey HSD, *p < 0.05). found. Concerning the Digit Span test, indexing the verbal work-
ing memory, children achieved lower DS scores, probably because
3.7. High-beta (ˇ2) band analysis of their incomplete cognitive and cerebral development, whereas
middle-aged participants’ low span may represent an initial, par-
Overall, ANOVA carried out on the normalized ␤2 band tial process of deterioration of the verbal, short-term memory. This
showed a significant main effect of the between-subjects factor was further supported by their longer RTs to linguistic tasks (i.e.,
Age (F(2,67) = 7.25, p < 0.001): middle-aged participants exhibited the Phonological and Semantic ones) as compared with those of
greater ␤2 amplitude than did children (i.e., 9.61% vs. 8.43%, respec- young adults, whereas no difference between adult groups was
tively; p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.82, large effect), but no differences found for the Orthographic, non-linguistic task. Yet, despite this
were found in the young adult group (8.83%). In addition, regard- decay of important components of language processing, middle-
less of age group, the highest ␤2 amplitude was reached in linguistic aged participants achieved good linguistic performance (i.e., low
(i.e., Phonological, 8.98%, and Semantic, 9.02%, tasks) with respect error rates), when compared with the young adult group, probably
to non-linguistic processing (i.e., the visuo-perceptual, Ortho- because of their long-term linguistic experience which allowed to
graphic task, 8.87%; p < 0.05 and p < 0.01, respectively; Task main develop highly interconnected, automated and functional linguistic
effect: F(2,134) = 5.05, p < 0.01, GG ε = 0.99; Cohen’s d ≥ 0.11, result- networks within the left hemisphere.
ing in negligible effects). Concerning the temporal dimension, the With regard to electrophysiological data, the qualitative analysis
word processing interval revealed lower ␤2 amplitude (8.86%) with of the whole normalized EEG spectrum attested that the typical pat-
respect to the baseline (9.05%; Interval main effect: F(1,67) = 62.48, tern found in children (i.e., increased amplitude of low frequency
p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.13, negligible effect). In addition, unlike all and decreased amplitude of high frequency rhythms) gradually
other EEG bands, ␤2 amplitude was greater on anterior (9.41%) than tends to reverse over age: beta and gamma bands being prevalent in
posterior sites (8.50%; Region main effect: F(1,67) = 65.55, p < 0.001; middle-aged participants. Concerning this point, ANOVA analyses
Cohen’s d = 0.73, medium effect). Unlike the other EEG bands so carried out separately for each EEG band confirmed the significant
far analyzed, no interaction including the group factor was found Age main effect in delta, theta as well as high-beta (␤2) frequency
for the ␤2 band ANOVA. However, the four-way Task by Interval rhythms, low frequency bands being more prevalent in children
by Region by Laterality interaction was significant (F(2,134) = 4.21, than in both adult groups and high frequency rhythms being more
p < 0.05, GG ε = 0.99), revealing a specific task-dependent pattern of dominant in middle-aged participants than in children. One addi-
lateralization, regardless of age group (Fig. 2, last row). tional important result was found in the delta band analysis which
On anterior locations (Fig. 6a), significantly greater ␤2 ampli- revealed, in children, bilateral delta distribution, regardless of task.
tude was found in left vs. right hemisphere in both intervals during In young adults, by contrast, greater delta amplitude over right than
Phonological and Semantic tasks (all p < 0.001). Conversely, the left cortical sites was found, a result indicating a relative inhibi-
non-linguistic task, i.e., the Orthographic one, exhibited bilateral tion of right with respect to left hemisphere. This finding suggests
C. Spironelli, A. Angrilli / Biological Psychology 85 (2010) 258–267 265

not only that anterior regions are not yet completely developed in and Angrilli, 2006). In a recent study in which alpha and theta EEG
children aged 10, but also that, in linguistic networks, there is a power were measured together with fMRI during a working mem-
significant maturation lag (in terms of lateralization) of left frontal ory maintenance task, Scheeringa et al. (2009) found significant
regions. Indeed, past studies carried out in neurological patients correlations between the increase of frontal theta power and the
suffering from tumors or cerebrovascular accidents have stated that decrease of BOLD signals, and between the posterior right alpha
the delta rhythm is a steady marker of neuronal damage (e.g., De increase and the decrease of BOLD signals in the primary visual
Jongh et al., 2001; Tanaka et al., 1998; Vieth et al., 2001), but more cortex and the posterior part of the middle temporal gyrus dur-
recent evidence further clarified this band as a marker of functional ing the maintenance interval. Authors concluded that an increase
deficit of high level cognitive processes in dyslexic, aphasic as well in alpha power represents the cortical marker of top-down func-
as schizophrenia patients (e.g., Hensel et al., 2004; Penolazzi et tional inhibition of those regions not involved in the maintenance of
al., 2008; Spironelli and Angrilli, 2009b; Spironelli et al., in press). the relevant information in the working memory. Thus, in line with
With regard to this approach, the present study investigated the Klimesch et al. (2007) and Scheeringa et al. (2009), an active inhibi-
developmental aspects of delta functional meaning and showed, in tion mechanism acts on right anterior, posterior or both locations of
children, a clear-cut lack of the typical adult linguistic hemispheric language homologues, according to the specific linguistic process-
specialization, a result which demonstrates the long-term difficult ing required by the task. This cognitive skill is equally efficient in
and effortful development of reading. both young adults and middle-aged participants, regardless of the
Analyses of theta and alpha EEG bands allowed us to draw simi- initial deterioration of the verbal working memory in middle-aged
lar conclusions. Indeed, children showed greater theta and alpha participants.
amplitudes compared to adults, particularly over anterior loca- Concerning the statistical analyses of high frequency bands, typ-
tions. However, unlike the delta rhythm, theta and alpha bands ically regarded as markers of cognitive processing, integration and
also revealed task-dependent patterns of lateralization in every age learning (e.g., Jensen et al., 2007; Miltner et al., 1999; Steriade,
group. Thus, children exhibited significantly greater left vs. right 2000; Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999; Weiss and Rappelsberger,
theta and alpha amplitude over posterior sites in those tasks which 1998), we found evidence that only the high-beta (␤2) rhythm,
required linguistic processing (i.e., the Phonological and the Seman- ranging from 21 to 28 Hz, revealed a significant pattern of linguis-
tic one; Figs. 3b and 4b), whereas bilateral distribution of theta and tic activation in agreement with the functional meaning provided
alpha rhythms was shown over anterior locations (Figs. 3a and 4a) by the past literature. Instead, the low-beta (␤1) rhythm, ranging
and for the Orthographic task in all analyzed sites (Figs. 3 and 4). from 13 to 20 Hz, showed a distribution similar to that exhibited
Conversely, adult groups revealed more specific, task-dependent by slower EEG bands, regardless of age group. In our opinion, this
patterns of lateralization. In detail, young adults had significantly result suggests that, in the linguistic domain, the low-beta rhythm
greater right vs. left theta amplitude, in both anterior and posterior represents a transition from slow to fast rhythms, rather than a
sites, during the visuo-perceptual and the phonological process- clear-cut rhythm with a specific functional meaning. From this per-
ing, but in the semantic judgment the same pattern was found spective, similarly to theta and alpha bands, the low-beta band
in posterior locations. The alpha rhythm revealed even greater could characterize, in adults, the active inhibition mechanism over
right hemispheric specialization, with young adults showing larger contralateral regions not involved in the main left linguistic net-
right-lateralization in anterior regions during the visuo-perceptual works (Klimesch et al., 2007). The high-beta (␤2) band showed
processing, in posterior sites during the semantic judgment, and significantly greater activation of left vs. right anterior sites in all
in both anterior and posterior sites during the Phonological task groups (indeed, Fig. 6 shows the results with age group collapsed,
(Fig. 4). Middle-aged participants showed similar patterns of theta since the five-way interaction – including Age-group variable –
band lateralization for Orthographic and Semantic tasks (signifi- was not significant). In detail, this pattern of left lateralization
cantly greater right vs. left amplitude in anterior locations) and marked both Phonological and Semantic tasks – i.e., those which
bilateral distribution for the Phonological one, whereas no task required specific linguistic processing – whereas ␤2 amplitude was
effect was found in the alpha rhythm (greater anterior right vs. bilaterally distributed during the Orthographic task – i.e., when a
left amplitude, regardless of task; Fig. 4). Taken together, these non-linguistic, visuo-perceptual matching was requested (Fig. 2,
results suggest that, in a linguistic context, low frequency bands last row). Thus, in agreement with past studies on language local-
have different functional roles according to age and maturity. In ization (e.g., Bookheimer, 2002; Hagoort, 2005; Poldrack et al.,
agreement with the past literature on language, the functional 1999; Xiang et al., 2010), the high-beta band marks the typical
interpretation of theta and alpha rhythms in children points to a left hemisphere specialization for language in adult samples, but
not completely automated learning mechanism underlying reading the novel finding with respect to the past literature is the discov-
skills (Caplan and Glaholt, 2007), which seems to be related to the ery that this band can also reflect language dominance in children
working memory involvement (Bastiaansen et al., 2002; Klimesch, with incomplete maturation of linguistic reading networks. The
1999; Scheeringa et al., 2009). Indeed, the significant left posterior only difference between phonological and semantic processes was
lateralization of theta as well as alpha bands during Phonological found in the baseline, an interval in which, given the blocked
and Semantic tasks suggests that left posterior regions are selec- presentation, a task-dependent pre-activation may occur in lin-
tively involved in the early processing of linguistic information, guistic networks: the Semantic task showed greater activation of
such as grapheme-phoneme conversion and storage and retrieval left vs. right posterior regions (Fig. 6b). This finding suggests that
of phonological information (Burton et al., 2000). On the other hand, the high-beta rhythm can roughly differentiate the tasks before
the observed greater theta/alpha amplitude in left posterior sites stimulus-driven linguistic processes develop. At the same time, this
could indicate a difficult, not yet complete, integration and hierar- band reflects the integration of the crucial features of each linguistic
chy of left anterior and posterior linguistic centers necessary for the task (i.e., phonological information for rhyming task, and category
full development of the phonological awareness and the achieve- classification for semantic judgment) when the linguistic process
ment of efficient reading skills. In adult groups, theta and alpha takes place. Notwithstanding slow rhythm analyses in children and
rhythms showed task-dependent modulations with greater right adults showed inverted patterns of lateralization (Fig. 2, first and
vs. left hemisphere amplitudes: these patterns suggest that, when second rows), pointing to different levels of linguistic development
the linguistic network is completely developed and interconnected and automation, the lack of age-group lateralization differences for
within the left hemisphere, the functionally less efficient linguistic ␤2 band suggests that this band can reflect the few specialized lin-
regions of the right hemisphere are relatively inhibited (Spironelli guistic networks active in children’s left hemisphere (Fig. 2, last
266 C. Spironelli, A. Angrilli / Biological Psychology 85 (2010) 258–267

row), and therefore represents the best EEG marker for investigat- icant increase of left posterior activation (theta and alpha bands),
ing left hemisphere linguistic dominance, making it applicable to possibly associated with the early processing of linguistic informa-
both children and adult samples. The only study in the past litera- tion, its transfer in working memory (Burton et al., 2000; Klimesch,
ture which has focused on the developmental aspects of language 1999; Scheeringa et al., 2009) and the concurrent activation of spe-
lateralization using the CNV paradigm in a rhyming task (Grossi et cific high level networks in left anterior regions (high-beta band),
al., 2001) found that the linguistic left lateralization of the Contin- involved in the integration of task-dependent linguistic informa-
gent Negative Variation (CNV) started at 10–11 years and increased tion (von Stein and Sarnthein, 2000). The high-beta band was, in
linearly with age until 19–20 years. The CNV wave reflects the acti- the present linguistic paradigm, the only rhythm which showed
vation of a large population of cortical neurons, but is less sensitive the same coherent left hemispheric pattern across all age groups,
to the activity of a more limited amount of neurons embedded in including children not yet fully competent in reading, with greater
linguistic networks. Therefore, the lack of left linguistic lateraliza- amplitude to linguistic tasks (i.e., the Phonological and the Seman-
tion found at the age of 10 by Grossi et al. (2001) for the CNV is tic ones) in left frontal clusters, a result in agreement with current
related to an overall reduced lateralization and expertise in chil- theories on the central role of left Broca area in the organization
dren. From the present study, the beta EEG band appears to be more and integration of the whole language (Bookheimer, 2002; Hagoort,
sensitive to the reduced, yet present, left lateralization in children. 2005).
Thus, to our knowledge, this is the first investigation showing the
capability of an electrophysiological marker to measure a consis-
Acknowledgements
tent, left hemisphere dominance during written word reading tasks
in children aged 10. In addition to this result, it can be noted that
This study was supported by a grant (PRIN 2006110284 001)
the Age main effect showed significantly lower ␤2 amplitude in
from the MIUR (Ministero dell’Istruzione, dell’Università e della
children than in middle-aged participants, a result which suggests
Ricerca) and University of Padova project n. CPDA047438 to A.A.
that linguistic cortical centers in children’s frontal lobes are not yet
complete–an observation in agreement with the well-known delay
in maturation and myelinization of frontal lobes which is complete References
at the age of about 18. This could be the reason why, during Phono-
Adrian, E.D., Matthews, B.H.C., 1934. The interpretation of potential waves in the
logical and Semantic tasks, school readers needed additional time,
cortex. Journal of Physiology 81, 440–471.
probably because of the limited capacity of the verbal working Angrilli, A., Dobel, C., Rockstroh, B., Stegagno, L., Elbert, T., 2000. EEG brain mapping
memory and the relative immaturity of left posterior regions (as of phonological and semantic tasks in Italian and German languages. Clinical
Neurophysiology 111, 706–716.
indexed by theta and alpha activity). Therefore, the high-beta band
Anokhin, A.P., Birbaumer, N., Lutzemberger, W., Nikolaev, A., Vogel, F., 1996. Age
was sensitive to the functional integration of essential informa- increases brain complexity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysi-
tion provided by different systems (and different EEG frequencies) ology 99, 63–68.
when the linguistic cortical areas are still not completely devel- Babiloni, C., Frisoni, G., Steriade, M., Bresciani, L., Binetti, G., Del Percio, C., Geroldi,
C., Miniassi, C., Nobili, F., Rodriguez, G., Zappasodi, F., Carfagna, T., Rossigni, P.M.,
oped. According to von Stein and Sarnthein (2000), in adults this 2006. Frontal white matter volume and delta EEG sources negatively corre-
anterior language center (in the left inferior frontal gyrus) plays late in awake subjects with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.
an important role in unification processes (Hagoort, 2005; Xiang et Clinical Neurophysiology 117, 1113–1129.
Bakker, D.J., 1979. Hemispheric differences and reading strategies: two dyslexia?
al., 2010), is able to organize not only linguistic functions, but also Bulletin of the Orton Society 29, 84–100.
hierarchically structured behaviors (Koechlin and Jubault, 2006), Bakker, D.J., 2006. Treatment of developmental dyslexia: a review. Pediatric Reha-
and acts through an active inhibition process over the right homo- bilitation 9, 3–13.
Bastiaansen, M.C.M., Posthuma, D., Groot, P.F., de Geus, E.J., 2002. Event-related
logues not involved in that specific task (as indexed by slow EEG alpha and theta responses in a visuo-spatial working memory task. Clinical
bands). Neurophysiology 113, 1882–1893.
In conclusion, EEG spectral analysis was effective at measuring Benson, R.R., FitzGerald, D.B., LeSueur, L.L., Kennedy, D.N., Kwong, K.K., Buchbinder,
B.R., Davis, T.L., Weisskoff, R.M., Talavage, T.M., Logan, W.J., Cosgrove, G.R., Bel-
functional and developmental aspects of language. The EEG delta
liveau, J.W., Rosen, B.R., 1999. Language dominance determined by whole brain
band, regardless of language, is an unambiguous index of gener- functional MRI in patients with brain lesions. Neurology 52, 798–809.
alized inhibitory process both in adults and in children: indeed, Bookheimer, S., 2002. Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding
the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annual Review of Neuroscience
this is a very slow rhythm and neurons synchronized in this fre-
25, 151–188.
quency are damaged, disconnected (e.g., Spironelli and Angrilli, Bortolini, V., Tagliavini, C., Zampolli, A., 1972. Lessico di frequenza della lingua ital-
2009; Spironelli et al., in press) or not engaged in specific cognitive iana contemporanea [Lexical frequency in current Italian]. Aldo Garzanti Editore
processes. In children delta EEG is higher than in adults because of SpA, Milano.
Burton, M.W., Small, S.L., Blumstein, S.E., 2000. The role of segmentation in phono-
incomplete cortical maturation, and is typically even larger in chil- logical processing: an fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12,
dren with learning disabilities (e.g., in dyslexic children; Penolazzi 679–690.
et al., 2008). Concerning other slow bands, alpha and theta EEG Caplan, J.B., Glaholt, M.G., 2007. The roles of EEG oscillations in learning relational
information. Neuroimage 38, 604–616.
frequency bands have opposite functional roles in adults and chil- Crow, T.J., 2004. Directional asymmetry is the key to the origin of the modern Homo
dren, at least within the linguistic framework. In adults, in whom sapiens (the Broca-Annett axiom). Laterality 9, 233–242.
language is fully automated, the key role of anterior left regions Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Montavonti, A., Jobert, A., Allirol, L., Dubois, J., Hertz-Pannier,
L., Dehaene, S., 2010. Language or music, mother or Mozart? Structural and envi-
becomes clear-cut in the range of high-beta frequencies. At the ronmental influences on infants’ language networks. Brain and Language 114,
same time, an active process of inhibition of task-irrelevant corti- 53–65.
cal regions within the right hemisphere is shown by slow rhythms, De Jongh, A., de Munck, J., Baayen, J., Jonkman, E., Heethaar, R., van Dijk, B., 2001. The
localization of spontaneous brain activity: first results in patients with cerebral
according to Klimesch et al. (2007). It is important to underline that
tumors. Clinical Neurophysiology 112, 378–385.
the two adult groups showed almost no differences in their patterns Démonet, J., Wise, R., Franckowiak, R., 1994. Language functions explored in normal
of activation, suggesting that there is no effect of moderate aging on subjects by positron emission tomography. Human Brain Mapping 1, 39–47.
Deppe, M., Knecht, M., Papke, K., Lohmann, H., Fleischer, H., Heindel, W., Ringelstein,
the specialization and the functioning of linguistic networks, which
E.-B., Henningsen, H., 2000. Assessment of hemispheric language lateraliza-
are resistant to the observed initial, partial process of deterioration tion: a comparison between fMRI and fTCD. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow
of the verbal working memory. Conversely, in children, in whom Metabolism 20, 263–268.
linguistic automation is not yet complete, slow and fast rhythm Fiez, J.A., Petersen, S.E., 1998. Neuroimaging studies of word reading. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Science United States of America 5, 914–921.
patterns suggest that different mechanisms are critically involved Friederici, A.D., Friedrich, M., Christophe, A., 2007. Brain responses in 4-month-old
in both phonological and semantic processing—i.e., there is a signif- infants are already language specific. Current Biology 17, 1208–1211.
C. Spironelli, A. Angrilli / Biological Psychology 85 (2010) 258–267 267

Frith, C.D., Friston, K.J., Liddle, P.F., Frackowiak, R.S., 1991. A PET study of word Rutten, G.J., Ramsey, N.F., van Rijen, P.C., Alpherts, W.C., van Veelen, C.W., 2002.
finding. Neuropsychologia 29, 1137–1148. fMRI-determined language lateralization in patients with unilateral or mixed
Geschwind, N., Galaburda, A.M., 1985. Cerebral lateralization, Biological mecha- language dominance according to the Wada test. Neuroimage 17, 447–460.
nisms, associations, and pathology. II. A hypothesis and a program for research. Scheeringa, R., Petersson, K.M., Oostenveld, R., Norris, D.G., Hagoort, P., Bastiaansen,
Archives of Neurology 42, 521–552. M.C.M., 2009. Trial-by-trial coupling between EEG and BOLD identifies networks
Grossi, G., Coch, D., Coffey-Corina, S., Holcomb, P.J., Neville, H.J., 2001. Phonologi- related to alpha and theta power increases during working memory mainte-
cal processing in visual rhyming: a developmental study. Journal of Cognitive nance. Neuroimage 44, 1224–1238.
Neuroscience 13, 610–625. Spironelli, C., Angrilli, A., 2006. Language lateralization in phonological, semantic
Hagoort, P., 2005. On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework. Trends in Cogni- and orthographic tasks: a slow evoked potential study. Behavioral and Brain
tive Sciences 9, 416–423. Research 175, 296–304.
Harmony, T., Marosi, E., Díaz de León, A.E., Becker, J., Fernández, T., 1990. Effect of Spironelli, C., Angrilli, A., 2007. Influence of phonological, semantic and orthographic
sex, psychosocial disadvantages and biological risk factors on EEG maturation. tasks on the early linguistic components N150 and N350. International Journal
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 75, 482–491. of Psychophysiology 64, 190–198.
Hensel, S., Rockstroh, B., Berg, P., Elbert, T., Schönle, W., 2004. Left-hemispheric Spironelli, C., Angrilli, A., 2009a. Developmental aspects of automatic word process-
abnormal EEG activity in relation to impairment and recovery in aphasic ing: language lateralization of early ERP components in children, young adults
patients. Psychophysiology 41, 394–400. and middle-aged subjects. Biological Psychology 80, 35–45.
Hiscock, M., 1998. Brain lateralization across the life span. In: Stemmer, B., Whitaker, Spironelli, C., Angrilli, A., 2009b. EEG delta band as a marker of brain dam-
H.A. (Eds.), Handbook of Neurolinguistics. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. age in aphasic patients after recovery of language. Neuropsychologia 47,
357–368. 988–994.
Ille, N., Berg, P., Scherg, M., 2002. Artifact correction of the ongoing EEG using spa- Spironelli, C., Penolazzi, B., Angrilli, A., 2008. Dysfunctional hemispheric asymmetry
tial filters based on artifact and brain signal topographies. Journal of Clinical of theta and beta EEG activity during linguistic tasks in developmental dyslexia.
Neurophysiology 19, 113–124. Biological Psychology 77, 123–131.
Jensen, O., Kaiser, J., Lachaux, J.-P., 2007. Human gamma-frequency oscillations asso- Spironelli, C., Penolazzi, B., Vio, C., Angrilli, A., 2006. Inverted EEG theta lateraliza-
ciated with attention and memory. Trends in Neurosciences 30, 317–324. tion in dyslexic children during phonological processing. Neuropsychologia 44,
Klimesch, W., 1999. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory 2814–2821.
performance: a review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews 29, 169–195. Spironelli, C., Penolazzi, B., Angrilli, A., 2010. Gender differences in reading in
Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., Hanslmayr, S., 2007. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition- school-aged children: an early ERP study. Developmental Neuropsychology 35,
timing hypothesis. Brain Research Reviews 53, 63–88. 357–375.
Koechlin, E., Jubault, T., 2006. Broca’s area and the hierarchical organization of Spironelli, C., Angrilli, A., Calogero, A., Stegagno, L., in press. Delta EEG band as a
human behavior. Neuron 50, 963–974. marker of left hypofrontality in schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin.
Kolev, V., Yordanova, J., Basar-Eroglu, C., Basar, E., 2002. Age effects on visual EEG doi:10.1093/schbul/sbp145.
responses reveal distinct frontal alpha networks. Clinical Neurophysiology 113, Steriade, M., 2000. Brain electrical activity and sensory processing during waking
901–910. and sleeping. In: Kryger, M.H., Roth, T., Dement, W.C. (Eds.), Sleep Medicine.
Liotti, M., Gay, C.T., Fox, P.T., 1994. Functional imaging and language: evidence Saunders Company, Philadelphia, pp. 93–111.
from positron emission tomography. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 11, Stiles, J., Bates, E.A., Thal, D., Trauner, D., Reilly, J., 2002. Linguistic, cognitive and
175–190. affective development in children with pre- and perinatal focal brain injury: a
Luria, A.R., 1973. The Working Brain. Basic Books, New York. ten-year overview from the San Diego longitudinal project. In: Johnson, M.H.,
Miltner, W.H.R., Braun, C., Arnold, M., Witte, H., Taub, E., 1999. Coherence of gamma- Munakata, Y., Gilmore, R.O. (Eds.), Brain Development and Cognition: A Reader,
band EEG activity as a basis for associative learning. Nature 397, 434–436. 2nd edition. Blackwell, London, pp. 272–291.
Mondini, S., Angrilli, A., Bisiacchi, P., Spironelli, C., Marinelli, K., Semenza, C., 2008. Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., 1999. Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its
Mass and count nouns activate different brain regions: an ERP study on early role in object representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3, 151–162.
components. Neuroscience Letters 430, 48–53. Tanaka, A., Kimura, M., Yoshinaga, S., Tomonaga, M., Mizoguchi, T., 1998. Quantita-
Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh tive electroencephalographic correlates of cerebral blood flow in patients with
Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. chronic subdural hematomas. Surgical Neurology 50, 235–240.
Oostenveld, R., Praamstra, P., 2001. The five percent electrode system for high- Vieth, J., Kober, H., Ganslandt, O., Möller, M., Kamada, K., 2001. The clinical use of
resolution EEG and ERP measurements. Clinical Neurophysiology 112, 713–719. MEG activity associated with brain lesions. In: Nenonen, J., Ilmoniemi, R., Katila,
Palva, S., Palva, M., 2007. New vistas for ␣-frequency band oscillations. Trends in T. (Eds.), Biomag 2000. Helsinki University of Technology, Espoo, pp. 387–394.
Neurosciences 30, 150–158. von Stein, A., Sarnthein, J., 2000. Different frequencies for different scales of corti-
Pfurtscheller, G., Stancár, A., Neuper, C., 1996. Event-related synchronization (ERS) cal integration: from local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization.
in the alpha band—an electrophysiological correlate of cortical idling: a review. International Journal of Psychophysiology 38, 301–313.
International Journal of Psychophysiology 24, 39–46. Weiss, S., Rappelsberger, P., 1998. Left frontal EEG coherence reflects modality inde-
Penolazzi, B., Spironelli, C., Angrilli, A., 2008. Delta EEG activity as a marker of dys- pendent language processes. Brain Topography 11, 33–42.
functional linguistic processing in developmental dyslexia. Psychophysiology Wienbruch, C., Moratti, S., Elbert, T., Vogel, U., Fehr, T., Kissler, J., Schiller, A.,
45, 1025–1033. Rockstroh, B., 2003. Source distribution of neuromagnetic slow wave activ-
Petersen, S.E., Fox, P.T., Posner, M.I., Mintun, M., Raichle, M.E., 1988. Positron emis- ity in schizophrenic and depressive patients. Clinical Neurophysiology 114,
sion tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing. 2052–2060.
Nature 331, 585–589. Xiang, H.-D., Fonteijn, H.M., Norris, D.G., Hagoort, P., 2010. Topographical functional
Poldrack, R.A., Wagner, A.D., Prull, M.W., Desmond, J.E., Glover, G.H., Gabrieli, J.D.E., connectivity pattern in the Perisylvian language networks. Cerebral Cortex 20,
1999. Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the 549–560.
left inferior prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 10, 15–35. Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., 1997. Developmental changes in the event-related EEG theta
Pulvermüller, F., 2001. Brain reflections of words and their meaning. Trends in Cog- response and P300. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 104,
nitive Sciences 5, 517–524. 418–430.
Ray, W.J., Cole, H.W., 1985. EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta Zatorre, R.J., Evans, A.C., Meyer, E., Gjedde, A., 1992. Lateralization of phonetic and
activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. Science 228, 750–752. pitch discrimination in speech processing. Science 256, 846–849.

You might also like