You are on page 1of 9

Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 50–58

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Facebook self-disclosure: Examining the role of traits, social cohesion,


and motives
Erin E. Hollenbaugh a,⇑, Amber L. Ferris b
a
School of Communication Studies, Kent State University at Stark, USA
b
Department of Speech and Theatre, The University of Akron Wayne College, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Facebook has been shown to be the most popular social network in the United States. Facebook not only
Available online 23 August 2013 has implications in the online world, but face-to-face connections are also affected by this medium. This
study explores the uses of Facebook for self-disclosure behavior utilizing the uses and gratifications per-
Keywords: spective. Using a convenience sample of Facebook users, this study examines individual and sociological
Facebook factors as well as Facebook motives to discover the impact on depth, breadth, and amount of user self-
Self-disclosure disclosure. Path analyses showed that the Big Five personality factors, self-esteem, social cohesion, and
Uses and gratifications
motives contribute to self-disclosure dimensions. However, demographic variables did not impact disclo-
Big five
Motives
siveness. Limitations are discussed and directions for future research are proposed.
Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction friends, and nearly half of the user’s social network is a part of their
Facebook world (Hampton et al., 2011). This means that many peo-
Since its founding in February, 2004, Facebook has become one ple in networks offline are also present in Facebook, including
of the leading social networking websites (Facebook.com, 2013a). friends, neighbors, classmates, coworkers, and family. Therefore,
Facebook.com (2013b) reports that at the time of this writing, the real-world ramifications of Facebook disclosures are important.
the site has more than one billion total active users, with over Although Facebook can bring us together and give us a sense of
600 million users accessing the site daily all over the world. In belonging, there may also be harmful effects of self-disclosure.
the United States, Facebook is a staple for social networking. In a The purpose of this study is to determine the predictors of self-
survey of over 2000 American adults, Hampton, Goulet, Rainie, disclosive behavior on Facebook. Doing so would not only reveal a
and Purcell (2011) found that 92% of all social network users are clearer picture of the social environment of Facebook, but would
on Facebook, and slightly more than half of those users access also allow scholars to predict the population most at-risk for highly
Facebook on a daily basis. The results showed that more than just disclosive and potentially damaging Facebook self-disclosure.
teenagers are using this medium. Hampton et al. (2011) found that Additionally, understanding predictors of self-disclosive behavior
the average Facebook users are 38 years old, female (58%), have at may reveal those users who are most likely to benefit from increas-
least some education beyond high school (69%), and are primarily ingly intimate relationships online. Toward that end, a review of
white (78%). With so many people accessing and using this tech- uses and gratifications theory will be provided as it applies to dis-
nology each day, it is important to examine how the medium is closiveness on Facebook, followed by a study testing the predictors
used and the possible implications of this use. of disclosive behavior on Facebook.
One reason Facebook is important to examine is the high
amounts of self-disclosure that are often displayed within this
1.1. Self-disclosure on Facebook
medium. Self-disclosure, the revelation of personal information
(Derlega, Metts, Petronio, & Margulis, 1993), can have immediate
According to research by the Pew Internet and American Life
implications due to the fact that users are linked into both virtual
Project (Smith, 2011), American adults say they are drawn to social
and face-to-face friends. The average Facebook user has 229
media sites such as Facebook to maintain contact with friends and
family, as well as to re-establish connections with old friends. It is
Abbreviations: U&G, Uses and gratifications theory; BFI, Big Five Inventory;
not surprising, then, that Facebook is characterized by high
RSDS, Revised Self-disclosure Scale.
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Kent State University at Stark, 6000 Frank Ave. amounts of self-disclosure because it is well-established that
NW, North Canton, OH 44720, USA. Tel.: +1 330 244 3458; fax: +1 330 494 6121. self-disclosure is a crucial element in relationship development
E-mail address: ehollen2@kent.edu (E.E. Hollenbaugh). (e.g., Altman & Taylor, 1973). Facebook’s mission statement is ‘‘to

0747-5632/$ - see front matter Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.07.055
E.E. Hollenbaugh, A.L. Ferris / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 50–58 51

make the world more open and connected’’ (Facebook.com, 2013b, ness, and companionship (Sheldon, 2008a, 2008b; Special & Li-Bar-
para. 1), which rests on its users’ willingness to present their inner ber, 2012). Other researchers have found additional motives
thoughts and emotional states on Facebook. The characteristics of including control (i.e., telling someone to do something; Baek, Hol-
Facebook encourage such self-disclosure (Mazer, Murphy, & Si- ton, Harp, & Yaschur, 2011), promoting work/professional
monds, 2007). Facebook’s status update box asks ‘‘what’s on your advancement (Baek et al., 2011; Papacharissi & Mendelson,
mind,’’ prompting users to disclose information. The social net- 2011), photo-related activities (Raacke & Bonds-Raacke, 2008; To-
working site has a user-friendly platform that allows for easy ac- sun, 2012), and learning/academic purposes (Hew, 2011; Raacke &
cess to picture posting, status updates, and other web content to Bonds-Raacke, 2008).
be shared with one’s network in both web and mobile formats. Motives for using Facebook are linked to one’s real-world com-
While Facebook functions as an outlet for disclosure, aspects of munication experiences, as the research reviewed above has
the internet as a medium encourage increasing amounts of per- shown. Research has also shown that motives are linked to self-
sonal self-disclosure. For example, social information processing disclosure. In a study on Facebook and self-disclosure, Special
theory explains that users can adapt their verbal cues to accommo- and Li-Barber (2012) found that those who were motivated to
date the largely text-based environment of computer-mediated use the medium for entertainment tended to disclose more infor-
communication (Walther, 1992). Additionally, the increased mation on the site. Additionally, those that were highly disclosive
amount, depth, and breadth of self-disclosure is one way that Face- were more likely to use Facebook to pass the time (Special & Li-
book users can express affinity to compensate for the largely tex- Barber, 2012). Tosun (2012) found that those people who felt they
tual nature of the online environment (Walther, 1992). could disclose their ‘‘true self’’ online were more likely to use Face-
Schumaker and Van Der Heide (2011) argued that self-disclosure book to establish new relationships and manage romantic relation-
is one vehicle that Facebook users may employ to express emo- ships. Additionally, research conducted by Smock et al. (2011)
tions when richer nonverbal communication channels are not showed that people who use Facebook for expressive information
present. Along with the lack of nonverbal cues, the asynchronous sharing motivations were more likely to post status updates.
nature of most Facebook activity can cause a person to self-disclose Existing research has shown that individual variables should be
intimate information (Suler, 2004; Walther, 1996). taken into consideration when examining the use of new media
Not only does the internet as a medium lend itself to self-disclo- (e.g., Hills & Argyle, 2003; Miura & Yamashita, 2007). Predictor
sure, but interpersonal aspects of disclosure are relevant to Face- variables such as personality traits, sociological variables, and
book. There are several dimensions of self-disclosure, such as demographics have an impact on the dynamics of self-disclosure
amount, depth, breadth, intent, valence, and honesty/accuracy on Facebook. These variables often play a role in the motives peo-
(Altman & Taylor, 1973; Wheeless, 1978). General disclosiveness ple have for using Facebook as well as their self-disclosive behav-
is represented by amount, depth, and breadth of self-disclosure iors online.
in the present study. Amount of self-disclosure is conceptualized
as the number of disclosures made on Facebook. Depth is charac- 1.3. Predictors of Facebook use
terized by more personal or intimate disclosures. People disclose
with more breadth when they discuss a wide variety of topics. 1.3.1. Individual variables
Two main individual factors are examined in this research: per-
1.2. Uses and gratifications theory sonality traits and self-esteem. Personality factors have often been
used as variables in U&G research (Ruggiero, 2000). The ‘‘Big Five’’
Uses and gratifications theory (U&G) has proven to be a useful personality traits are neuroticism, agreeableness, openness, consci-
and popular theory to frame the study of computer-mediated com- entiousness, and extraversion (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991). In a
munication, including Facebook use. U&G proposes that media study on Facebook use and personality traits, Ross et al. (2009)
uses and effects are best understood within the context of the indi- found that personality traits were linked to several functions and
vidual’s psychological and sociological characteristics, as well as motivations. For example, participants who were highly extro-
his/her motives for using a medium (Katz, Blumler, & Gurevitch, verted were more likely to join Facebook groups. With regards to
1974). People approach media with goals in mind that they wish openness, those that were highly open were more likely to indicate
to fulfill, making this a functional perspective (Rubin, 2002). This a need to be sociable on Facebook. People low in neuroticism were
limited effects model requires researchers to consider each of these more likely to use photos on Facebook, whereas people highly neu-
variables when examining the whole picture of media effects. Ex- rotic enjoyed the Wall function (Ross et al., 2009). Additionally, in a
tant Facebook research using U&G has largely explored the motives study of Australian Facebook users, Ryan and Xenos (2011) found
for Facebook use and significant predictors of Facebook use. that Facebook users are more likely to be extraverted and narcissis-
tic. People who scored high on exhibitionism were more likely to
1.2.1. Motives for Facebook use prefer photos and status updates. As found in Ross et al. (2009),
Modern U&G studies often focus on understanding the motives, people who were more neurotic preferred using the wall function
or reasons people use media, which in turn helps predict the grat- on Facebook.
ifications they will gain from media use (Rubin, 2002; Ruggiero, In addition to personality characteristics, self-esteem, or the be-
2000). Researchers have been very interested in discovering the lief that one has self-worth (Crocker & Park, 2004), is another indi-
motives for Facebook use, most often under the U&G framework vidual variable that should be considered when examining online
(e.g., Hunt, Atkin, & Krishnan, 2012; Joinson, 2008; Raacke & self-disclosure. The social compensation hypothesis posits that
Bonds-Raacke, 2008; Sheldon, 2008a, 2008b; Smock, Ellison, people use media to fulfill social needs that are unmet in everyday
Lampe, & Wohn, 2011; Tosun, 2012; Yang & Brown, 2013). This life (Davis & Kraus, 1989). Specifically, the internet may be a forum
body of research focuses on developing motive typologies as well for those who are inhibited in everyday life to branch out and form
as the effects of these motives on Facebook use. social relationships (McKenna, Green, & Gleason, 2002). According
Many researchers have developed motives typologies for social to this perspective, people with low self-esteem who may other-
network use. Often combined with interpersonal motives, social wise be withdrawn in social situations have the opportunity to
networking research has shown several major motives for use. flourish on Facebook.
These include relationship maintenance, to pass the time, virtual There is some Facebook research that supports the social com-
community (i.e., develop new relationships), entertainment, cool- pensation hypothesis. Facebook users with lower self-esteem tend
52 E.E. Hollenbaugh, A.L. Ferris / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 50–58

to have more Facebook friends (Lee, Moore, Park, & Park, 2012), Raacke and Bonds-Raacke (2008) found that there were no gender
spend more time on Facebook, and have a stronger emotional con- differences in reported motivations for Facebook use between men
nection to Facebook (Kalpidou, Costin, & Morris, 2011). Undergrad- and women. Additionally, Baek et al. (2011) found no gender dif-
uate students in Forest and Wood’s (2012) studies who were lower ferences in posting links and motivations on Facebook.
in self-esteem saw self-disclosure on Facebook as appealing, but Finally, age appears to be a potentially important predictor to
their disclosures were generally more negative than positive. include in this study. Because Facebook users’ ages range widely
and the average Facebook user is 38 years old (Hampton et al.,
1.3.2. Social cohesion 2011), age may play an important part in the self-disclosive behav-
Although the research above partially support the social com- iors of Facebook users. Existing research suggests that younger
pensatory features of Facebook, the social enhancement hypothesis people tend to disclose more than older people in face-to-face
argues that people behave online in similar ways as they do face- (e.g., Knapp, Ellis, & Williams, 1980; Sinha, 1972) and computer-
to-face (Valkenburg, Schouten, & Peter, 2005). In other words, mediated communication (e.g., Ma & Leung, 2006). Much of the
those who are sociable and have strong connections offline are existing Facebook research samples from undergraduate students;
likely to benefit from this sociability and maintain highly social thus, the participants do not vary much in age. However, age differ-
Facebook pages, rife with self-disclosure and connection. There- ences may exist in the broader Facebook population.
fore, it may be useful to measure sociological indices when explor-
ing how users self-disclose on Facebook. 1.4. Research questions
One sociological variable that may be of particular interest with
respect to the social enhancement hypothesis is social cohesion. U&G is a useful model for examining the predictors of self-dis-
Social cohesion is the connection one feels to a larger group, estab- closive behavior on Facebook. Fig. 1 presents the model tested in
lished through social bonds (Yamamoto, 2011). Members of so- this study. It is expected that individual and sociological variables
cially cohesive groups feel a sense of engagement through their will predict motives for using Facebook, which in turn will predict
groups (Rosell & et al., 1995). Facebook disclosiveness. In this study, disclosiveness is considered
Papacharissi and Mendelson’s (2011) research supports the a particular type of media use, which is fitting for U&G. To explore
assumption that those with stronger real-life ties utilize Facebook the relationships among study variables, the following research
to strengthen these ties. Results showed that participants who questions were posed:
used Facebook for expressive information sharing, habitual/pass
the time, relaxation/entertainment, coolness, companionship, so- RQ1: What individual variables (age, sex, Big Five traits, self-
cial interaction, and professional advancement were most likely esteem), sociological variables (social cohesion), and Facebook
to score high on social capital measures (staying connected, meet- motives characterize large amounts of self-disclosure on
ing new people, etc.). In other words, those who were socially ac- Facebook?
tive in their real life were more likely to benefit from Facebook’s RQ2: What individual variables (age, sex, Big Five traits, self-
ability to connect with others (Papacharissi & Mendelson, 2011). esteem), sociological variables (social cohesion), and Facebook
According to Hampton et al. (2011), Facebook users have more motives characterize more depth of self-disclosure on
confidants and closer ties to offline network than others, again in Facebook?
support of the social enhancement hypothesis. Examining both RQ3: What individual variables (age, sex, Big Five traits, self-
self-esteem and social cohesion in the present study will provide esteem), sociological variables (social cohesion), and Facebook
an opportunity to test the social compensation and social enhance- motives characterize more breadth of self-disclosure on
ment hypotheses. Facebook?

1.3.3. Demographic factors 2. Methods


In addition to individual and sociological characteristics, demo-
graphics can impact self-disclosure on Facebook. Sex is the most A cross-sectional survey design was used to test the study’s re-
commonly examined individual difference with regard to self-dis- search questions and hypotheses. The participants and procedures
closure (Dindia, 2002). Generally speaking, research has shown are described in the following sections.
that women tend to disclose more personal information in face-
to-face contexts than men (e.g., Buhrke & Fuqua, 1987; Davidson 2.1. Participants
& Duberman, 1982; Jourard, 1971; Morton, 1978). This trend has
been supported in online communication, as well (e.g., Peter, Valk- Participants were adults over the age of 18 who posted on Face-
enburg, & Schouten, 2005; Punyanunt-Carter, 2006). book at least once per month. Following approval from the Institu-
When looking at Facebook specifically, there have been some tional Review Board, the researchers attempted to collect a random
studies that show sex differences in Facebook use and motives. sample by utilizing a random number generator and the ‘‘People’’
Sheldon (2008b) found that female students used Facebook to pass directory provided by Facebook. However, the recruitment mes-
the time, for entertainment, and for maintaining existing relation- sage was quickly reported as SPAM, which blocked the researchers
ships. Males in this study, however, tended to use Facebook more from further data collection in this way. Thereafter, the researchers
to develop new relationships and to develop romantic relation- ultimately used a convenience sampling method, posting a call for
ships. Hunt et al. (2012) also found that women used Facebook participation in numerous venues, including listservs, Facebook
more for entertainment and interpersonal communication mo- groups, the researcher’s home institutions’ faculty listservs to cir-
tives. With regards to self-disclosure on Facebook, Special and Li- culate among students, and the researchers’ Facebook pages. Every
Barber (2012) found that males disclosed more basic information attempt was made to collect data from Facebook users that were
and contact information than females. not personally connected to the researchers via a snowball tech-
Other studies have shown that men and women do not differ in nique (asking friends to post the link on their wall), but this could
why they use Facebook. When examining motives for making a not be guaranteed.
relationship ‘‘Facebook official,’’ Fox and Warber (2013) found that Data was collected from 305 Facebook users; however, four
there were no differences in motivations between men and women cases were excluded because they reported no Facebook usage.
(interpersonal or social) in making a relationship status disclosure. The final data set include 301 Facebook users. Participants’ ages
E.E. Hollenbaugh, A.L. Ferris / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 50–58 53

Individual
Variables
Age
Sex
Self-esteem
Extraversion
Conscientiousness
Neuroticism
Agreeableness
Openness
Facebook Motives
Virtual Community Self-Disclosure
Companionship Depth
Exhibitionism Breadth
Relationship Maintenance Amount
Passing Time

Sociological
Variable
Social Cohesion

Fig. 1. Proposed model of self-disclosure in Facebook.

ranged from 18 to 68 years (M = 31.85, SD = 12.92). The sample 2.3.1. Self-esteem


was predominantly women (n = 232, 77.1%; males n = 68, 22.6%) Trait self-esteem was measured with Rosenberg’s (1979) 10-
who were Caucasian (n = 276, 91.7%). Participants logged onto item self-esteem scale. This 5-point, Likert-type scale measures
Facebook an average of 8.24 times per day (SD = 22.31), using Face- trait self-esteem. Five items were reverse-coded, and then scores
book a total of 109.67 min per day (SD = 125.10). On average, they were summed and averaged to reveal each participant’s scores.
had 434.98 Facebook friends (SD = 758.16). Higher scores indicate higher self-esteem.

2.2. Procedures 2.3.2. ‘‘Big Five’’ personality traits


John et al.’s (1991) Big Five Inventory (BFI) was used to test
After providing informed consent, participants completed a bat- extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and
tery of surveys designed in Qualtrics. Data for this study was a sub- openness. The BFI has undergone extensive testing, resulting in a
set of this larger data set. Personality characteristics were relatively short, reliable, and valid instrument (see John, Naumann,
measured first, followed by motives for using Facebook, self-disclo- & Soto, 2008, for a thorough review of the BFI). This 5-point, Likert-
sure on Facebook, and demographic and descriptive information. type scale asks participants to indicate how much they agree that
they are like the 44 phrases in the scale. Each dimension is mea-
2.3. Measures sured by 8–10 items. When averaged, higher scored indicate more
of the measured personality trait.
All measures in this study were found to be reliable. Table 1 in-
cludes means, standard deviations, and Cronbach’s alphas for all 2.3.3. Social cohesion
study variables. Each measure is described in detail below. Social cohesion was measured by Malone, Pillow, and Osman
(2012) 10-item General Belongingness Scale. All items were mea-
Table 1
sured on a 5-point, Likert-type scale, with 5 indicating high social
Means, standard deviations, and Cronbach’s alphas for all study variables. cohesion. Five items were reverse-coded and all items were aver-
aged to form a composite measure of cohesion.
M SD a
Age (in years) 31.85 12.92
Self-esteem 3.94 .68 .88 2.3.4. Facebook motives
Extraversion 3.43 .80 .89 There have been several inventories of Facebook motives for-
Conscientiousness 3.81 .55 .79 warded by researchers. Sheldon’s (2008b) Facebook motives index
Neuroticism 2.93 .69 .83 appeared to be heavily used in existing research. Therefore, it was
Agreeableness 3.79 .57 .80
adopted in this study. This 26-item measure asks participants to
Openness 3.76 .59 .84
Social cohesion 3.89 .73 .94 indicate on a 5-point scale how likely they are to use Facebook
for each of six motives – relationship maintenance, passing time,
Facebook motives
Virtual community 2.02 .92 .89 virtual community, entertainment, coolness, and companionship.
Companionship 1.93 1.07 .94 To ensure that we measured all relevant motives, we included 10
Exhibitionism 2.01 1.00 .90 items from Barker and Ota’s (2011) research in the virtual commu-
Relationship maintenance 4.18 .72 .87
nity and companionship motives, as well as three items from Hol-
Passing time 4.05 .92 r = .66
lenbaugh’s (2011) blogging motives index measuring
Self-disclosure dimensions
exhibitionism. The full index included 39 items.
Amount 2.34 .64 .71
Depth 1.92 .73 .79
Responses on the full index were subjected to a two-stage
Breadth 3.37 .79 .76 exploratory factor analysis with Varimax rotation to reveal latent
factors. Analyses revealed a five-factor solution, including 24 items
Note: Age was measured with one item each. Therefore, reliability estimates could
not be computed. All other variables were measured on a 1–5 scale, with 5 repre-
and accounting for 71.60% of the variance. See Table 2 for items,
senting more of the measured variable. factor loadings, and variance explained by each motive. Scores
Note: Pass time Facebook motive was measured with two items, yielding a signif- were computed by summing and averaging each participant’s re-
icant, large correlation (r = .66, p < .001). sponses on the items loaded on each factor.
54 E.E. Hollenbaugh, A.L. Ferris / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 50–58

Table 2
Factor loadings for final 5-factor solution of Facebook motives.

1 2 3 4 5
Factor 1: Virtual community
To meet new people like me .87
To meet new friends .87
To meet people like me .84
To find more interesting people than in real life .67
To develop a romantic relationship .67
To find companionship .65
To see people with a similar background .62
Factor 2: Companionship
To feel less lonely .86
Because it makes me feel less lonely .85
So I won’t be alone .84
Because there’s no one to talk to .81
Because I have no one to talk or be with .79
Factor 3: Exhibitionism
For attention .84
To get attention .84
Because my posts make me cool among my peers .76
To gain fame or notoriety .74
Because I like when people read things about me .66
Factor 4: Relationship maintenance
To communicate with my friends .84
To stay in touch with friends .82
To get in touch with people I know .78
To post a message on my friend’s wall .76
To send a message to a friend .74
1 2 3 4 5
Factor 5: Passing time
To occupy my time .85
To pass time when bored .85
Variance explained (%) 18.13 17.45 14.68 14.63 6.71

Note: Factor loadings and variance reported from final, rotated factor solution.

Factor 1, virtual community, included seven items from the cor- 2.3.6. Demographics and descriptive information
responding a priori motive that measure people’s need to use Face- Participants reported age, sex, ethnicity, and amount of time
book to forge new relationships. Factor 2, companionship, consisted spent on Facebook on an average day to describe the sample. The
of five items from the a priori category. Participants who scored researchers also asked participants to log onto their Facebook ac-
high on this motive used Facebook to compensate for loneliness. count to look up and provide their number of Facebook friends.
Exhibitionism was Factor 3, which measures participants’ desire Age and sex were used to address the study’s research questions.
to use Facebook to get attention. This factor included five items
from the a priori motives of coolness and exhibitionism. The fourth 3. Results
factor, relationship maintenance, included five items from the same
a priori category. This motive represents using Facebook to sustain Path analyses were conducted with regression to test the re-
existing relationships. Factor 5, passing time, consisted of two items search questions. U&G was consulted to determine how the inde-
that were highly correlated with one another. These items repre- pendent variables would be entered into the models (see Fig. 1).
sent a well-established motive of media use – to relieve boredom. To conduct the path analysis, five separate regressions were run
to determine the predictors of Facebook motives (see Table 3). In
2.3.5. Self-disclosure dimensions these analyses, Facebook motives were regressed on individual
The researchers used a modified version of Wheeless’ (1978) and sociological variables (age, sex, self-esteem, Big Five traits,
Revised Self-disclosure Scale (RSDS) to measure amount and depth and social cohesion. Results are summarized in Table 3 and below,
of self-disclosure. This scale asks participants to indicate how as they serve as indirect predictors of certain disclosive behaviors.
much they agree on a scale from 1 (‘‘strongly disagree’’) to 5 Next, hierarchical regressions were run on each of the three self-
(‘‘strongly agree’’) with each statement about self-disclosive disclosure dimensions. Individual and sociological variables were
behavior. Amount is measured by seven items, and depth is mea- entered as the first block of predictors, followed by Facebook mo-
sured by five. The researchers revised the scale to fit within the tives as the second block (see Table 4). This two-stage method of
Facebook context. Responses were averaged for each of the sub- regression analysis allows the researchers to examine direct and
scales to reveal scores for amount and depth of self-disclosure. indirect predictors of the outcome variables. See Fig. 2 for signifi-
A close examination of the RSDS in relation to foundational re- cant study results.
search in self-disclosure (e.g., Altman & Taylor, 1973) revealed that
breadth, a central dimension of self-disclosure in much disclosure 3.1. Amount of self-disclosure
research and theory, was not measured in this scale. Therefore, five
items were created to measure breadth of self-disclosure, including RQ1 was posed to explore the characteristics of Facebook users
items such as ‘‘My Facebook posts range over a wide variety of top- who disclose large amounts of personal information. The total
ics’’ and ‘‘My Facebook posts tend to center around one subject of model explained 17% of the variance in amount of self-disclosure,
interest’’ (reverse coded). F(14, 285) = 4.05, p < .001. Two motives were direct predictors of
E.E. Hollenbaugh, A.L. Ferris / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 50–58 55

Table 3
Regressing Facebook motives on individual and sociological variables.

Virtual community Companion-ship Exhibitionism Relationship maintenance Passing time


Predictors Final b Final b Final b Final b Final b
Age .08 .05 .04 .03 .27***
Sex .11 .04 .03 .10 .09
Self-esteem .09 .12 .05 .03 .01
Extraversion .10 .03 .02 .08 .03
Conscientiousness .16* .11 .15* .00 .11
Neuroticism .07 .11 .05 .05 .03
Agreeableness .13* .00 .17** .01 .03
Openness .06 .12* .13* .13* .04
Social cohesion .15 .38*** .20* .13 .07
Model R2 .09** .34*** .16*** .04 .10***

Note: All betas are standardized betas. Although openness was a significant individual predictor of relationship maintenance, this predictor was not interpreted in the present
study because the model was not significant. N = 301.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

Table 4 online social networks disclosed more intimate, personal informa-


Regressing self-disclosure on individual variables, social cohesion, and motives. tion on Facebook than others. Conscientiousness and agreeable-
Amount Depth Breadth ness were indirect predictors through the virtual community
Predictors b b b
motive (see Table 3 and Fig. 2). Less conscientious, more agreeable
Step 1 individuals who were motivated by the desire to meet new people
Age .02 .05 .00 online disclosed information of greater depth.
Sex .08 .10 .10
Self-esteem .12 .14 .22**
Extraversion .09 .15* .08 3.3. Breadth of self-disclosure
Conscientiousness .00 .01 .01
Neuroticism .00 .02 .22** RQ3 asked what variables predicted disclosing more breadth of
Agreeableness .03 .02 .06
Openness .05 .08 .20***
information on Facebook. The total model explained 18% of the
Social cohesion .02 .07 .07 variance in breadth of self-disclosure, F(14, 285) = 4.43, p < .001.
Four variables served as direct predictors: self-esteem, neuroti-
Step 2 cism, openness, and relationship maintenance motive (see Table 4).
Virtual Community .09 .18** .05 Not surprisingly, people who are more open, using Facebook to
Companionship .06 .14 .16 maintain existing relationships were more likely to post about a
Exhibitionism .21** .12 .07
variety of topics. Additionally, those with lower self-esteem and
Relationship maintenance .14* .09 .29***
Passing Time .03 .00 .01 less neuroticism disclosed with more breadth on Facebook.
Final model R2 .17*** .24*** .18***

Note: All betas are final standardized betas on the last step of the regression. 4. Discussion
N = 301.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .05.
This study used U&G to examine the predictors of self-disclo-
***
p < .001. sure on Facebook. Ultimately, the purpose of this study was to
determine which predictors and motives were characteristic of dis-
closive Facebook users. Facebook users disclosing the largest
amount of information used the medium for exhibitionism and
amount of self-disclosure: exhibitionism and relationship mainte- relationship maintenance. These participants were also likely to
nance (see Table 4). Participants who used Facebook to get atten- be less conscientious, be less agreeable, perceive less social cohe-
tion and to maintain existing relationships posted the largest sion, and be more open. The profile of the most disclosive Facebook
amount of personal information about themselves. The path anal- users in terms of amount, therefore, includes those who want to
ysis showed several indirect predictors as well, mediated through maintain their existing relationships, as well as those who want
the exhibitionism motive (see Table 3 and Fig. 2). Participants low- to get attention, perhaps because of their diminished social cohe-
er in conscientiousness, agreeableness, and social cohesion, as well sion and agreeableness offline.
as those higher in openness disclosed more information on Facebook users who were more extraverted and who used Face-
Facebook. book to establish a virtual community disclosed the most personal
information. Less conscientiousness and more agreeableness were
indirect predictors of depth of self-disclosure through the virtual
3.2. Depth of self-disclosure community motive. Finally, Facebook users posted about a wider
variety of topics when they had lower self-esteem, lower neuroti-
Depth of self-disclosure was explored in RQ2. To determine the cism, and more openness. Additionally, participants who used
characteristics associated with participants who disclosed more Facebook to maintain their existing relationships discussed more
intimate information on Facebook, the path analysis was exam- breadth of topics.
ined. The total model explained 24% of the variance in depth of The results of this study are in many ways confirming of exist-
self-disclosure, F(14, 285) = 6.42, p < .001. Extraversion and virtual ing research and theory. U&G provides a helpful framework with
community were direct predictors of depth (see Table 4). Specifi- which to study Facebook self-disclosure. Each of the total models
cally, more extraverted individuals who used Facebook to develop predicting self-disclosiveness was significant at the p < .001 level,
56 E.E. Hollenbaugh, A.L. Ferris / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 50–58

Self-esteem Extraversion

Virtual
Community
(conscientiousness-,
agreeableness+)
SD Amount

Companionship
(conscientiousness-,
openness+, social
cohesion-)

Exhibitionism SD Depth
(conscientiousness-,
agreeableness-,
openness+, social
cohesion-)

Relationship SD Breadth
Maintenance

Passing Time
(men)

Neuroticism Openness

Positive β
Negative β

Fig. 2. Model of significant profile characteristics of Facebook users’ self-disclosure (SD). Significant predictors of each Facebook motive are listed in the box in parentheses.
See Table 4 for beta weights. Solid lines indicate positive relationships, and dashed lines indicate negative relationships.

and the only motive that was not significantly explained by the same motives as previous research (e.g., Sheldon, 2008b), with
predictor variables was relationship maintenance. Scholars should the exception of coolness. Items from the coolness and exhibition
employ U&G as they continue to examine such individualized, ac- motive loaded together, revealing the latent motive exhibitionism.
tive uses of the internet. Due in part to the ubiquity of Facebook, it is no longer very ‘‘cool’’
Facebook is a highly social environment, and its users do seem to maintain a Facebook profile. In fact, anecdotal evidence would
to adjust their verbal communication to convey affection and suggest that it is, in fact cooler to not be on Facebook. However,
sociability (Walther, 1992). Open, extraverted individuals who Facebook users can get attention and establish a sense of coolness
use Facebook for relationship maintenance and to establish a vir- through the things they post on Facebook.
tual community are generally more disclosive, as measured by This research has also contributed to the discipline by revealing
amount, depth, and breadth of self-disclosure. This is fitting con- the individual and social factors that affect people’s motives for
sidering our traditional views of the role of self-disclosure in rela- using Facebook (see Table 3). The Big Five seem to be useful in this
tionship development (Altman & Taylor, 1973). model, as consciousness, agreeableness, and openness predicted
Along with the more expected findings, there were several new motives, while extraversion, neuroticism, and openness directly
contributions made to the literature. For example, using Facebook predicted self-disclosure dimensions. However, age and sex made
for exhibitionism resulted in high amounts of self-disclosure. little to no impact in the total model to predict motives or self-dis-
Although little research has supported this, it is not surprising closure. The only significant difference among these well-estab-
due to the highly textual Facebook environment; self-disclosure lished predictors of online behavior was that younger
is likely to get people’s attention. Another quite surprising finding participants were more motivated to use Facebook to pass time
was the lack of significant relationships among age, sex, and self- than older participants.
disclosure dimensions. These two demographic characteristics – This study’s findings related to self-esteem and social cohesion
sex in particular – have consistently shown to affect self-disclosure lend support to the social compensation hypothesis. Although
patterns. Perhaps the social environment of Facebook where every- social cohesion was not a direct predictor of self-disclosure, it
one, regardless of sex or age, is encouraged to self-disclose is scrub- was inversely related to the motives of companionship and exhibi-
bing out these traditional differences. On the other hand, an tionism. In other words, those who felt less connected to their
examination of the relatively low means and standard deviations social groups were drawn to Facebook to feel less lonely by con-
of scores for the self-disclosure dimensions in this study suggest necting to others, as well as to get attention. Similarly, those with
that perhaps Facebook users are becoming more selective in when, lower self-esteem were more likely to disclose about a variety of
how, and to whom they disclose. topics on Facebook. In these ways, it appears that Facebook is
In addition to the findings regarding predictors of self-disclosive one way that some choose to fill a void in their lives.
behavior, another contribution of this study concerns the motives On the other hand, there is some support for the social enhance-
for using Facebook. The exploratory factor analyses revealed the ment hypothesis as well. Participants who were more open as a
E.E. Hollenbaugh, A.L. Ferris / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 50–58 57

personality trait disclosed with more breadth and tended to use In conclusion, this research builds upon existing motives stud-
Facebook for companionship and exhibitionism. Extraverted Face- ies and supports the application of U&G in studying the implica-
book users reported greater depth of self-disclosure. Clearly the tions of Facebook use. Individual and sociological variables, as
use of Facebook is complex enough to accommodate all types of well as Facebook motives were shown to have a significant impact
individuals, whether they are searching to compensate for some- on users’ self-disclosure. As Facebook continues to dominate the
thing missing in their everyday lives or to extend their existing social networking landscape, it is important to persist in making
sociability online. sense of this important medium in 21st century culture.

4.1. Limitations and future research directions


References

The implications of this study should be interpreted with some Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development of interpersonal
reservation due to study’s limitations. Facebook in itself posed a relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Baek, K., Holton, A., Harp, D., & Yaschur, C. (2011). The links that bind: Uncovering
limitation with regard to data collection. As noted previously, at-
novel motivations for linking on Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 27,
tempts to randomly select users through the ‘‘People’’ directory 2243–2248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.07.003.
were met with spam reports. Based on this experience, random Barker, V., & Ota, H. (2011). Mixi diary versus Facebook photos: Social networking
sampling may not be a reality in the Facebook realm. There is a site use among Japanese and Caucasian American females. Journal of
Intercultural Communication Research, 40, 39–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
general distrust of email requests, so it is becoming more difficult 17475759.2011.558321.
to convince people to participate in social scientific research. As Buhrke, R., & Fuqua, D. (1987). Sex differences in same- and cross-sex supportive
advertisements increase in newsfeeds and viruses are spread relationships. Sex Roles, 17, 339–352.
Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem. Psychological
through unknown links, it is becoming increasingly difficult to gain Bulletin, 130, 392–414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.392.
trust from strangers via Facebook. Paid advertisements can be uti- Davidson, L., & Duberman, L. (1982). Friendship: Communication and interactional
lized, but researchers are limited to people who self-select into patterns in same-sex dyads. Sex Roles, 8, 809–822.
Davis, M. H., & Kraus, L. A. (1989). Social contact, loneliness, and mass media use: a
their study. At the time of this study, it seems that random sam- test of two hypotheses. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19, 1100–1124.
pling of Facebook users poses a challenge that must be met by fu- Derlega, V. J., Metts, S., Petronio, S., & Margulis, S. T. (1993). Self-disclosure. Newbury
ture researchers. Park, CA: Sage.
Dindia, K. (2002). Self-disclosure research: Knowledge through meta-analysis. In M.
As a result of the challenges of data collection, a convenience Allen, R. W. Preiss, B. M. Gayle, & N. Burrell (Eds.), Interpersonal communication
sample was chosen. While the sample does reach beyond the social research: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 169–185). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
networks of the two researchers, this sample may not be represen- Facebook.com (2013a). Basic info. <https://www.facebook.com/facebook/info>
Retrieved 27.03.13.
tative of the Facebook population. This sample was predominantly
Facebook.com (2013b). Newsroom: Key facts. <http://newsroom.fb.com/Key-
Caucasian women, and although this group does make up the Facts>. Retrieved 27.03.013.
majority of Facebook users (Hampton et al., 2011), this group Forest, A. L., & Wood, J. V. (2012). When social networking is not working:
was overrepresented in the present study. Perhaps sex differences Individuals with low self-esteem recognize but do not repeat the benefits of
self-disclosure on Facebook. Psychological Science, 23, 295–302. http://
did not emerge because there were not enough men to test for dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611429709.
them. While the average age of the participants in this study was Fox, J., & Warber, K. M. (2013). Romantic relationship development in the age of
lower than the average Facebook user (38 years), the study does Facebook: An exploratory study of emerging adults’ perceptions, motives, and
behaviors. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16, 3–7. http://
move Facebook research beyond the college/high school student dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0288.
demographic. Future research should consider a quota sample to Hampton, K. N., Goulet, L. S., Rainie, L., & Purcell, K. (2011) Social networking sites
mimic the Facebook landscape more closely. and our lives: How people’s trust, personal relationships, and civic and political
involvement are connected to their use of social networking sites and other
Another potential limitation involves the relatively narrow technologies. Pew Internet & American Life Project. <http://www.
scope of measuring self-disclosure. The present study asked partic- pewinternet.org/~/media/Files/Reports/2011/PIP%20-%20Social%
ipants to consider only their self-disclosive behavior, but in fact 20networking%20sites%20and%20our%20lives.pdf>. Retrieved 14.05.13.
Hew, K. F. (2011). Students’ and teachers’ use of Facebook. Computers in Human
self-disclosure does not exist in a vacuum. Instead, the social net- Behavior, 27, 662–676. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.11.020.
working capabilities of Facebook allow for others to contribute to Hills, P., & Argyle, M. (2003). Uses of the internet and their relationships with
the body of information about a person (Walther, Van Der Heide, individual differences in personality. Computers in Human Behavior, 19, 59–70.
Hollenbaugh, E. E. (2011). Motives for maintaining personal journal blogs.
Kim, Westerman, & Tong, 2008). Facebook users can post verbal
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 13–20. http://dx.doi.org/
information on others’ walls, tag each other in pictures or in geo- 10.1089/cyber.2009.0403.
graphic locations, and comment on each other’s posts, contributing Hunt, D., Atkin, D., & Krishnan, A. (2012). The influence of computer-mediated
more information than one individual may disclose about him/her- communication apprehension on motives for Facebook use. Journal of
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 56, 187–202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
self. Furthermore, pictures may disclose more information than 08838151.2012.678717.
words for certain types of Facebook users (Van Der Heide, D’Ange- John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The big five inventory–Versions 4a
lo, & Schumaker, 2012). Future research should make every at- and 54. Berkeley: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and
Social Research.
tempt to explore how people co-create identities on Facebook by John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big-
contributing personal information about others to the greater Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual Issues. In O. P. John,
Facebook community through text, picture, and video. R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research
(pp. 114–158). New York: Guilford Press.
This study revealed some intriguing findings related to self-es- Joinson, A. N. (2008). ‘‘Looking at’’, ‘‘looking up’’ or ‘‘keeping up with’’ people?
teem and social cohesion. Namely, individuals with lower self-es- Motives and uses of Facebook. CHI 2008 Proceedings: Online social networking
teem and less social cohesion were more disclosive, either (pp. 1027–1036).
Jourard, S. M. (1971). The transparent self (rev. ed.). New York: Van Nostrand
directly or indirectly through motives. More research is needed Reinhold.
to look at the reasons for and social implications of self-disclosure Kalpidou, M., Costin, D., & Morris, J. (2011). The relationship between Facebook and
in such a public format for these individuals. People with lower the well-being of undergraduate college students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior,
and Social Networking, 14, 183–189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0061.
self-esteem tend to disclose more negative and less positive infor-
Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1974). Utilization of mass communication by
mation on Facebook (Forest & Wood, 2012), which may be per- the individual. In J. G. Blumler & E. Katz (Eds.), The uses of mass communications:
ceived socially as fishing for compliments or searching for Current perspectives on gratifications research (pp. 19–32). Beverly Hills, CA:
confirmation. It is unclear how this is received within their online Sage.
Knapp, M. L., Ellis, D. G., & Williams, B. A. (1980). Perceptions of communication
networks, as well as whether or not this exchange of disclosure and behavior associated with relationship terms. Communication Monographs, 47,
responses is personally fulfilling. 262–278.
58 E.E. Hollenbaugh, A.L. Ferris / Computers in Human Behavior 30 (2014) 50–58

Lee, J.-E. R., Moore, D. C., Park, E.-A., & Park, S. G. (2012). Who wants to be ‘‘friend- paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication
rich’’? Social compensatory friending on Facebook and the moderating role of Association, New Orleans, LA.
public self-consciousness. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1036–1043. http:// Sheldon, P. (2008a). The relationship between unwillingness-to-communicate and
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.01.006. students’ Facebook use. Journal of Media Psychology, 20, 67–75. http://
Ma, M. L., & Leung, L. (2006). Unwillingness-to-communicate, perceptions of the dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105.20.2.67.
internet and self-disclosure in ICQ. Telematics and Informatics, 23, 22–37. http:// Sheldon, P. (2008b). Student favorite: Facebook and motives for its use.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2005.01.001. Southwestern Mass Communication Journal, 23(2), 39–53.
Malone, G. P., Pillow, D. R., & Osman, A. (2012). The General Belongingness Scale Sinha, V. (1972). Age differences in self-disclosure. Developmental Psychology, 7,
(GBS): Assessing achieved belongingness. Personality and Individual Differences, 257–258.
52, 311–316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.027. Smith, A. (2011). Why Americans use social media: Social networking sites are
Mazer, J. P., Murphy, R. E., & Simonds, C. J. (2007). I’ll see you on ‘‘Facebook’’: The appealing as a way to maintain contact with close ties and reconnect with old
effects of computer-mediated teacher self-disclosure on student motivation, friends. Pew Internet & American Life Project. <http://www.pewinternet.org/~/
affective learning, and classroom climate. Communication Education, 56, 1–17. media//Files/Reports/2011/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03634520601009710. Why%20Americans%20Use%20Social%20Media.pdf>. Retrieved 14.05.13.
McKenna, K. Y. A., Green, A. S., & Gleason, M. E. J. (2002). Relationship formation on Smock, A. D., Ellison, N. B., Lampe, C., & Wohn, D. Y. (2011). Facebook as a toolkit: A
the internet: what’s the big attraction? Journal of Social Issues, 5, 9–31. uses and gratification approach to unbundling feature use. Computers in Human
Miura, A., & Yamashita, K. (2007). Psychological and social influences on blog Behavior, 27, 2322–2329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.07.011.
writing: An online survey of blog authors in Japan. Journal of Computer-Mediated Special, W. P., & Li-Barber, K. T. (2012). Self-disclosure and student satisfaction with
Communication, 12, 1452–1471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007. Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 624–630. http://dx.doi.org/
00381.x. 10.1016/j.chb.2011.11.008.
Morton, T. L. (1978). Intimacy and reciprocity of exchange: A comparison of spouses Suler, J. (2004). The online disinhibition effect. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 7,
and strangers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 72–81. 321–326.
Papacharissi, Z., & Mendelson, A. (2011). Toward a new(er) sociability: Uses, Tosun, L. P. (2012). Motives for Facebook use and expressing ‘‘true self’’ on the
gratifications and social capital on Facebook. In S. Papathanassopoulos (Ed.), internet. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1510–1517. http://dx.doi.org/
Media perspectives for the 21st century (pp. 212–230). NY: Routledge. 10.1016/j.chb.2012.03.018.
Peter, J., Valkenburg, P. M., & Schouten, A. P. (2005). Developing a model of Valkenburg, P. M., Schouten, A. P., & Peter, J. (2005). Adolescents’ identity
adolescent friendship formation on the internet. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 8, experiments on the internet. New Media & Society, 7, 383–402. http://
423–430. dx.doi.org/10.1177/1461444805052282.
Punyanunt-Carter, N. M. (2006). An analysis of college students’ self-disclosure Van Der Heide, B., D’Angelo, J. D., & Schumaker, E. M. (2012). The effects of verbal
behaviors on the internet. College Student Journal, 40, 329–331. versusphotographic self-presentation on impression formation in Facebook.
Raacke, J., & Bonds-Raacke, J. (2008). MySpace and Facebook: Applying the uses and Journal of Communication, 62, 98–116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-
gratifications theory to exploring friend-networking sites. CyberPsychology & 2466.2011.01617.x.
Behavior, 11, 169–174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2007.0056. Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction: A
Rosell, S. A. et al. (1995). Changing maps: Governing in a world of rapid change. relational perspective. Communication Research, 19, 52–90.
Ottawa, ON: Carleton University Press. Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal,
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books Inc.. interpersonal, and hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23,
Ross, C., Orr, E. S., Sisic, M., Arsenault, J. M., Simmering, M. G., & Orr, R. R. (2009). 3–43.
Personality and motivations associated with Facebook use. Computers in Human Walther, J. B., Van Der Heide, B., Kim, S., Westerman, D., & Tong, S. T. (2008). The role
Behavior, 25, 578–586. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.024. of friends’ appearance and behavior on evaluations of individuals on Facebook:
Rubin, A. M. (2002). The uses-and-gratifications perspective of media effects. In J. Are we known by the company we keep? Human Communication Research, 34,
Bryant & D. Zillman (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (2nd 28–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2007.00312.x.
ed., pp. 525–548). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Wheeless, L. R. (1978). A follow-up study of the relationships among trust,
Ruggiero, T. E. (2000). Uses and gratifications in the 21st century. Mass disclosure, and interpersonal solidarity. Human Communication Research, 4,
Communication & Society, 3, 3–37. 143–157.
Ryan, T., & Xenos, S. (2011). Who uses Facebook? An investigation into the Yamamoto, M. (2011). Community newspaper use promotes social cohesion.
relationship between the Big 5, shyness, narcissism, loneliness, and Facebook Newspaper Research Journal, 32, 19–33.
usage. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 1658–1664. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Yang, C.-C., & Brown, B. B. (2013). Motives for using Facebook, patterns of Facebook
j.chb.2011.02.004. activities, and late adolescents’ social adjustment to college. Journal of Youth and
Schumaker, E. M., & Van Der Heide, B. (2011). Interpersonal feedback on Facebook: Adolescence, 42, 403–416. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-012-9836-x.
The effects of feedback volume, valence, and mode on self-esteem. Unpublished

You might also like