You are on page 1of 10

602266

research-article2015
HEBXXX10.1177/1090198115602266Health Education & BehaviorParent and Alquist

Article

Health Education & Behavior

Born Fat: The Relations Between 2016, Vol. 43(3) 337­–346


© 2015 Society for Public
Health Education
Weight Changeability Beliefs and Health Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Behaviors and Physical Health DOI: 10.1177/1090198115602266


heb.sagepub.com

Mike C. Parent, PhD1, and Jessica L. Alquist, PhD1

Abstract
Although some popular press and nonscholarly sources have claimed that weight is largely unchangeable, the relationship
between this belief and objective measures of health remains unclear. We tested the hypothesis that people who believe
weight is unchangeable will have poorer objective and subjective health, and fewer exercise behaviors and poorer eating
habits, than people who believe weight is changeable. Participants were 4,166 men and 4,655 women enrolled in the National
Health and Nutrition Examination Survey in the 2007 to 2010 iterations. Believing that weight was uncontrollable was
negatively related to exercise and healthful dietary practices and positively related to unhealthful eating. Lack of exercise
and unhealthful eating were, in turn, associated with poor physical health. Age, but not gender, moderated the relationships
between belief in weight changeability and exercise behaviors, healthful eating, and unhealthful eating. This study suggests that
believing weight is unchangeable is associated with poor health behaviors and poorer physical health.

Keywords
body weight, eating, exercise, health behavior

Rising rates of obesity are one of the most important public the “Healthy at Every Size” movement (Bacon, 2008; Bacon
health concerns in the United States (Flegal, Carroll, Kit, & & Aphamor, 2011; O’Hara & Gregg, 2010; Rothblum, 2014).
Ogden, 2012; Panzer, 2006; Throp, Owen, Neuhaus, & However, belief that weight is not controllable may be asso-
Dunstan, 2011; Y. C. Wang, McPherson, Marsh, Gortmaker, ciated with decreases in healthful behaviors and increases in
& Brown, 2011; White House Task Force on Childhood unhealthful behaviors. The present study tested the hypothe-
Obesity Report to the President, 2010) and abroad (Karnik & sis that believing that weight is changeable will be associated
Kanekar, 2012; Y. C. Wang et al., 2011; Withrow & Alter, with better physical health and that this relationship will be
2011). Obesity and obesity-predictive behaviors, such as mediated by more healthful dietary practices, fewer unhealth-
sedentary lifestyles and poor dietary decisions, are associ- ful dietary practices, and more exercise behaviors.
ated with health conditions including type 2 diabetes, cardio- Because of the rise in popular books and online forums
vascular disease, some types of cancer, back pain, depression, (Dean, 2013) claiming that weight is primarily genetic, that
body image disturbance, and sleep disorders (Akinnusi, individuals can do little to influence weight, and that dieting
Saliba, Porhomayon, & El-Solh, 2012; Boutelle, Hannan, and exercise are ineffective for controlling weight, some
Fulkerson, Crow, & Stice, 2010; Coates, Slattery, Potter, individuals may be skeptical about recommendations to
Quesenberry, & Edwards, 1998; Schwartz & Brownell, engage in behaviors that would facilitate weight loss. It is
2004; Shiri, Karppinen, Leino-Arjas, Solovieva, & Viikari- possible that such beliefs may be negatively related to health
Juntura, 2010; Y. C. Wang et al., 2011). Whether a person behaviors (such as healthful dietary practices and exercise
engages in health-promoting behaviors and maintains physi- behaviors); if an individual believes weight to be outside of
cal health can be affected by a variety of factors, including the influence of diet and exercise, she or he may engage in
their beliefs about weight (Burnette, 2010; Clark, Abrams, more behaviors that are rewarding in the short term (e.g.,
Niaura, Eaton, & Rossi, 1991; Kitsantas, 2000; Linde,
Rothman, Baldwin, & Jeffry, 2006; Saltzer, 1982). Some 1
Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
writers in the field of obesity and health work have advo-
Corresponding Author:
cated that body weight is overwhelmingly determined by Mike C. Parent, PhD, Texas Tech University, MS 2051, Lubbock, TX
genetic influence and that individuals have little to no control 79409, USA.
over their weight; this position is especially prominent within Email: michael.parent@ttu.edu
338 Health Education & Behavior 43(3)

eating unhealthful foods, avoiding exercise) rather than 1998; Pollard, Steptoe, & Wardle, 1998). Most relevant to the
healthful behaviors with more long-term benefits for weight present study, a recent article identified implicit theories of
management. Although health care providers’ advice to lose weight as a significant predictor of weight loss intentions and
weight positively influences patient health behaviors and behavior (Burnette, 2010). Participants who were randomly
facilitate weight loss (Rose, Poynter, Anderson, Noar, & assigned to read an article claiming that weight was unchange-
Conigliaro, 2013), patients’ beliefs in the unchangability of able reported less intention to exercise than participants who
weight may act to decrease the influence of expert advice or read an article claiming that weight was changeable.
evidence contrary to their beliefs (Davies, 1997), akin to Furthermore, in a study of participants who expressed interest
self-efficacy beliefs within the health belief model in losing weight, having an entity theory of body weight (as
(Rosenstock, Strecher, & Becker, 1988; Strecher, DeVellis, measured by items such as “You have a certain body weight,
Becker, & Rosenstock, 1986). and you can’t really do much to change it”) was associated
with lower expectations of weight loss success, more avoidant
coping when faced with setbacks in their weight loss, and, ulti-
Implicit Theories
mately, less weight loss, even when controlling for nutrition
Previous research has shown that whether people believe a self-efficacy and exercise self-efficacy. This study provided
particular attribute is changeable has extensive implications evidence that believing weight is unchangeable was associated
for goal pursuit in that domain. Belief in the changeability of with decreased intentions to engage in healthy behaviors,
a particular attribute is referred to as an implicit theory that is lower beliefs in one’s ability to maintain diet and exercise rou-
either incremental (the attribute is changeable) or entity (the tines, and ultimately decreased success in weight loss plans.
attribute is unchangeable; for a review, see Dweck, Chiu, & This research laid important groundwork on implicit theories
Hong, 1995). People who believe they can increase their of weight and health behaviors.
intelligence (incremental theorists) are more likely to attri-
bute failure to lack of effort, are more likely to continue to try
to improve after failure, and ultimately improve more over
The Present Study
time compared with people who believe intelligence is fixed The present study was designed to build on previous research
(entity theorists; Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007; on implicit theories of weight by using objectively measured
Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin, & Wan, 1999). weight, measuring additional health variables, and testing for
Research on implicit theories of physical ability has shown moderation by age and gender. Because previous research on
that people who believe physical ability is malleable have implicit theories of weight relied entirely on self-reported
lower anxiety, have higher self-efficacy, and engage in less weight, it remains possible that participants who believed
self-handicapping when completing physical activities than weight was changeable felt greater pressure to report weight
people who believe physical ability is fixed (Ommundsen, loss (regardless of actual weight loss) than participants who
2001a, 2001b; C. K. J. Wang, Chatzisarantis, Spray, & Biddle, believed weight was out of their control. Because self-reported
2002). This provides evidence that incremental theories can weight is sometimes inaccurate (Gorber, Tremblay, Moher, &
have positive effects for physical abilities. Gorber, 2007) and participants may be motivated to provide
Although having an incremental theory (whether measured information that confirms their beliefs, we sought to test the
or manipulated) is often related to high self-efficacy (Komarraju relationship between beliefs about weight changeability and
& Nadler, 2013; Martocchio, 1994; Taberno & Wood, 1999), investigator-measured body mass index (BMI). We further
implicit theories are distinct from self-efficacy and behavioral extended previous research by including other health mea-
control. Implicit theories reflect participants’ beliefs about the sures in addition to BMI (fasting blood glucose and subjective
malleability of a particular outcome (intelligence, weight), ratings of health). In addition, we sought to extend previous
whereas self-efficacy and behavioral control reflect partici- findings on the relationship between weight changeability
pants’ beliefs about their ability to enact particular behaviors beliefs and health by testing gender and age as moderators in a
(Ajzen & Madden, 1986; Bandura, 1977). Individuals could large, nationally representative sample. Some research has
believe that they could change their eating habits if they wanted, suggested that these two factors may affect weight-related
but believe that weight is unchangeable. behaviors; for example, women are more likely than men to
report weight control as a motivation for exercise (Furnham,
Badmin, & Sneade, 2002; Kilpatrick, Hebert, & Bartholomew,
Implicit Theories of Weight and Health Behaviors 2005; K. McDonald & Thompson, 1992), and weight as a
Because implicit theories affect goal-directed behavior, beliefs motivation for exercise has been found to decrease with age
about the changeability of weight likely play a role in whether (Davis, Fox, Brewer, & Ratusny, 1995; Tiggemann, 2004).
people engage in healthful behaviors. Controlling weight is The present research sought to test whether gender and age
one reason that people engage in physical activity and avoid moderate the relationship between weight implicit theories
unhealthful foods (Fotopoulos, Krystallis, Vassallo, & and health or if the relationships between weight implicit theo-
Pagiaslis, 2009; Glanz, Basil, Maibach, Goldberg, & Snyder, ries and health are consistent across various genders and ages.
Parent and Alquist 339

This study was designed to test the hypothesis that believ- Table 1.  Demographic Data (N = 8,821).
ing weight is changeable will be related to better health (as
Characteristic Data
indicated by BMI, fasting glucose levels, and subjective rat-
ings of health) in a large nationally representative sample. Categorical variables  
Furthermore, we predicted that the relationship between  Sex  
fixed weight beliefs and physical health would be mediated   Male 47.47%
by healthful dietary practices, unhealthful dietary practices,   Female 52.53%
and exercise behaviors. We also assessed for moderation of  Race/ethnicity  
this relationship by gender and age.   White 70.14%
  Non-Hispanic Black 11.43%
  Mexican American 8.19%
Method   Other—Hispanic 4.77%
  Other 5.46%
Participants   Physical activity  
Participants were 4,166 men and 4,655 women enrolled in the   Moderate (yes) 46.05%
National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey   Vigorous (yes) 25.47%
  Walk/bike (yes) 24.49%
(NHANES) in the 2007-2008 and 2009-2010 iterations (dif-
  Blood glucose  
ferent participants were enrolled in the separate iterations and
  Low 0.29%
thus the data are cross-sectional and not longitudinal).
  Normal 51.82%
NHANES is an annual national study of health in the United
  Prediabetes 39.35%
States, and the data are publically available for researchers   Diabetes-indicating 8.54%
(Centers for Disease Control, 2007, 2009). For each location Continuous variablesa  
sampled, local government members were notified about the  Age 46.78 (17.05)
upcoming data collections and residents of the area were sent   Beliefs about weight 3.64 (1.73)
letters notifying them about the study. Potential participants   Healthful eating  
were then interviewed briefly at their homes to determine eli-   Nutrition facts 2.80 (1.46)
gibility. Participation was facilitated by transportation assis-   Fruit 1.48 (1.46)
tance to the testing center, financial compensation, and a   DG veg 1.76 (1.71)
personalized report of medical findings from individual tests.   Unhealthful eating  
The unweighted response rates were 78.4% (interview) and   Meals out 3.80 (6.23)
75.4% (examination) for 2007-2008 and 79.4% (interview)   R2E meals 1.75 (5.50)
and 77.3% (examination) for 2009-2010. The current analy-   Frozen 2.77 (10.74)
ses were conducted using participants aged 18 to 79 years.  BMI 28.77 (10.38)
Pregnant participants were excluded from the analysis.   Days health was bad 0.89 (3.11)
Demographic data for the sample are presented in Table 1.   General health condition 2.61 (1.98)

Note. DG veg = dark green vegetables; R2E meals = ready-to-eat meals;


BMI = body mass index.
Procedures a
Data for continuous variables are presented as mean (standard
deviation).
Participants completed informed consent procedures admin-
istered by the NHANES research team. Computer-assisted
interviews were conducted in participant homes. Participants significantly older than 80, only data from participants up to
were asked each question by an interviewer, and their age 79 were used in the present study.
responses were immediately logged on a computer. After the
interview was complete, participants were asked to schedule Beliefs About Weight. Participants were asked to indicate
an appointment to visit the mobile testing center where health their agreement with the statement, “Some people are born
measurements (e.g., height and weight measurement, blood to be fat and some thin; there is not much you can do to
draws) were completed. change this” on a scale of 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly
disagree). In the present study, the coding of this item was
reversed such that higher scores indicated greater agree-
Instruments ment with the item (i.e., higher scores indicated greater
Demographic Moderators.  Gender was assessed via a single, entity beliefs).
forced choice item between “male” and “female.” Age was
assessed with a forced response for age, though individuals Physical Activity.  Participants were asked to indicate (with a
80 years and older were coded as 80 in the NHANES data “yes” or “no”) whether they (1) engage in vigorous intensity
set. Because participants coded as 80 for age may be recreational activities (defined in the survey as “any
340 Health Education & Behavior 43(3)

vigorous-intensity sports, fitness, or recreational activities estimation to account for missing data under normal method-
that cause large increases in breathing or heart rate like run- ology for using NHANES data (Centers for Disease Control,
ning or basketball for at least 10 minutes continuously”); (2) 2013). The model was run using COMPLEX model type,
engage in moderate-intensity recreational activities (defined required for large survey data with weights, strata, and clus-
in the survey as “any moderate-intensity sports, fitness, or ters. Sampling weights, strata, and clusters from the NHANES
recreational activities that cause a small increase in breathing data set were included in analyses as per requirements of
or heart rate such as brisk walking, bicycling, swimming, or using large national data sets in general, and NHANES data
golf for at least 10 minutes continuously”); and (2) walk or in particular. Sample sizes required for structural equation
bicycle for at least 10 minutes each day. Higher values indi- modeling are generally accepted to be at least 200 partici-
cate greater physical activity. pants (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007) and thus the present sam-
ple exceeded minimum sample size requirements.
Healthful Eating. Participants were asked to indicate, on a
scale of 1 (always) to 5 (never), how often they (1) use the
nutrition facts panel on food, (2) have fruits available at
Results
home, and (3) have dark green vegetables available at home. Consistent with recommendations for conducting structural
For the present study, values were recoded so that higher val- equation modeling (R. P. McDonald & Ho, 2002; Weston &
ues indicated greater healthful eating. Gore, 2006), we first tested a measurement model by con-
straining indicators to load onto intended latent variables,
Unhealthful Eating.  Participants were asked to indicate how and freeing all covariances among latent variables and the
many “ready to eat foods” (e.g., deli foods) they ate in the born fat/thin item. This model was an acceptable fit to the
past 30 days, how often they ate frozen meals or frozen piz- data, χ2(68) = 411.07, p < .001, comparative fit index [CFI] =
zas in the past 30 days (ready-to-eat and frozen food con- .95, root mean square error of approximation [RMSEA] =
sumption were banded into units of 10s to improve normality .02. Indicators loaded into their intended latent constructs at
of the variables), and how many meals they ate away from p < .01, and covariances among latent constructs and the
home (e.g., at restaurants, fast food places, or food stands) in born fat/thin item were significant at p < .05.
the past 7 days. Higher scores indicate greater unhealthful Because the measurement model was an acceptable fit to
eating. the data, we tested the structural model (see Figure 1). This
model was a good fit to the data, χ2(69) = 441.23, p < .001, CFI =
Poor Health.  BMI was calculated based on height and weight .98, RMSEA = .03. Standardized path coefficients are pre-
as measured by a health technician. Height was measured sented in Figure 1, and unstandardized path coefficients, stan-
with a digital stadiometer; participants were asked to remove dard errors, and standardized path coefficients are presented in
their shoes. In the event that they refused to remove their Table 2. The model R2 for the latent health variable was 0.85.
shoes a correction was applied after measuring the height of We investigated the hypothesized moderators of the rela-
the shoe heel. Weight was measured by a health technician in tionship between the born fat/thin variable and the media-
pounds using a digital scale; participants in the mobile exam- tors. First, we assessed for a moderating influence of gender
ination center wore a disposable shirt, pants, and slippers; (Figure 2). An unconstrained model with gender as the
participants who refused to remove their shoes were coded as grouping variable indicated acceptable fit to the data,
missing; participants with limb amputations were coded as χ2(153) = 1016.42, p < .001, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .04. The
missing. To measure fasting blood glucose (grouped into ≤69 constrained model (with the three paths from the born fat/
mg/dL, 70-99 mg/dL, 100-126 mg/dL, and 126 mg/dL and thin variable to the three mediators constrained to equality
over; corresponding to recommendations for low, normal, between groups) was also a good fit to the data, χ2(156) =
prediabetes, and diabetes-indicating levels; American Diabe- 1015.21, p < .001, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .04; the models did
tes Association, 2011), a health technician collected blood not differ, χ2(3) = 1.21, ns, indicating that there were no gen-
from participants after a 9-hour fast. Subjective health was der differences among those three paths. Unstandardized
measured using two questions: the number of days their path coefficients, standard errors, and standardized path
health was not good with regard to physical illness or injuries coefficients for the two groups are presented in Table 2.
in the past 30 days (banded into units of 5 days to improve Next, we tested for a moderating influence of age (Figure 3).
normality of the variable) and their general health on a scale Because age was a continuous variable, an interaction term
of 1 (excellent) to 5 (poor). Higher scores on this variable approach was used. This model was an acceptable fit to the
indicate poorer health. data, χ2(89) = 672.31, p < .001, CFI = .93, RMSEA = .03.
Age significantly predicted exercise (B = −0.03, SE = 0.00,
β = −0.61, p < .001), healthful eating (B = 0.00, SE = 0.00,
Analysis β = −0.17, p < .001), and unhealthful eating (B = −0.04,
Analyses were conducted in Mplus, Version 6 (Muthén & SE = 0.00, β = −0.39, p < .001). Age was associated nega-
Muthén, 2010) using full information maximum likelihood tively with exercise behaviors, unhealthful eating, and
Parent and Alquist 341

Vig. rec. Mod. rec. Walk/bike

.64** .43** .18**

Exercise
-.99**
-.17** -.31**

Meals out R2E meals Glucose


.27**
.49** .17** .07**

Unhealthful Poor .48**


Born fat/thin BMI
.05** Eang .29** Health
.36**
.36**

Frozen .42** Days


-.07**

-.14** .65**
Healthful Gen. health
Eang

.32** .60** .67**

Nut. facts Fruit DG veg.

Figure 1.  Structural equation model.


Note. Vig. rec. = vigorous recreational activity; Mod. rec. = moderate recreational activity; Walk/bike = walking or biking for at least 10 minutes each day;
Meals out = meals eaten away from home; R2E meals = ready-to-eat meals; Frozen = frozen meals/pizza; Nut. Facts. = use of nutritional panels on food;
Fruit = availability of fruit at home; DG veg. = availability of dark green vegetables at home; Days = subjective health (days health was not good); Gen.
health = subjective health (general rating of health).

Table 2.  Unstandardized, Standardized, and Significance Levels for Models.

Parameter estimate B (SE) β p


Baseline model  
  Born fat/thin → Unhealthful eating 0.07 (0.004) 0.05 <.001
  Born fat/thin → Healthful eating −0.02 (0.001) −0.07 <.001
  Born fat/thin → Exercise −0.08 (0.002) −0.17 <.001
  Unhealthful eating → Poor health 0.03 (0.001) 0.29 <.001
  Healthful eating → Poor health −0.07 (0.011) −0.14 <.001
 Exercise → Poor health −0.27 (0.003) −0.99 <.001
Moderated paths—Gender  
  Born fat/thin → Unhealthful eatingmen 0.08 (0.006) 0.06 <.001
  Born fat/thin → Healthful eatingMEN −0.01 (0.002) −0.04 <.001
  Born fat/thin → ExerciseMEN −0.07 (0.002) −0.14 <.001
  Born fat/thin → Unhealthful eatingwomen 0.08 (0.006) 0.04 <.001
  Born fat/thin → Healthful eatingWOMEN −0.01 (0.001) −0.04 <.001
  Born fat/thin → ExerciseWOMEN −0.07 (0.002) −0.24 <.001
Moderated paths—Age  
  Born fat/thin → Unhealthful eating 0.17 (0.062) 0.12 <.01
  Born fat/thin → Healthful eating −0.00 (0.001) −0.01 ns
  Born fat/thin → Exercise −0.03 (0.014) −0.05 <.05
 Age → Unhealthful eating −0.04 (0.000) −0.39 <.001
 Age → Healthful eating 0.00 (0.000) 0.17 <.001
 Age → Exercise −0.03 (0.001) −0.61 <.001
  Born fat/thin × Age → Unhealthful eating 0.00 (0.001) −0.06 <.05
  Born fat/thin × Age → Healthful eating 0.00 (0.000) 0.08 <.001
  Born fat/thin × Age → Exercise 0.00 (0.000) 0.14 <.01
342 Health Education & Behavior 43(3)

Gender

Exercise

Unhealthful Poor
Born fat/thin
Eang Health

Healthful
Eang

Figure 2.  Moderation by gender.

Born fat/thin
X
Age

Exercise

Age

Unhealthful Poor
Born fat/thin
Eang Health

Healthful
Eang

Figure 3.  Moderation by age.

healthful eating. There was also a significant interaction those who had entity, rather than incremental, implicit theo-
between belief and age on exercise (B = 0.00, SE = 0.00, β = ries of weight. Among both younger and older participants,
0.14, p < .01), between belief and age on healthful eating unhealthful eating increased slightly with entity beliefs
(B = 0.00, SE = 0.00, β = −0.08 p < .001), and between belief though this association was weaker for older adults. Among
and age on unhealthful eating (B = 0.00, SE = 0.00, β = younger participants, healthful eating was not associated
−0.06, p < .05). Among younger participants, exercise behav- with incremental beliefs while among older participants,
iors remained stable (and high) across implicit theories; healthful eating was negatively associated with entity beliefs.
among older participants, exercise behaviors declined among Simple slopes of these relationships are presented in Figure 4
Parent and Alquist 343

Exercise, unhealthful eating, and healthful eating were, in turn,


2 associated with physical health. Furthermore, we found evi-
Younger dence that the relationship between changeability beliefs about
1.5
Older weight and exercise, healthful eating, and unhealthful eating dif-
1 fers by age.
Although previous research has found gender differ-
0.5
ences in weight as a motivation for exercise and healthful
Exercise

0 eating, we did not find evidence that gender moderated the


relationship between health beliefs and activity or healthful
-0.5 eating. Previous research has shown that gender differences
-1
in exercise and weight control motivation for exercise
decrease with age (Brunet & Sabiston, 2011; Davis et al.,
-1.5 1995). The discrepancy between past results and our find-
Entity Beliefs Incremental beliefs ings could be due, in part, to the fact that our sample is
somewhat older (M = 46.78; SD = 17.05) than studies that
2 have found gender differences in weight control as a moti-
vation for healthful behaviors, many of which focus on
1.5 Younger
adolescents or young adults (Furnham et  al., 2002;
Older
Kilpatrick et al., 2005; K. McDonald & Thompson, 1992).
Unhealthful food

1
We did find evidence of moderation of relationships by age,
0.5 such that holding entity theories (i.e., believing that weight
0
is not changeable) was associated with less exercise among
older participants and less healthful eating among younger
-0.5 participants, though incremental beliefs were associated
with greater unhealthful eating among younger and older
-1
participants, and less healthful eating among older partici-
-1.5 pants. Believing weight was unchangeable was generally
Entity beliefs Incremental beliefs associated with less healthy behaviors than believing
2 weight was changeable, though some moderation was
observed with age.
1.5
Our research builds on previous work on the relationship
1 between changeability beliefs about weight and weight loss
Healthful food

in a number of ways. First, previous work on the relationship


0.5 between weight changeability beliefs and diet and exercise
0 has used self-report measures of weight (e.g., Burnette,
2010). Because people are often inaccurate when self-report-
-0.5 ing their weight and height (Gorber et al., 2007), the present
Younger
study used height and weight as measured by the NHANES
-1 Older
study personnel rather than self-reported weight. Our study
-1.5 also measured health by assessing factors other than weight:
Entity beliefs Incremental beliefs fasting glucose and subjective health. By using the NHANES
data set, we were able to test the relationship between
changeability beliefs and health using a large, nationally rep-
Figure 4. Interactions.
resentative sample. This research also builds on previous
research by showing that the effects of beliefs about weight
(simple slopes were defined using the standard procedure of may differ with age.
assessing values at one standard deviation about the means). The results of the present study must be interpreted in
Unstandardized path coefficients, standard errors, and stan- light of its limitations. First, the present study is cross-sec-
dardized path coefficients are presented in Table 2. tional, and longitudinal research on the relationship between
implicit theories of weight, health behaviors, and physical
health is needed. Second, although the present study used
Discussion
some objective measures of health (investigator-measured
The present study provided evidence that believing weight was BMI and blood glucose levels), exercise, healthful eating,
unchangeable was negatively related to exercise and healthful and unhealthful eating were assessed with self-report and
dietary practices, and positively related to unhealthful eating. thus may be open to incorrect responding, or inaccurate
344 Health Education & Behavior 43(3)

perception, by participants. Finally, the present study Journal of Internal Medicine, 23, 219-226. doi:10.1016/j.
assessed implicit theories of weight with only a single item. ejim.2011.10.016
In future research, researchers may want to combine current American Diabetes Association. (2011). Standards of medical care
scales designed to measure implicit theories of weight in diabetes—2011. Diabetes Care, 34, S11-S61. doi:10.2337/
dc11-S011
(Burnette, 2010) with objective measures of health.
Bacon, L. (2008). Health at every size: The surprising truth about
Despite the limitations of this study, it does have implica-
your weight. Dallas, TX: BenBella Books.
tions for research and clinical work. Although the present Bacon, L., & Aphamor, L. (2011). Weight science: Evaluating the
research does not determine causal direction between health evidence for a paradigm shift. Nutrition Journal, 10, 1-13.
beliefs and behaviors, it builds on previous experimental doi:10.1186/1475-2891-10-9
research showing participants who are given information Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
supporting an entity theory of weight report being more behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
likely to quit dieting after a setback than participants who Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007).
are given information supporting an incremental theory Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an
(Burnette, 2010). Experimental studies might build on this adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention.
work by giving participants materials supporting either Child Development, 78, 246-263.
Boutelle, K. N., Hannan, P. J., Fulkerson, J. A., Crow, S. J., &
incremental or entity implicit theories, and assess the impact
Stice, E. (2010). Obesity as a prospective indicator or depres-
of such manipulations on health behaviors such as exercise
sion in adolescent females. Health Psychology, 29, 293-298.
persistence or food choice. Another way to improve health doi:10.1037/a0018645
may be to focus on whether people with motivations other Brunet, J., & Sabiston, C. M. (2011). Exploring motivation for
than weight management to exercise and eat healthfully are physical activity across the adult lifespan. Psychology of Sport
protected against the effects of weight beliefs on behavior. and Exercise, 12, 99-105.
Such work might occur in conjunction with studies on attri- Burnette, J. L. (2010). Implicit theories of body weight: Entity
tion in dieting and exercise studies or effectiveness of diet beliefs can weigh you down. Personality and Social Psychology
and exercise programs. Encouraging the belief that weight is Bulletin, 36, 410-422.
changeable and encouraging nonweight motivations for Centers for Disease Control. (2007). National Health and Nutrition
healthy behaviors may both be effective strategies for Examination Survey Data. Retrieved from http://wwwn.cdc.
improving health behaviors. Finally, the etiology of entity gov/nchs/nhanes/search/nhanes07_08.aspx
theories about weight may be further explored to assess Centers for Disease Control. (2009). National Health and Nutrition
whether and how those beliefs develop and can be changed. Examination Survey Data. Retrieved from http://wwwn.cdc.
gov/Nchs/Nhanes/Search/nhanes09_10.aspx
With regard to clinical applications, the present results
Centers for Disease Control. (2013, September). National Health
found that the popular belief that weight is almost entirely and Nutrition Examination Survey: Analytic guidelines,
genetic and uncontrollable is associated with poor health. 1999-2010. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 2, Number 161.
Psychologists, physicians, dietitians, and other health care Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/series/sr_02/
providers might have the opportunity to discourage the belief sr02_161.pdf
that weight is uncontrollable or emphasize the importance of Clark, M. M., Abrams, D. B., Niaura, R. S., Eaton, C. A., & Rossi,
other motivations for healthful eating and exercise (Panzer, J. S. (1991). Self-efficacy in weight management. Journal of
2006). By fighting the perception that weight is unchangeable, Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 739-744.
health care providers may be able to increase healthful behav- Coates, C. B. J., Slattery, M. L., Potter, J. D., Quesenberry, C. P.,
iors among their patients. Jr., & Edwards, S. M. (1998). Body size and the risk of colon
cancer in a large case-control study. International Journal of
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Obesity, 22, 178-184.
Davies, M. F. (1997). Belief persistence after evidential discredit-
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect ing: The impact of generated versus provided explanations on
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. the likelihood of discredited outcomes. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 33, 561-578. doi:10.1006/jesp.1997.1336
Funding Davis, C., Fox, J., Brewer, H., & Ratusny, D. (1995). Motivations
The authors received no financial support for the research, author- to exercise as a function of personality characteristics, age, and
ship, and/or publication of this article. gender. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 165-174.
Dean, M. (2013). On fat agency, activism, and academia. Fat
Studies, 2, 210-215.
References Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories:
Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal directed Elaboration and extension of the model. Psychological Inquiry,
behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral con- 6, 322-333.
trol. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 453-474. Flegal, K. M., Carroll, M. D., Kit, B. K., & Ogden, C. L. (2012).
Akinnusi, M. E., Saliba, R., Porhomayon, J., & El-Solh, A. Prevalence of obesity and trends in the distribution of body
A. (2012). Sleep disorders in morbid obesity. European mass index among US adults, 1999-2010. Journal of the
Parent and Alquist 345

American Medical Association, 307, 491-497. doi:10.1001/ nature of ability and achievement goals. European Physical
jama.2012.39 Education Review, 7, 219-242.
Fotopoulos, C., Krystallis, A., Vassallo, M., & Pagiaslis, A. (2009). Ommundsen, Y. (2001b). Self-handicapping strategies in physi-
Food Choice Questionnaire (FCQ) revisited. Suggestions cal education classes: The influence of implicit theories of the
for the development of an enhanced general food motivation nature of ability and achievement goal orientations. Psychology
model. Appetite, 52, 199-208. of Sport and Exercise, 2, 139-156.
Furnham, A., Badmin, N., & Sneade, I. (2002). Body image dissatis- Panzer, B. (2006). Integrating pediatric obesity treatment into clini-
faction: Gender differences in eating attitudes, self-esteem, and cal practice. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 76, 531-
reasons for exercise. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary 544.
and Applied, 136, 581-596. Pollard, T. M., Steptoe, A., & Wardle, J. (1998). Motives underlying
Glanz, K., Basil, M., Maibach, E., Goldberg, J., & Snyder, D. healthy eating: Using the food choice questionnaire to explain
(1998). Why American eat what they do: Taste, nutrition, cost, variation in dietary intake. Journal of Biosocial Science, 30,
convenience, and weight control concerns as influences on food 165-179.
consumption. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, Rose, S. A., Poynter, P. S., Anderson, J. W., Noar, S. M., &
98, 1118-1126. Conigliaro, J. (2013). Physician weight loss advice and patient
Gorber, C., Tremblay, M., Moher, D., & Gorber, B. (2007). A com- weight loss behavior change: A literature review and meta-
parison of direct vs. self-report measures for assessing height, analysis of survey data. International Journal of Obesity, 37,
weight and body mass index: A systematic review. Obesity 118-128. doi:10.1038/ijo.2012.24
Reviews, 8, 307-326. Rosenstock, I. M., Strecher, V. J., & Becker, M. H. (1988). Social
Hong, Y., Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M. S., & Wan, W. (1999). learning theory and the health belief model. Health Education
Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system & Behavior, 15, 175-183. doi:10.1177/109019818801500203
approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, Rothblum, E. D. (2014). Commentary: Lesbians should take the
588-599. lead in removing the stigma that has long been associated with
Karnik, S., & Kanekar, A. (2012). Childhood obesity: A global body weight. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender
public health crisis. International Journal of Preventative Diversity, 1, 377-382.
Medicine, 3, 1-7. Saltzer, E. B. (1982). The Weight Locus of Control (WLOC) scale:
Kilpatrick, M., Hebert, E., & Bartholomew, J. (2005). College stu- A specific measure for obesity research. Journal of Personality
dents’ motivations for physical activity: Differentiating men’s Assessment, 46, 620-628.
and women’s motives for sport participation and exercise. Schwartz, M. B., & Brownell, K. D. (2004). Obesity and body image.
Journal of American College Health, 54, 87-94. Body Image, 1, 43-56. doi:10.1016/S1740-1445(03)00007-X
Kitsantas, A. (2000). The role of self-regulation strategies and self- Shiri, R., Karppinen, J., Leino-Arjas, P., Solovieva, S., & Viikari-
efficacy perceptions in successful weight loss maintenance. Juntura, E. (2010). The association between obesity and low
Psychology & Health, 15, 811-820. back pain: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology,
Komarraju, M., & Nadler, D. (2013). Self-efficacy and academic 171, 135-154.
achievement: Why do implicit beliefs, goals, and effort regula- Strecher, V. J., DeVellis, B. M., Becker, M. H., & Rosenstock, I. M.
tion matter? Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 67-72. (1986). The role of self-efficacy in achieving health behavior
Linde, J. A., Rothman, A. J., Baldwin, A. S., & Jeffry, R. W. change. Health Education & Behavior, 13, 73-91.
(2006). The impact of self-efficacy on behavior change and Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statis-
weight change among overweight participants in a weight loss tics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
trial. Health Psychology, 25, 282-291. Taberno, C., & Wood, R. E. (1999). Implicit theories versus the
Martocchio, J. J. (1994). Effects of conceptions of ability on anxi- social construct of ability in self-regulation and performance
ety, self-efficacy, and learning in training. Journal of Applied on a complex task. Organizational Behavior and Human
Psychology, 79, 819-825. Decision Processes, 78, 104-127.
McDonald, K., & Thompson, J. K. (1992). Eating disturbance, Throp, A. A., Owen, N., Neuhaus, M., & Dunstan, D. W. (2011).
body image dissatisfaction, and reasons for exercising: Sedentary behaviors and subsequent health outcomes in adults:
Gender differences and correlational findings. International A systematic review of longitudinal studies, 1996-2011.
Journal of Eating Disorders, 11, 289-292. doi:10.1002/1098- American Journal of Preventative Medicine, 41, 207-215.
108X(199204)11:3<289::AID-EAT2260110314>3.0.CO;2-F doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2011.05.004
McDonald, R. P., & Ho, M.-H. R. (2002). Principles and practice in Tiggemann, M. (2004). Body image across the adult life span:
reporting structural equation analyses. Psychological Methods, Stability and change. Body Image, 1, 29-42.
7, 64-82. Wang, C. K. J., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Spray, C. M., & Biddle,
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2010). Mplus user’s guide. Los S. J. H. (2002). Achievement goal profiles in school physi-
Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. cal education: Differences in self-determination, sport ability
O’Hara, L., & Gregg, J. (2010). Don’t diet: Adverse effects of the beliefs, and physical activity. British Journal of Educational
weight centered health paradigm. In F. De Meester, S. Zibadi Psychology, 72, 433-445.
& R. R. Watson (Eds.), Modern dietary fat intakes in disease Wang, Y. C., McPherson, K., Marsh, T., Gortmaker, S. L., &
promotion (pp. 431-444). New York, NY: Springer. Brown, M. (2011). Health and economic burden of the pro-
Ommundsen, Y. (2001a). Pupils’ affective responses in physical jected obesity trends in the USA and the UK. Lancet, 378, 815-
education classes: The association of implicit theories of the 825. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60814-3
346 Health Education & Behavior 43(3)

Weston, R., & Gore, P. A. (2006). A brief guide to structural equa- sites/letsmove.gov/files/TaskForce_on_Childhood_Obesity_
tion modeling. The Counseling Psychologist, 34, 719-751. May2010_FullReport.pdf
doi:10.1177/0011000006286345 Withrow, D., & Alter, D. A. (2011). The economic burden of obe-
White House Task Force on Childhood Obesity Report to the sity worldwide: A systematic review of the direct costs of
President. (2010). Solving the problem of childhood obesity obesity. Obesity Reviews, 12, 131-141. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
within a generation. Retrieved from http://www.letsmove.gov/ 789X.2009.00712.x

You might also like