You are on page 1of 7

Research

JAMA Pediatrics | Original Investigation

Association Between Moderate and Late Preterm Birth


and Neurodevelopment and Social-Emotional Development
at Age 2 Years
Jeanie L. Cheong, MD; Lex W. Doyle, MD; Alice C. Burnett, PhD; Katherine J. Lee, PhD; Jennifer M. Walsh, MD; Cody R. Potter, PhD;
Karli Treyvaud, PhD; Deanne K. Thompson, PhD; Joy E. Olsen, PhD; Peter J. Anderson, PhD; Alicia J. Spittle, PhD

Supplemental content
IMPORTANCE Moderate and late preterm (MLPT) births comprise most preterm infants.
Therefore, long-term developmental concerns in this population potentially have a large
public health influence. While there are increasing reports of developmental problems in
MLPT children, detail is lacking on the precise domains that are affected.

OBJECTIVE To compare neurodevelopment and social-emotional development between


MLPT infants and term-born control infants at age 2 years.

DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS This investigation was a prospective longitudinal cohort
study at a single tertiary hospital. Participants were MLPT infants (32-36 weeks’ completed
gestation) and healthy full-term controls (ⱖ37 weeks’ gestation) recruited at birth. During a
3-year period between December 7, 2009, and November 7, 2012, MLPT infants were
recruited at birth from the neonatal unit and postnatal wards of the Royal Women’s Hospital,
Melbourne, Australia. The term control recruitment extended to March 26, 2014. The dates of
the data developmental assessments were February 23, 2012, to April 8, 2016.

EXPOSURE Moderate and late preterm birth.

MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Cerebral palsy, blindness, and deafness assessed by a
pediatrician; cognitive, language, and motor development assessed using the Bayley Scales of
Infant Development–Third Edition (developmental delay was defined as less than −1 SD
relative to the mean in controls in any domain of the scales); and social-emotional and
behavioral problems assessed by a parent questionnaire (Infant Toddler Social Emotional
Assessment). Outcomes were compared between birth groups using linear and logistic
regression, adjusted for social risk.

RESULTS In total, 198 MLPT infants (98.5% of 201 recruited) and 183 term-born controls
(91.0% of 201 recruited) were assessed at 2 years’ corrected age. Compared with controls,
MLPT children had worse cognitive, language, and motor development at age 2 years, with
adjusted composite score mean differences of −5.3 (95% CI, −8.2 to −2.4) for cognitive
development, −11.4 (95% CI, −15.3 to −7.5) for language development, and −7.3 (95% CI, −10.6
to −3.9) for motor development. The odds of developmental delay were higher in the MLPT
group compared with controls, with adjusted odds ratios of 1.8 (95% CI, 1.1-3.0) for cognitive
delay, 3.1 (95% CI, 1.8-5.2) for language delay, and 2.4 (95% CI, 1.3-4.5) for motor delay.
Overall social-emotional competence was worse in MLPT children compared with controls (t
statistic mean difference, −3.6 (95% CI, −5.8 to −1.4), but other behavioral domains were
similar. The odds of being at risk for social-emotional competence were 3.9 (95% CI, 1.4-10.9)
for MLPT children compared with controls.

CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Moderate and late preterm children exhibited developmental
Author Affiliations: Author
delay compared with their term-born peers, most marked in the language domain. This affiliations are listed at the end of this
knowledge of developmental needs in MLPT infants will assist in targeting surveillance and article.
intervention. Corresponding Author: Jeanie L.
Cheong, MD, Neonatal Services,
Royal Women’s Hospital, Level 7,
JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171(4):e164805. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.4805 Parkville, Victoria, Australia 3052
Published online February 6, 2017. (jeanie.cheong@thewomens.org.au).

(Reprinted) 1/7

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 10/20/2018


Research Original Investigation Association Between Preterm Birth and Neurodevelopment, Social-Emotional Development at 2 Years

P
rematurity is a recognized risk factor for developmen-
tal problems in childhood and adolescence.1 For many Key Points
years, the focus of research has been on developmen-
Question What is the influence of moderate and late preterm
tal outcomes of infants born very preterm (<32 weeks’ gesta- birth on neurodevelopment and social-emotional development at
tion). However, in recent times, data are emerging that chil- age 2 years?
dren born moderate and late preterm (MLPT) between 32 and
Findings This longitudinal cohort study found that, compared with
36 weeks’ completed gestation are also at greater risk of de-
term-born children, children born moderate and late preterm are
velopmental problems compared with their term-born peers.2-5 delayed in cognitive, language, and motor development. In addition,
Because MLPT infants comprise the largest group of preterm they have poorer social competence at 2 years’ corrected age.
infants (approximately 80%),6,7 small increases in adverse out-
Meaning Developmental surveillance is important given the risk
comes in this population have the potential to be a consider-
of developmental delay in moderate and late preterm children.
able public health burden. Recent reports on early childhood
outcomes of MLPT infants highlight deficits in cognitive and
motor domains, as well as social functioning.5,8-10 However,
limitations of the studies include reliance on questionnaire as- structure, education of the primary caregiver, employment sta-
sessments (rather than direct) and low follow-up rates, which tus of the primary income earner, occupation of the primary
potentially underestimate the prevalence of developmental income earner, language spoken at home, and maternal age at
problems. Furthermore, a better understanding of the spe- birth of the child. Each variable was scored on a 3-point scale,
cific developmental deficits in MLPT children is needed to in- where zero represented lowest risk and 2 represented highest
form public health policy, as is targeted surveillance and in- risk, and summed to give a total score (range, 0-12). Social risk
tervention in developmental domains at greatest risk. This was then dichotomized to higher (total social risk score of ≥2)
study aimed to fill this gap in the literature by comparing neu- or lower (total social risk score of <2) risk.12 All participants were
rosensory outcomes and cognitive, language, motor, and social- followed up at age 2 years, corrected for prematurity, at The
emotional development between MLPT children and term- Royal Children’s Hospital Melbourne.
born control children at 2 years’ corrected age. Given that there This study was approved by the Human Research Ethics
may be differences in developmental outcomes within the Committee of the Royal Women’s Hospital. Written informed
MLPT group, which spans from 32 to 36 weeks’ completed ges- consent was obtained from parents of all participants.
tation, we also aimed to explore whether there was an asso-
ciation between gestational age at birth and developmental out- 2-Year Assessments
comes within this group. Cerebral palsy was diagnosed in children with abnormal tone
and function, after the exclusion of other causes of motor dys-
function, by a pediatrician. The topography and severity of ce-
rebral palsy were classified according to the Gross Motor Func-
Methods tion Classification Scale.13 Blindness and deafness were also
Participants assessed by a pediatrician. Blindness was defined as having vi-
The Late Preterm MRI Study11 is a longitudinal cohort study of sual acuity of less than 6/60 in the better eye, and deafness was
brain development and outcomes in MLPT children compared defined as a hearing impairment requiring amplification or a
with term-born children. During a 3-year period between De- cochlear implant or worse.
cember 7, 2009, and November 7, 2012, MLPT infants were re- Cognitive, language, and motor development were as-
cruited at birth from the neonatal unit and postnatal wards of sessed using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development–Third Edi-
the Royal Women’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia, the largest tion (Bayley-III).14 The Bayley-III is age standardized, is widely
of the 3 perinatal centers in the state of Victoria where tertiary used in research and clinical settings, and has published norms
neonatal care is provided. The dates of the data neurodevelop- that have a mean (SD) of 100 (15). Given the concerns about
mental assessments were February 23, 2012, to April 8, 2016. underestimation of developmental delay in the Australian
It is hospital policy for all infants born less than 36 weeks’ population,15,16 delay in any developmental domain was de-
gestation to be admitted to the neonatal unit. Infants with con- fined as less than −1 SD relative to the mean for the control chil-
genital abnormalities or genetic syndromes known to affect de- dren in the present study. We also defined moderate to severe
velopment were excluded from the study. Healthy term-born developmental delay as less than −2 SDs relative to the mean
infants (≥37 weeks’ gestation and birth weight ≥2500 g) were for the control children. All assessors (one of us, C.R.P., and
recruited from the postnatal wards at the Royal Women’s Hos- other nonauthors) were experienced in both the Bayley-III and
pital. Term infants were excluded from the study if they were neurological assessments and were unaware of group alloca-
unwell at birth, received resuscitation, were admitted to the tion and perinatal history.
neonatal nursery, or were identified as having conditions af- Social-emotional and behavioral problems were assessed
fecting growth or development. using the Infant Toddler Social Emotional Assessment (ITSEA).17
Perinatal, neonatal, and maternal details were recorded by The ITSEA is a parent-report questionnaire developed for chil-
medical record review at the time of recruitment. Social risk dren aged 12 to 36 months. Aspects of child behavior are rated
was assessed using sociodemographic factors known to be as- on 135 items using a 3-point Likert-type rating scale (0 is not true
sociated with outcomes in preterm infants, including family or rarely, 1 is somewhat true or sometimes, and 2 is very true or

2/7 JAMA Pediatrics April 2017 Volume 171, Number 4 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 10/20/2018


Association Between Preterm Birth and Neurodevelopment, Social-Emotional Development at 2 Years Original Investigation Research

often [“no opportunity” is available for some questions]). Age- able to be contacted) and 91.0% (183 of 201) for controls (1 with-
and sex-specific t statistics (mean [SD], 50 [10]; range, 25-80) drew from the study, 3 were living abroad and 14 were unable
are calculated for the following 4 domains: externalizing be- to be contacted). Compared with those who were assessed at
havior problems, internalizing behavior problems, dysregula- age 2 years, MLPT children who were not assessed had younger
tion, and social-emotional competence. The mean scores at or mothers (26.7 vs 34.1 years) and higher social risk (100% [3 of
above the 90th percentile for externalizing behavioral prob- 3] vs 32.8% [63 of 192]). Compared with those who were as-
lems, internalizing behavioral problems, and dysregulation at sessed at age 2 years, controls who were not assessed had
or below the 10th percentile for social-emotional competence younger gestational age (39.2 vs 39.8 weeks), younger moth-
were defined as being at risk of behavioral or social-emotional ers (30.6 vs 33.5 years), and higher birth weight z score (0.80
problems. The ITSEA has good test-retest reliability and good vs 0.16). Other characteristics were similar.
criterion validity.18 Two-year outcomes are summarized in Table 2. There were
2 children in the MLPT group with cerebral palsy, one with
Statistical Analysis hemiplegia (Gross Motor Function Classification System level
The expected rate of developmental delay for the controls 1) and another with quadriplegia (Gross Motor Function Clas-
based on previous literature was 16%.19 With a sample size of sification System level 3) (P = .17 for adjusted by logistic re-
201 participants in each group, the study was powered to de- gression). No children had blindness or deafness in either
tect an increase in the prevalence of developmental delay to group. There was evidence that MLPT children performed more
27.5% or higher among the MLPT children (equivalent to an poorly than controls in all domains of development, most
odds ratio [OR] of 2) with 80.0% power based on a 2-sided test marked in language, with a mean difference of approxi-
with type I error of 5.0%, which would represent a clinically mately 0.7 SD. This finding appeared to be driven equally by
important difference. performance in the receptive and expressive language subdo-
Participant characteristics were compared between those mains. There was also evidence that the MLPT group had
who were assessed at age 2 years and those who were not using poorer social-emotional competence compared with con-
means (SDs) for continuous variables and proportions for cat- trols, with a mean difference of 0.4 SD, but were similar to con-
egorical variables. Two-year development was compared be- trols in the other emotional and behavioral domains. Adjust-
tween groups using linear regression, with a separate model for ment for social risk did not alter the conclusions and had little
each developmental outcome. The analysis was first performed effect on the mean differences. Adjustment for imbalances in
unadjusted and then adjusted for social risk, which is known to perinatal characteristics, including birth weight z score and
affect development. Rates of cerebral palsy, blindness, deafness, multiple birth, did not alter any conclusions.
developmental delay, and social-emotional problems were com- There was evidence that rates of a developmental delay
pared between groups using logistic regression, both unadjusted were higher in the MLPT group than in controls in all do-
and adjusted for social risk, which is an important factor influ- mains, with weaker evidence for moderate or severe cogni-
encing development. All regression analyses were fitted using tive delay (Table 3). Compared with controls, MLPT children
generalized estimating equations and are reported with robust were more likely to be at risk in the social-emotional compe-
(sandwich) estimates of standard errors to account for cluster- tence domain but not other behavioral domains. Adjustment
ing because of multiple births within the same family. Linear re- for social risk did not alter any conclusions.
gression was also used to determine the association between ges- Within the MLPT group, there was little evidence of an as-
tational age at birth and developmental outcomes at age 2 years sociation between gestational age at birth and neurodevelop-
in the MLPT group. Data were analyzed using statistical software ment or social-emotional development at age 2 years. These
(Stata, version 14.0; StataCorp LP). results are summarized in the eTable in the Supplement.

Results Discussion
Two hundred one MLPT infants and an equal number of term- This study adds to the growing body of evidence that MLPT
born controls were recruited. Participant characteristics are birth is associated with an increased risk of developmental
summarized in Table 1. Compared with controls, the MLPT in- problems compared with term birth. Using direct, objective,
fants were more likely to be small for gestational age at birth. standardized assessments, MLPT children at 2 years’ cor-
Compared with mothers of control infants, mothers of MLPT rected age performed more poorly in cognitive, language, and
infants had higher rates of pregnancy-related complications, motor domains compared with term-born controls. The dis-
such as preeclampsia and antepartum hemorrhage, and higher parity was greatest in the language domain, where MLPT chil-
rates of assisted conception, multiple births, antenatal corti- dren had 3 times higher odds of language impairment than their
costeroid use, and cesarean delivery. Moderate and late pre- term-born peers, with both receptive and expressive lan-
term infants had higher rates of neonatal morbidity com- guage equally affected. It is also of great concern that MLPT
pared with controls, although the overall rates were low. Social children appear to be at much greater risk of motor impair-
risk was comparable between the groups. ment, with the odds of MLPT children scoring less than −2 SDs
The follow-up rates were high for both groups at 98.5% (198 being 9 times higher than their term-born peers (after adjust-
of 201) for MLPT children (1 was living abroad and 2 were un- ment for social risk). We also found evidence of poorer social-

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics April 2017 Volume 171, Number 4 3/7

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 10/20/2018


Research Original Investigation Association Between Preterm Birth and Neurodevelopment, Social-Emotional Development at 2 Years

Table 1. Demographic Data


MLPT Group Term Group
Variable (n = 201) (n = 201) P Value
Maternal age, mean (SD), y 34.0 (4.8) 33.2 (4.7) .12
Maternal preeclampsia, No. (%) 36 (17.9) 3 (1.5) <.001
Assisted conception, No./total No. (%) 39/198 (19.7) 20/199 (10.1) .01
Gestational age at birth, mean (SD), wk 34.4 (1.2) 39.8 (1.2) <.001
Antepartum hemorrhage, No. (%) 19 (9.5) 6 (3.0) .01
Antenatal corticosteroid use, No. (%) 121 (60.2) 6 (3.0) <.001
Antenatal magnesium sulfate use, No. (%) 15 (7.5) 0 NA
Induction of labor, No./total No. (%) 15/200 (7.5) 79 (39.3) <.001
Multiple birth, No. (%) 74 (36.8) 2 (1.0) <.001
Twins, No. 71 2 NA
Triplets, No. 3 0 NA
Cesarean delivery, No. (%) 137 (68.2) 77 (38.3) <.001
Male birth, No. (%) 96 (47.8) 107 (53.2) .27
Birth weight, mean (SD), g 2161 (463) 3538 (447) <.001
Birth weight z score, mean (SD) −0.35 (1.16) 0.21 (0.82) <.001
Apgar score, median (IQR)
At 1 min 8 (6-9) 9 (8-9) <.001
At 5 min 9 (8-9) 9 (9-9) <.001
Major resuscitation at birth, No. (%)a 5 (2.5) 0 NA
Respiratory support, No. (%)b 28 (13.9) 0 NA
Jaundice requiring treatment, No./total No. (%) 147/201 (73.1) 5/200 (2.5) <.001 Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile
range; MLPT, moderate and late
Sepsis, No. (%)c 1 (0.5) 0 NA
preterm; NA, not applicable.
Necrotizing enterocolitis, No. 0 0 NA a
Received positive-pressure
Oxygen therapy at 36 wk, No. 0 0 NA ventilation, cardiopulmonary
Postnatal corticosteroid use, No. 0 0 NA resuscitation, or adrenaline.
b
Any intraventricular hemorrhage, No./total No.d 0 0/88 NA Includes both invasive and
noninvasive respiratory support.
Cystic periventricular leukomalacia, No./total No.d 0 0/88 NA c
Culture positive or raised white
Surgery in the newborn period, No. 0 0 NA blood cell count or C-reactive
Days in the hospital, median (IQR) 20 (14-27) [n = 199] 2 (2-3) [n = 142] <.001 protein level with at least 5 days of
antibiotic treatment.
Higher social risk, No./total No. (%) 66/195 (33.8) 48/184 (26.1) .10
d
As noted on magnetic resonance
Corrected age at follow-up, mean (SD), mo 25.3 (1.3) [n = 198] 25.6 (1.2) [n = 183] .01
imaging at term-equivalent age.

emotional competence in the MLPT group. The competence study.22 They did not report differences in motor develop-
domain of the ITSEA reflects aspects of children’s attention, ment. Only late preterm children (34-36 weeks’ gestation) were
compliance, and motivation, as well as social relationships (eg, included in the Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey–Birth Co-
play, empathy, and prosocial behavior). It is possible that these hort study. Our findings of greater disparity between groups
early signs of challenge are precursors for some of the school- may be partially explained by our sample, who were re-
age behavioral and learning problems described in MLPT cruited from a tertiary hospital and included moderate pre-
children.20,21 term infants. It is possible that our cohort included a greater
In our MLPT sample, cognitive, language, and motor de- proportion of sicker MLPT infants who were admitted to the
velopment were delayed compared with term-born controls neonatal nursery than in the general population, especially in
in the order of 0.5 to 0.7 SD (−6.3 to −11.8 points), which are the more mature gestational age range.
clinically important differences. Although our findings con- Rates of developmental delay in all domains were higher
cur with other studies that MLPT children demonstrate poorer in the MLPT children compared with controls. In this study,
development as early as age 2 to 3 years, the disparity be- we calculated rates of delay based on means (SDs) of the con-
tween MLPT children and term-born controls was larger in our temporaneous control group, which are higher than the nor-
study. Voigt et al9 reported a mean difference of −5.4 on the mative mean (SD) of 100 (15) in the Bayley-III. Nonetheless,
Bayley-II Mental Development Index between MLPT chil- the values of our present full-term control cohort are compa-
dren and term-born controls using a sample from a single cen- rable to data from contemporary term-born controls as-
ter. The population-based Early Childhood Longitudinal Sur- sessed using the Bayley-III.19,23 Previous studies have simi-
vey–Birth Cohort study reported a mean t statistic difference larly reported higher rates of developmental delay in MLPT
of −1.4 (95% CI, −2.7 to −0.2) on the Bayley mental score using children than in controls in this age group. Using a parent ques-
a short form of the Bayley-II modified specifically for that tionnaire, a large regional cohort from the United Kingdom had

4/7 JAMA Pediatrics April 2017 Volume 171, Number 4 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 10/20/2018


Association Between Preterm Birth and Neurodevelopment, Social-Emotional Development at 2 Years Original Investigation Research

Table 2. Two-Year Outcomes Compared Between the Birth Groups

Mean (SD)
MLPT Group Term Group Unadjusted Mean Adjusted Mean Adjusted Adjusted
Variable (n = 198) (n = 183) Difference (95% CI) βa Differenceb (95% CI) βa,b P Valueb
Neurodevelopment
Cognitive 102.5 (13.9) [n = 197] 109.1 (13.7) [n = 181] −6.3 (−9.2 to −3.4) −0.23 −5.3 (−8.2 to −2.4) −0.20 <.001
composite
Language 99.1 (17.7) [n = 190] 111.3 (16.6) [n = 178] −11.8 (−15.5 to −8.2) −0.34 −11.4 (−15.3 to −7.5) −0.31 <.001
composite
Receptive 10.1 (3.3) [n = 190] 12.2 (3.2) [n = 178] −2.0 (−2.7 to −1.4) −0.31 −1.9 (−2.6 to −1.2) −0.29 <.001
language
standard score
Expressive 9.5 (3.2) [n = 191] 11.6 (2.9) [n = 178] −2.0 (−2.7 to −1.4) −0.32 −1.9 (−2.6 to −1.2) −0.30 <.001
language
standard score
Motor composite 103.5 (16.0) [n = 194] 111.5 (15.2) [n = 177] −7.9 (−11.2 to −4.7) −0.25 −7.3 (−10.6 to −3.9) −0.23 <.001
Fine motor 11.8 (2.9) [n = 196] 13.2 (3.0) [n = 181] −1.4 (−2.0 to −0.8) −0.23 −1.2 (−1.8 to −0.6) −0.21 <.001
standard score
Gross motor 9.4 (2.9) [n = 194] 10.5 (3.0) [n = 177] −1.1 (−1.7 to −0.5) −0.19 −1.1 (−1.7 to −0.4) −0.18 <.001
standard score
Social-Emotional Development
Externalizing 47.8 (9.0) [n = 176] 47.3 (7.8) [n = 151] 0.5 (−1.4 to 2.4) 0.02 −0.1 (−1.9 to 1.8) −0.01 .95
behavior
Internalizing 45.7 (10.4) [n = 176] 45.2 (9.3) [n = 151] 0.6 (−1.6 to 2.8) 0.03 0.9 (−1.3 to 3.2) 0.04 .42
behavior
Dysregulation 44.0 (11.8) [n = 176] 44.0 (10.7) [n = 151] 0.2 (−2.4 to 2.7) −0.01 −0.1 (−2.6 to 2.6) −0.01 .99
Social-emotional 48.3 (10.7) [n = 176] 52.5 (8.4) [n = 150] −4.0 (−6.1 to −1.8) −0.21 −3.6 (−5.8 to −1.4) −0.20 .001
competence

Abbreviation: MLPT, moderate and late preterm.


a
Standardized regression coefficient.
b
Adjusted for social risk.

Table 3. Developmental Delay and At-Risk Social-Emotional Development Compared Between the Birth Groups

No./Total No. (%) Odds Ratio (95% CI)


MLPT Group Term Group Adjusted
Variable (n = 198) (n = 183) Unadjusted Adjusteda P Valuea
Neurodevelopment
Cognitive delay
Less than −1 SD 63/197 (32.0) 34/181 (18.8) 2.0 (1.2-3.3) 1.8 (1.1-3.0) .03
Less than −2 SDs 11/197 (5.6) 4/181 (2.2) 2.5 (0.8-8.3) 1.9 (0.6-6.6) .30
Language delay
Less than −1 SD 85/190 (44.7) 34/178 (19.1) 3.3 (2.0-5.4) 3.1 (1.8-5.2) <.001
Less than −2 SDs 26/190 (13.7) 6/178 (3.4) 4.3 (1.7-10.9) 3.9 (1.5-10.2) .005
Motor delay
Less than −1 SD 49/194 (25.3) 20/177 (11.3) 2.7 (1.5-4.8) 2.4 (1.3-4.5) .004
Less than −2 SDs 13/194 (6.7) 1/177 (0.6) 12.3 (1.6-95.9) 9.5 (1.2-74.4) .03
Social-Emotional Development
Externalizing behavior 8/176 (4.5) 3/151 (2.0) 2.3 (0.6-9.3) 1.6 (0.4-6.6) .49
Internalizing behavior 8/176 (4.5) 4/151 (2.6) 1.8 (0.5-6.0) 2.2 (0.5-8.4) .27
Dysregulation 10/176 (5.7) 7/151 (4.6) 1.3 (0.5-3.7) 1.1 (0.4-3.1) .82 Abbreviation: MLPT, moderate and
Social-emotional 23/176 (13.1) 5/150 (3.3) 4.4 (1.6-11.9) 3.9 (1.4-10.9) .008 late preterm.
competence a
Adjusted for social risk.

an adjusted relative risk of 2.09 (95% CI, 1.19-3.64) for cogni- compared with term-born children for both cognitive (mild de-
tive impairment in MLPT children compared with term-born lay OR, 1.43; 95% CI, 1.22-1.67) and motor (mild delay OR, 1.58;
children at 2 years’ corrected age.5 If anything, this finding may 95% CI, 1.37-1.83) domains.8 Our findings of higher ORs may
be an underestimation of the risk of delay because the fol- be accounted for by differences in samples, assessment tools,
low-up rates were only approximately 60%. The Early Child- and definitions of delay.
hood Longitudinal Survey–Birth Cohort had increased odds of Social and emotional behavioral problems in early
developmental delay at 24 months in late preterm children childhood24 and at school age25,26 have been described in the

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics April 2017 Volume 171, Number 4 5/7

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 10/20/2018


Research Original Investigation Association Between Preterm Birth and Neurodevelopment, Social-Emotional Development at 2 Years

very preterm population. Early identification of these prob- work Neurobehavioral Scale, at term-equivalent age were as-
lems is important because it is increasingly recognized that they sociated with developmental delay at 2 years of age in MLPT
are associated with later psychopathological conditions.27,28 infants.35 Further research into neurobehavioral deficits as-
Although data on MLPT children are scarce, there are reports sociated with MLPT birth may provide markers for specific de-
of increased externalizing and internalizing problems in MLPT velopmental deficits in MLPT children.
children at preschool and early school age compared with term-
born controls.29,30 Recently, delays in social competence (but Strengths and Limitations
not other behavioral domains) were identified in MLPT chil- The strengths of our study are that we recruited a large, pro-
dren compared with term-born controls.10 Our results concur spective cohort and achieved high follow-up rates at age 2 years.
with this finding, highlighting the potential importance of so- We evaluated a range of developmental outcomes using di-
cial competence as a basis for longer-term developmental and rect criterion standard assessments in addition to parent-
behavioral problems described in school-age MLPT children. report questionnaires. However, we were limited by the inclu-
It is also possible that the parent-reported concerns about MLPT sion of participants from a single tertiary center, which may have
children’s social and behavioral competence are a reflection included sicker MLPT infants who were admitted to the neo-
of cognitive and language developmental delays in everyday natal nursery, especially those who were in the more mature
functioning. gestational age range within the MLPT group. This inclusion
The underlying brain basis for developmental problems in potentially limits the generalizability of our findings to MLPT
MLPT children is poorly understood. The MLPT period involves children in the wider population. However, our results are gen-
considerable growth and maturation of the brain. Increases in eralizable to the population of MLPT children who were more
brain volume, whole-brain weight, and gyral and sulcal devel- unwell and needed admission to a neonatal nursery after birth.
opment are substantial in this period of late gestation.31 There-
fore, there is potential for aberration of these processes after
MLPT birth. Our group has recently reported differences in brain
volumes, maturation, and microstructure at term-equivalent age
Conclusions
in MLPT infants compared with term-born controls.11,32 Larger In summary, we have presented evidence that MLPT birth is
volumes of total brain tissue, white matter, and cerebellum were associated with greater morbidities in neurodevelopment and
associated with better cognitive, language, and motor scores at social-emotional competence than term-born birth. This popu-
2 years’ corrected age in MLPT children.33 Moreover, altered neu- lation is a group of infants who traditionally receive little neu-
ral activity as measured using functional magnetic resonance rodevelopmental surveillance. While there is good evidence
imaging has been reported in the primary motor and sensory re- that early intervention is effective in improving cognitive out-
gions in MLPT children compared with term-born controls.34 comes for preterm children up to school age, further research
Collectively, there is accumulating evidence that alterations in is needed on effective intervention to improve language and
MLPT brain growth and development may underlie some of the motor outcomes specifically in the MLPT population.36 To pro-
deficits described in these children. vide developmental follow-up and intervention to this group,
In addition to neuroimaging findings, clinical assess- it will be vital to identify risk factors to target those at highest
ment tools in the neonatal period may also provide a valuable risk of developmental problems given the large numbers of
indication of later functioning in MLPT infants. For instance, MLPT births. Further research directions into potentially modi-
developmental delay in MLPT children may manifest at an ear- fiable factors, markers of poor outcome (eg, neuroimaging and
lier age as abnormal neurobehavior. In the same cohort, our neurobehavioral assessments in the newborn period), and the
group has recently shown that suboptimal neurological and spectrum of deficits at school age and older have the poten-
neurobehavioral assessments, including on the Hammer- tial to greatly improve the long-term care for this large group
smith Neonatal Neurological Examination and the NICU Net- of children.

ARTICLE INFORMATION Institute, Melbourne, Australia (Lee); Paediatric Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data:
Accepted for Publication: November 30, 2016. Infant Perinatal Emergency Retrieval, The Royal Cheong, Doyle, Burnett, Lee, Walsh, Potter,
Children’s Hospital Melbourne, Melbourne, Treyvaud, Thompson, Olsen, Anderson.
Published Online: February 6, 2017. Australia (Walsh); Department of Psychology and Drafting of the manuscript: Cheong, Burnett,
doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.4805 Counselling, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Treyvaud.
Author Affiliations: Neonatal Services, Royal Australia (Treyvaud); Developmental Imaging, Critical revision of the manuscript for important
Women’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia (Cheong, Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Melbourne, intellectual content: All authors.
Doyle, Burnett, Walsh, Olsen, Spittle); Victorian Australia (Thompson); Florey Institute of Statistical analysis: Cheong, Doyle, Lee.
Infant Brain Studies, Murdoch Childrens Research Neuroscience & Mental Health, Melbourne, Obtained funding: Cheong, Thompson, Anderson,
Institute, Melbourne, Australia (Cheong, Doyle, Australia (Thompson); Department of Spittle.
Burnett, Walsh, Potter, Treyvaud, Thompson, Physiotherapy, University of Melbourne, Administrative, technical, or material support:
Olsen, Anderson, Spittle); Department of Obstetrics Melbourne, Australia (Spittle). Cheong, Walsh, Potter, Treyvaud, Thompson, Olsen.
and Gynaecology, University of Melbourne, Author Contributions: Dr Cheong had full access Study supervision: Walsh, Treyvaud, Anderson,
Melbourne, Australia (Cheong, Doyle, Walsh); to all the data in the study and takes responsibility Spittle.
Department of Paediatrics, University of for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia (Doyle, Burnett, data analysis.
Lee, Thompson, Anderson); Clinical Epidemiology Funding/Support: This work was supported by the
Study concept and design: Cheong, Doyle, following grants from the Australian National
and Biostatistics Unit, Murdoch Childrens Research Treyvaud, Anderson, Spittle.

6/7 JAMA Pediatrics April 2017 Volume 171, Number 4 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 10/20/2018


Association Between Preterm Birth and Neurodevelopment, Social-Emotional Development at 2 Years Original Investigation Research

Health and Medical Research Council: project born late and moderately preterm. J Dev Behav 25. Anderson P, Doyle LW; Victorian Infant
grants 1028822 and 1034516, Centre of Research Pediatr. 2015;36(9):690-699. Collaborative Study Group. Neurobehavioral
Excellence grant 1060733, Early Career Fellowship 11. Walsh JM, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, Lee KJ, outcomes of school-age children born extremely
grant 1053787 (Dr Cheong), Senior Research Cheong JL. Moderate and late preterm birth: effect low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s. JAMA.
Fellowship grant 1081288 (Dr Anderson), Career on brain size and maturation at term-equivalent 2003;289(24):3264-3272.
Development Fellowship grant 1085754 (Dr age. Radiology. 2014;273(1):232-240. 26. Arpi E, Ferrari F. Preterm birth and behaviour
Thompson), grant 1053609 (Dr Lee), and grant problems in infants and preschool-age children:
1108714 (Dr Spittle). This study was also supported 12. Roberts G, Howard K, Spittle AJ, Brown NC,
Anderson PJ, Doyle LW. Rates of early intervention a review of the recent literature. Dev Med Child
by The Royal Children’s Hospital Foundation and by Neurol. 2013;55(9):788-796.
the Victorian Government’s Operational services in very preterm children with
Infrastructure Support Program. developmental disabilities at age 2 years. J Paediatr 27. Treyvaud K, Doyle LW, Lee KJ, et al.
Child Health. 2008;44(5):276-280. Social-emotional difficulties in very preterm and
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding sources term 2 year olds predict specific social-emotional
had no role in the design and conduct of the study; 13. Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, Russell D,
Wood E, Galuppi B. Development and reliability of a problems at the age of 5 years. J Pediatr Psychol.
collection, management, analysis, and 2012;37(7):779-785.
interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or system to classify gross motor function in children
approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1997; 28. Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS. Social-emotional
the manuscript for publication. 39(4):214-223. screening status in early childhood predicts
14. Bayley N. The Bayley Scales of Infant Development. elementary school outcomes. Pediatrics. 2008;121
Additional Contributions: We thank the research (5):957-962.
coordinators, families, and children for their 3rd ed. New York, NY: Psychological Corp; 2005.
participation in this study. 15. Anderson PJ, De Luca CR, Hutchinson E, 29. Potijk MR, de Winter AF, Bos AF, Kerstjens JM,
Roberts G, Doyle LW; Victorian Infant Collaborative Reijneveld SA. Co-occurrence of developmental
REFERENCES Group. Underestimation of developmental delay by and behavioural problems in moderate to late
the new Bayley-III scale. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. preterm-born children. Arch Dis Child. 2016;101(3):
1. Saigal S, Doyle LW. An overview of mortality and 217-222.
sequelae of preterm birth from infancy to 2010;164(4):352-356.
adulthood. Lancet. 2008;371(9608):261-269. 16. Anderson PJ, Burnett A. Assessing 30. Talge NM, Holzman C, Wang J, Lucia V,
developmental delay in early childhood: concerns Gardiner J, Breslau N. Late-preterm birth and its
2. Cheong JL, Doyle LW. Increasing rates of association with cognitive and socioemotional
prematurity and epidemiology of late preterm with the Bayley-III scales [published online August
12, 2016]. Clin Neuropsychol. outcomes at 6 years of age. Pediatrics. 2010;126(6):
birth. J Paediatr Child Health. 2012;48(9):784-788. 1124-1131.
3. Chan E, Quigley MA. School performance at age 17. Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ. Infant Toddler
Social Emotional Assessment (ITSEA). New York, NY: 31. Kinney HC. The near-term (late preterm)
7 years in late preterm and early term birth: human brain and risk for periventricular
a cohort study. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. Psychological Corp; 2005.
leukomalacia: a review. Semin Perinatol. 2006;30
2014;99(6):F451-F457. 18. Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Jones SM, Little (2):81-88.
4. Woythaler M, McCormick MC, Mao WY, Smith TD. The Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional
Assessment (ITSEA): factor structure, reliability, 32. Kelly CE, Cheong JL, Gabra Fam L, et al.
VC. Late preterm infants and neurodevelopmental Moderate and late preterm infants exhibit
outcomes at kindergarten. Pediatrics. 2015;136 and validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31(5):
495-514. widespread brain white matter microstructure
(3):424-431. alterations at term-equivalent age relative to
5. Johnson S, Evans TA, Draper ES, et al. 19. Doyle LW, Roberts G, Anderson PJ; Victorian term-born controls. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016;10
Neurodevelopmental outcomes following late and Infant Collaborative Study Group. Outcomes at age (1):41-49.
moderate prematurity: a population-based cohort 2 years of infants < 28 weeks’ gestational age born
in Victoria in 2005. J Pediatr. 2010;156(1):49-53.e1. 33. Cheong JL, Thompson DK, Spittle AJ, et al.
study. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2015;100 Brain volumes at term-equivalent age are
(4):F301-F308. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.07.013
associated with 2-year neurodevelopment in
6. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. 20. Chyi LJ, Lee HC, Hintz SR, Gould JB, Sutcliffe moderate and late preterm children. J Pediatr.
Australia’s Mothers and Babies 2013: In Brief. TL. School outcomes of late preterm infants: special 2016;174:91-97.e1. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.04.002
Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare; needs and challenges for infants born at 32 to 36
weeks gestation. J Pediatr. 2008;153(1):25-31. 34. Wu X, Wei L, Wang N, et al. Frequency of
December 14, 2015. Perinatal Statistics Series 31. spontaneous BOLD signal differences between
Catalog PER 72. 21. Huddy CLJ, Johnson A, Hope PL. Educational moderate and late preterm newborns and term
7. Hamilton BE, Martin JA, Osterman MJ. Births: and behavioural problems in babies of 32-35 weeks newborns. Neurotox Res. 2016;30(3):539-551.
Preliminary Data for 2015. Natl Vital Stat Rep. 2016; gestation. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2001;85
(1):F23-F28. 35. Spittle AJ, Walsh JM, Potter C, et al.
65(3):1-15. Neurobehaviour at term-equivalent age and
8. Woythaler MA, McCormick MC, Smith VC. Late 22. Nepomnyaschy L, Hegyi T, Ostfeld BM, neurodevelopmental outcomes at 2 years in infants
preterm infants have worse 24-month Reichman NE. Developmental outcomes of born moderate-to-late preterm. Dev Med Child
neurodevelopmental outcomes than term infants. late-preterm infants at 2 and 4 years. Matern Child Neurol. 2017;59(2):207-215.
Pediatrics. 2011;127(3):e622-e629. doi: Health J. 2012;16(8):1612-1624.
36. Spittle A, Orton J, Anderson PJ, Boyd R, Doyle
10.1542/peds.2009-3598 23. Serenius F, Källén K, Blennow M, et al; EXPRESS LW. Early developmental intervention programmes
9. Voigt B, Pietz J, Pauen S, Kliegel M, Reuner G. Group. Neurodevelopmental outcome in extremely provided post hospital discharge to prevent motor
Cognitive development in very vs. moderately to preterm infants at 2.5 years after active perinatal and cognitive impairment in preterm infants.
late preterm and full-term children: can effortful care in Sweden. JAMA. 2013;309(17):1810-1820. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015;(11):CD005495.
control account for group differences in 24. Spittle AJ, Treyvaud K, Doyle LW, et al. Early
toddlerhood? Early Hum Dev. 2012;88(5):307-313. emergence of behavior and social-emotional
10. Johnson S, Matthews R, Draper ES, et al. Early problems in very preterm infants. J Am Acad Child
emergence of delayed social competence in infants Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(9):909-918.

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics April 2017 Volume 171, Number 4 7/7

Copyright 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: on 10/20/2018

You might also like