You are on page 1of 15

546228

research-article2014
JTRXXX10.1177/0047287514546228Journal of Travel ResearchOh et al.

Research Article

Journal of Travel Research

Motivations and Goals of Slow Tourism


2016, Vol. 55(2) 205­–219
© The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0047287514546228
jtr.sagepub.com

Haemoon Oh1, A. George Assaf1, and Seyhmus Baloglu2

Abstract
The concept of slow tourism is gaining attention, and this study provides new insights into the phenomenon from the
perspective of a goal-driven consumption process. The authors conduct primary qualitative research to define slow tourism,
build a conceptual model, and develop measurement scales. Based on the data collected from four popular U.S. tourist
destinations, the authors find that goal-driven consumption theory exhibits compelling explanatory power for the slow
behavioral process in the tourism context. The slow tourism process also appears to coexist with fast modes of travel and
they contribute differently to general tourism experience outcomes. Discussion includes approaches to understanding the
slow tourism phenomenon, issues around slowness in human behavior, and future directions for research on slow tourism.

Keywords
slow tourism, goal-driven consumption, time, mass tourism, travel modes, tourism motivations

Introduction spatio-temporal practices, immersive modes of travel and


ethical relations that are premised on the desire to connect in
Recently, the field of tourism has witnessed a growing prolif- particular ways and to disconnect in others” (Fullagar,
eration of new forms of tourism. People travel to appreciate Markwell, and Wilson 2012, p. 3). In more recent years,
the natural environment (ecotourism), immerse into different therefore, the focus of slow tourism has been shifting from
cultures (cultural tourism), visit the authentic past/remains of mere environmental sustainability to the practice of time and
people and places (heritage tourism), learn the harmony space in ways conducive to personal satisfaction and well-
among the environment, local residents, and visitors (green being (Parkins and Craig 2009).
tourism), or obtain desirable medical treatments and services The phenomenon of slow tourism also needs to be under-
(health/medical tourism). Some countries like Japan offi- stood in a broad sociocultural context of slow movement
cially recognize these new forms of tourism to encourage (Fullagar, Markwell, and Wilson 2012; Groenendaal 2012).
more travels as well as develop effective promotional strate- In more developed and industrialized societies where travel
gies (Maruyama and Parker 2012). Broadly underpinning demands are becoming increasingly cultural and normative,
these new fads of tourism are two driving forces, say, envi- people tend to experience disempowerment, powerlessness,
ronmental sustainability and personal/social well-being and dehumanization due to the voracious competition on
(Moore 2012). Emerging from these environmental and per- speed (O’Regan 2012; Parkins 2004). Moreover, as advanc-
sonal concerns, too, is another form of tourism called slow ing technology and inundating information constantly accel-
tourism. erate the pace of life, people are susceptible to undergoing
The concept of slow tourism is relatively new and, to date, time poverty, surface engagement and processing of infor-
defies an exact definition. Its origin traces back to some more mation, loss of control, a blasé attitude (i.e., an indifference
institutionalized social movements such as slow food and toward the distinctions between things in value and mean-
slow cities (CittaSlow) that started in Italy in the 1980s and ing), and eventually loss of the self (Howard 2012). Thus,
1990s (Fullagar, Markwell, and Wilson 2012; Hall 2012). In people’s desire to “slow down” may be a consequential
its early notion, slow tourism often emphasized its close link reflection of their mental resistance to the ever-accelerating
to speed and mobilities, especially with a particular interest pattern of lifestyle in search of opportunities to vent their
in the transportations to and from destinations that could not
only reduce environmental pollution through use of low car- 1
University of Massachusetts Amherst, MA, USA
bon emission vehicles but also save depleting energy 2
University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
resources. It therefore has been a concept frequently tied
with sustainable tourism. However, researchers argue that Corresponding Author:
A.George Assaf, Department of Hospitality and Tourism Management,
slow tourism is much more than movement or transport Isenberg School of Management, University of Massachusetts Amherst, 90
between places (Dickinson and Lumsdon 2010); “the term Campus Center Way Flint 107, Amherst, MA 01003, USA.
‘mobilities’ [i.e., slow tourism] encapsulates a range of Email: assaf@isenberg.umass.edu

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


206 Journal of Travel Research 55(2)

stress, decelerate their pace, and ultimately restore and enrich Conceptualizing Slow Tourism
the self. In that sense, travel may offer an apropos channel,
especially if it is in a slow form. Slow Tourism as a Goal-Driven Consumption
Despite the growing sociocultural awareness of and To develop a conceptual model of traveler-based slow tour-
movement toward slowness, and more specifically slow ism, this study relies on the theory of goal-driven consumer
tourism in the tourism field, the literature documents little behavior that has been gaining attention in consumer research
systematic research on the topic (Fullagar, Markwell, and (Bagozzi and Dholakia 1999; Bagozzi and Warshaw 1990;
Wilson 2012). In particular, slow tourism and its underlying Chernev 2004; Novak, Hoffman, and Duhachek 2003).
process from the perspective of personal meaning and According to goal-driven consumption theory (also synony-
engagement are yet to see seminal empirical research for bet- mous with “goal-oriented” or “goal-directed”), much of con-
ter understandings of how travelers make related decisions sumer behavior is goal-directed, and consumers make
(Dickinson and Lumsdon 2010). Much of the existing purchases to produce or yield one or more end-state goals or
research on slow tourism is either descriptive or qualitative outcomes (Bagozzi and Dholakia 1999). Goals are central to
and, thus, empirical evidence is still lacking to offer an consumer decision making (Bettman 1979; Higgins 2002),
extended insight into its dimension and process (Groenendaal and consumption goals are not limited to end states but also
2012). One reason for this lack of research could be due sim- include experiences, ongoing processes, or a series of many
ply to its relative recency. Another potential reason is the happenings that are interconnected (Novak, Hoffman, and
skepticism that critics—with some being active travel Duhachek 2003). For example, “a person’s vacation goal
researchers—hold against this new form of tourism. might not be limited to a particular location and time period
Recognizing the parallel presence of research needs and per se but rather might reside in anticipated educational, rec-
skepticism on slow tourism, this study aims to investigate reational, and interpersonal experiences” (Bagozzi and
whether the phenomenon of slowness is empirically valid in Dholakia 1999, p. 19). Once goals are set, consumers strive
tourist behavior. More specifically, we identify motivations to achieve them in as effectual ways as possible. Striving for
and goals reflecting slow tourism and attempt to show goals means aligning behavior and sourcing relevant means
whether realization of such individual motivations and goals toward the goals or standards.
is facilitated by the active choice of a slow pace of traveling. Slow tourism in general can be a goal-driven activity or
To this end, this study develops and tests a conceptual model series of activities, as in a trip down the Amazon, an archeo-
of traveler-based slow tourism. We also test whether the slow logical excursion, or a religious pilgrimage all having expe-
tourism process has implications for general tourist experi- riential goals of one sort or another (Bagozzi and Dholakia
ence outcomes that frequently serve as criterion variables in 1999). Sirgy (2010) illuminates how goal-driven consump-
travel research. Such a test will offer an additional opportu- tion epitomizes tourist behavior in general. Thus, slow tour-
nity to validate the ontological value of slow tourism for fur- ism, either as the sole or as a partial goal/action of a trip,
ther academic as well as pragmatic inquiries. may as well follow a goal-driven behavioral process.
In the dearth of relevant theories and direct evidence, it is Bagozzi and Dholakia present an organizing model of the
difficult to take on a strong position to hypothesize any par- goal-driven consumer behavior process in which three
ticular phenomenon of slow tourism. We therefore do not activities appear to be key elements: goal setting, action
necessarily view slowness in tourism as an anti-speed or anti- planning, and goal attainment/failure. In a slow tourism
mass tourism concept; rather, we suspect that both slow and context, immersing into a natural environment and thereby
fast modes of travel coexist and indeed constantly interplay revitalizing body and mind may be goals to a traveler taking
within the same unit of travel, within the same traveler, and a vacation. As the action plan, the traveler may want to
both within and across destinations. That is, the dichotomy of choose the destination(s) and the way to travel (pace, access,
slow versus fast tourism is a matter of categorizing conve- length, company, etc.) that are aligned with travel goals. The
nience, reflecting a generalized pace, mode, or pattern of the degree of goal attainment will then determine his/her satis-
focal travel within which the traveler purposefully chooses to faction with the trip and relevant future behavioral inten-
engage in either a slow or fast mode of travel to maximize his tions (Higgins 2002 ).
or her travel goal(s), be it satisfaction, personal well-being, or Based on the discussion above, we propose in Figure 1 a
self-fulfillment (Moore 2012; Singh 2012). Consequently, we model of slow tourism from the perspective of the traveler’s
model both slow and fast modes of travel within a single goal striving process. In the model, the first stage pertains to
framework as competing and complementary, rather than slow tourism motivations. While discussing the goal-setting
exclusive, choices toward a maximal attainment of the travel stage of a goal-driven consumer behavior, Bagozzi and
goal, thereby assessing the relative roles of slow- versus fast- Dholakia (1999) explain how the motivational aspects of a
paced travel modes in tourist behaviors. This approach could goal are either consciously or unconsciously represented in
extend the asymmetric dialectics of “enlightened mass tour- the mind of the consumer and summarize goal-setting activ-
ism” proposed recently by Weaver (2013). ities in a goal hierarchy framework. At the lowest

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


Oh et al. 207

Fast Mode of
Travel

General
Slow Slow
Tourism
Tourism Tourism
Experience
Motivations Goals
Outcomes

Slow Mode of
Travel

Figure 1.  A conceptual framework of slow tourism.

abstraction, the goal hierarchy conceives subordinate goals Defining Slow Tourism
(e.g., saving money for a trip). At the next level of the hier-
archy are focal goals (e.g., to escape the daily routine by Slowness in tourism has been characterized as an engagement
taking a trip), then followed at the highest level of abstrac- in a number of behavioral modes while traveling. Dickinson
tion by superordinate goals (e.g., enriching the self through and Lumsdon (2010), for example, emphasize that any defini-
the trip). We adopted the concept of focal goals to specify tion of slow travel should focus on transportation for the sake
slow tourism motivations in our model and superordinate of environmental protection and participation in relatively
goals as ultimate goals of slow tourism. In a trip, both fast slower forms of travel such as exploring local history, culture,
and slow modes of travel may facilitate realizing motiva- and people. They define slow tourism as “a conceptual frame-
tions (focal goals) and attaining ultimate goals (superordi- work that involves people who ‘travel to destinations more
nate goals), thereby contributing to general outcomes of the slowly overland, stay longer and travel less’ and who incorpo-
entire trip (Dickinson and Peeters 2012). The stronger the rate travel to a destination as itself an experience and, once at
motivations, the more likely the goals will be achieved, the destination, engage with local transport options and ‘slow
because the traveler will strive harder. Finally, we expect food and beverage,’ take time to explore local history and cul-
that attainment of slow tourism goals contributes positively ture, and support the environment” (pp. 1-2). Lipman and
to general trip outcomes. Murphy (2012) recognize sustainable consumption through
Conceptualizing slow tourism in application of a main- “slower” transport and products, reduced mobility, and “less”
stream consumption theory is a novel attempt. Although travel as a key ingredient of slow tourism. Slow tourism also
various writings, especially the recent volumes by Fullagar, means eschewing “fast leisure” and “fast tourism,” such as in
Markwell, and Wilson (2012) and Dickinson and Lumsdon package tours and holiday taking, to alleviate the feelings of a
(2010), provide a collection of relevant thoughts and prac- lack of time in the midst of the pressure for identity fulfill-
tices, generalizable theoretical frameworks of slow tourism ment (Moore 2012). These rather categorical views to slow
are yet to appear in the literature. Given the current status of tourism tend to focus on manifested forms of travel behavior,
developing thoughts around slow tourism, primary research not the underlying psychological process.
geared to both conceptual and empirical validation of the The form-based, categorical understanding of slow tour-
phenomenon is indispensable. Therefore, we conducted a ism, however, poses several limitations. First, it does not
series of both qualitative and quantitative studies, as depicted clearly explain why people engage in slow forms of travel
in Figure 2, to test the proposed model in Figure 1. Much of (e.g., motivations) and what they are pursuing by such a slow
our discussion that follows in this section relies on not only practice of time and space (e.g., goals). Second, such overt
the handful literature but also the findings from our primary behavioral categories do not elucidate the decision-making
research, including the expert interviews and focus group, process from the individual traveler’s perspective, thereby
which are detailed later. The focus of these primary studies limiting our understanding of the phenomenon. Third, cate-
was on developing a content-valid set of scales to measure gorical definitions of slow tourism assume that travelers
slow tourism that might be omnipresent in various types of share common standards or perceptions of temporality in
trips and destinations. light of travel pace, mode, tempo, and rhythm. “Yet there is

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


208 Journal of Travel Research 55(2)

• Collect secondary evidence of slow tourism


Literature Review • Explore meaning and definition of slow tourism
• Structure the research plan

• Identify key issues around slow tourism


• Define meaning of slow tourism
Expert Interview
• Generate dimensions of slow tourism
• Identify slow tourism motivations and goals

• Discuss personal meaning of slow tourism


Focus Group • Discuss motivations and goals for slow tourism
• Generate possible measurement items

• Review key results of focus group


• Reassure the validity of the preliminary findings
Expert Review • Review the draft instrument
• Confirm direction of measurement

• Assure workability of the national survey


Pilot Survey • Confirm direction of measurement
• Refine and finalize the instrument

• Administer the survey nationally


Main Survey • Assure random sampling and data quality
• Focus on generalizability of the results

Figure 2.  The research procedure employed.

little consensus on what ‘slow’ actually means, and how it is process), rather than form-based (i.e., based on the manifest
practiced or interpreted in relation to different tourism con- behavior), view of tourism experiences (Uriely, Yonay, and
texts, cultures, and mobilities” (Fullagar, Markwell, and Simchai 2002). Consistent with the goal-driven consumer
Wilson 2012, p. 3). Moreover, the categorical approach may behavior literature, therefore, slow tourism in this study
also overlook slow tourism components embedded in many refers to a trip or a series of trips taken in the subjectively
other types of what can be so called “fast” and “non-slow” determined, mentally slow pace of actions or movement for
travels. Although slow tourism may coincide with some realization of the motivations and goals that are specific to
forms or categories of travel, it may reside more deeply in the trip(s). This is a broader, attitude-based definition than
individual motivations, subjective choice of travel modes, that implied prevalently in the extant literature in that a per-
and other personal reasons such as preference and lifestyle, son does not have to engage in particular forms of travel to
all driven by either clearly or loosely set goals for the focal be a slow traveler and, at the same time, that not all travelers
travel. engaged in particular categories of travel are slow travelers.
We define slowness attitudinally rather than in behavioral Slowness in tourism is essentially intrinsic, and it is deter-
categories. That is, we argue that the term “slow” should be mined less extrinsically.
defined subjectively by individual travelers, rather than
that, for example, all visitors in certain travel genres or to
certain destinations are categorized as slow travelers. This
Slow Tourism Motivations
attitudinal approach is similar to the case of defining cus- At the focal level of Bagozzi and Dholakia’s (1999) goal
tomer loyalty by Dick and Basu (1994) and Oliver (1999), hierarchy, we determined six general slow tourism motiva-
who distinguished and emphasized the importance of attitu- tions for this study by means of expert interviews, a focus
dinal loyalty over behavioral loyalty. It is also in line with the group, and literature reviews. These motivations were based
type-based (i.e., based on the underlying psychological on the consensus between what the expert and focus groups

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


Oh et al. 209

(interjudge reliability > .80) commonly suggested through 1998). Finally, labeled often as learning, education, or
free elicitation methods and included relaxation, self-reflec- knowledge acquisition (Beh and Bruyere 2007; Fodness
tion, escape, novelty-seeking, engagement, and discovery. 1994; Ryan and Glendon 1998), discovery was suggested
Although suggested as common slow tourism motivations, also as a slow tourism motivation and is defined for this
these motivations significantly overlapped with general study as a desire to learn and understand something new. We
travel motivations reported in the literature. People may not borrowed some measurement items for these motivation
necessarily have a set of motivations unique to slow tourism constructs from many of the studies referred to above.
per se; instead, they may be motivated to travel “slowly” in
the same way they are for most other modes of travel, yet
Slow versus Fast Travel Modes
maybe to practice a slow tempo and rhythm more exten-
sively so as to maximize attainment of some superordinate As shown in Figure 1, the traveler, while striving for goals
goals associated closely with slow tourism. This is a premise (Bagozzi and Dholakia 1999), may opt to travel either fast or
we are to empirically test in this study. slow dependent upon time pressure, destination, and tourist
We briefly define the six motivations for this study with characteristics (Dickinson and Peeters 2012; Lew and
reference to the results of our primary research and the exist- McKercher 2006). The fast mode of travel may work as a
ing literature. Relaxation refers to a state of being free from “time saving” choice, while the slow mode as a “time savor-
pressure, stress, tension, anxiety, and excitement (nervous- ing” choice (Dickinson and Peeters 2012). Few studies, how-
ness), giving the feeling of ease, comfort, calm, and relief ever, have empirically incorporated both fast and slow modes
(McCabe 2009; Ryan and Glendon 1998; Yoon and Uysal of travel as a mentally instrumental process toward achiev-
2005). The traveler may arrive at such a mental state through ing some superordinate travel goals from a “type” or attitudi-
temporal deceleration and spatial distance from the pace and nal perspective (Uriely, Yonay, and Simchai 2002). Most
complexity of modern life (Howard 2012; Krippendorf studies touching on fast versus slow modes of travel focused
1987). Exercising a decelerated temporality in the novelty of on the “forms” or means of movement, that is, transporta-
the physical and social context, the traveler may also want to tion, from one destination to another (e.g., Dickinson and
identify self-status, gain insights into self, enhance feelings Peeters 2012; Lew and McKercher 2006). For the purpose of
of self-growth, and experience authentic selfhood (Crompton our study, we define the fast mode of travel as the mental
1979; Howard 2012). Similar to the concept of self-actual- tempo of movement or travel activities that is felt by the trav-
ization (Pearce and Lee 2005) and self-development (Li and eler, mostly under time pressure, as fast, rushed, or hastened
Cai 2012), therefore, self-reflection is defined as a generic in processing external stimuli during the trip. The slow mode
need for feeling connected to self, nourishing self, and iden- of travel then is the counteracting “type” of perceptions
tifying with self (Howard 2012). Escape has been a dominant about the conduct of travel (Lumsdon and McGrath 2011). It
travel motivation (e.g., Beh and Bruyere 2007; Fodness is the tourist’s mentally and attitudinally slowed temporal
1994; Iso-Ahola 1982), and so is it to slow travelers accord- process of responding to external stimuli.
ing to our expert and focus groups. Combining both personal We expect that the traveler will utilize either the fast or
and interpersonal aspects (Snepenger et al. 2006), escape in slow travel mode back and forth to maximize achieving his
this study refers to a desire to detach oneself both physically or her travel goals. That is, the travel mode serves as an
and mentally from one’s daily routines and obligations. Both instrumental process of behavior and mediates travel motiva-
the expert and focus groups suggested that people get moti- tions toward travel goals, as in a means-end process (Gutman
vated to engage in slow travels to seek novelty experience 1982; Klenosky 2002). When the travel goals are more
through new temporalities, new places, and new people that closely aligned with institutionalized mass tourism, the trav-
offer them sensations of thrills, adventures, and stimulation. eler tends to employ faster travel modes more frequently
Many studies support such novelty seeking as an important and, hence, the fast travel mode will play a more influential
travel motivation (see Cohen 1979; Lee and Crompton 1992; mediating role. Conversely, for less institutionalized slow
Li and Cai 2012; Pearce and Lee 2005). While the traveler tourism goals, the slow travel mode will be a dominant
may seek many outcomes as a result of taking a trip, an choice facilitating goal attainment. We test these rationales in
intrinsic pleasure of engaging in the travel activities, envi- the context of slow tourism motivations and goals, thereby
ronment, and local culture seems to motivate people to travel expecting that the slow travel mode will show its dominant
slowly according to our focus group. Engagement motiva- role in our proposed model.
tion, defined in this study as the extent to which the traveler
is engrossed to or enjoys the action of traveling or the travel
Slow Tourism Goals
environment, has seen slightly different operationalizations
in previous studies such as reward maximizing utilitarian The focus group suggested most frequently two universal
function (Fodness 1994), hedonism (Kim, Ritchie, and goals of slow tourism: revitalization and self-enrichment.
McCormick 2010), aesthetic experience (Oh, Fiore, and For the revitalization goal, for example, the traveler would
Jeong 2007), and reward maximization (Waller and Lea want to get refreshed, reinvigorated, and recharged as a result

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


210 Journal of Travel Research 55(2)

of completing a slow travel. The self-enrichment goal would the motivations, modes, and goal attainment of slow tourism;
help the traveler get inspired, discover self, expand one’s the relationships among them are not a concern in this study
perspective, understand and connect more deeply to the des- as they are addressed in many previous studies (e.g., Oh,
tination, and restore positive attitudes and mindset. Fiore, and Jeong 2007).
Intuitively, the traveler will strive to attain the revitalization
goal out of slow tourism, given their related motivations
Methods
such as relaxation and escape. Similarly, the traveler with
such travel motivations as self-reflection, novelty-seeking, Preliminary Studies
and discovery may enrich self through the trip. Literature
supports the revitalization goal, both physical and mental, in We conducted both qualitative and quantitative studies as
various forms of the travel motivations and benefits that are described in Figure 2 where we summarized both the study
realized through a travel and that are often operationalized as methods and key activities. Toward instrumentation, primary
travel outcomes (e.g., McCabe 2009; Ryan and Glendon qualitative studies were necessary as our literature review
1998). Previous studies also operationalized the self-enrich- found few studies clearly defining the motivations and goals
ment goal in such concepts as self-development and self- associated with slow tourism (Iacobucci and Churchill 2010).
actualization (e.g., Pearce and Lee 2005) or personal growth For expert interviews, we recruited six prolific researchers
(Pearce and Packer 2013). In line with the extant research, internationally via email who had recently published work
revitalization in this study refers to a state of both mental and on slow tourism or related topics. Upon their agreement, we
physical vigor restored through a slow travel and self-enrich- sent a set of structured, open-ended questions asking them to
ment is a perceived degree of enhanced self-esteem, confi- define slow tourism and its possible subdimensions, list the
dence, and self-assurance obtained as a result of slow key motivations first and then goals that the slow tourist
tourism. might hold, and identify issues and directions for future aca-
demic inquiries. Of the “six experts,” three provided their
opinions: one was indifferent about slow tourism; one was
General Travel Outcome Correlates extremely critical of slow tourism and related advocacies,
Slower practices of time and space are conducive to the gen- rejecting them en masse; one did not respond. The respond-
eral outcomes reflecting tourist experiences. We argued ear- ing experts shared an opinion about the lack of clear under-
lier that fast and slow travels were not exclusive to each standing about slow tourism (i.e., unclear definition),
other; rather, they coexist complementing each other in any undefined roles of slow tourism relative to prevailing forms
travel unit for a maximal attainment of travel goals. If slow of mass tourism, and absence of theoretical research frame-
modes of travel are to complement fast counterparts, they works and supporting evidence.
should contribute to not only slow tourism goals but also Ten recent travelers, balanced in sex, participated in the
general tourist outcomes that have been traditionally used to focus group. We conveniently recruited these participants
evaluate the goodness of travels including mass tourism. from the staff directory of a university. In addition to balanc-
Thus, relating both travel modes and attainment of slow tour- ing sex, we screened them so that they had at least one over-
ism goals to general tourism experience outcomes, such as night vacation trip within the previous six months. The
satisfaction, return intention, and referral intention, would qualifying respondents participated in three sessions of
offer an opportunity to assess the nomological and criterion 90-minute long intense discussions for a stipend of $100.
validities of slow tourism in the spirit of an integrated The first session ran for about 20 minutes with a focus on
approach (Dickinson and Peeters 2012; Weaver 2013). understanding what slow tourism meant, especially in con-
We chose satisfaction, future return intention, and refer- trast to institutionalized, mass tourism. Next was a 40-min-
ral/recommendation intention as three criterion variables ute session to discuss important reasons or motivations for
based on their prevalent usage in tourism research (Baker which people would engage in slow tourism. The last
and Crompton 2000; Bowen and Clarke 2009; Lam and Hsu 30-minute discussion concentrated on the ultimate benefits
2006; Yoon and Uysal 2005). Satisfaction refers to the degree they could get from conducting slow tourism. The group also
of “pleasurable fulfillment of some need, desire, or goal” suggested that they would opt for either a fast or slow mode
(Oliver 1999, p. 34) and, thus, it qualifies as a criterion vari- of traveling whenever and wherever necessary to fulfill what
able reflecting goal fulfillment as a result of a goal-driven they were looking for in the trip. We recorded and transcribed
slow tourism consumption. Future revisit intention is the the discussion, and used two trained judges to sort the infor-
likelihood of returning to the same destination for similar mation by following our frameworks. We used the informa-
travel goals in similar travel situations, and referral intention tion that achieved at least 80% of between-judge reliability.
is the likelihood of spreading positive words about or recom- Based on the expert interviews and focus group, we
mending the travel experience specific to a particular desti- drafted a survey questionnaire and submitted it to four newly
nation (Lam and Hsu 2006; Oh, Fiore, and Jeong 2007). recruited tourism research “experts” to ensure content valid-
These criterion variables are modeled merely as correlates of ity. Two of the “experts” provided the comments and

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


Oh et al. 211

suggestions by email that helped us refine the instrument. 30–40 travelers, per destination, randomly drawn from the
The other two experts did not respond in time. We also asked same national consumer panel. Descriptive analyses of the
10 undergraduate students to read the questionnaire and sim- pilot data led us to revising two measures of travel modes to
ulate survey participation. Their suggestions helped us esti- derive measurement in more intended directions.
mate the time taken to finish the survey and refine the The main survey targeted obtaining a minimum of 250
questionnaire further. complete responses for each destination for sufficient statis-
tical power in testing the model, given the expected model
size and number of parameters to estimate (Kline 2011). As a
Instrumentation
result, we collected a total of 1,068 responses, with 266 for
All variables in our model were operationalized as multivari- Hawaii, 269 for Las Vegas, 265 for Yellowstone, and 268 for
ate constructs, with slow tourism motivations having six sub- Martha’s. We conducted parallel analyses of the model struc-
dimensions, slow tourism goals two subdimensions, and ture and measurement integrity across the four destinations
general travel outcomes three different correlates. We relied and, with sufficient evidence for equivalence in the model
on our primary research as well as the tourism motivation structure and measurement, we combined the data so that our
literature to build the measurement scales for the motiva- results and discussions could focus on a general role of slow
tions, goals, and general tourism experience outcomes, while tourism as intended (Milfont and Fischer 2010; Oh and Hsu
the scales for both fast and slow travel modes were based 2014; Steenkamp and Baumgartner 1998). We employed
solely on our primary research. The specific measurement structural equation modeling methods analyzing the vari-
items for each construct appear in Table 2. Except for the ance–covariance matrices via LISREL 8.3 with the maxi-
general tourism outcomes, all items were measured on a mum likelihood estimator. Assessments of the model relied
5-point Likert-type scale with 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = dis- mainly on comparative fit index (CFI), non-normed fit index
agree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, and 5 = strongly agree. Two (NNFI), and root mean square error of approximation
items comprised tourist satisfaction, one anchored on a (RMSEA), following the suggestions by Anderson and
5-point scale with 1 = very dissatisfied, 2 = dissatisfied, 3 = Gerbing (1988) and Hu and Bentler (1999).
neutral, 4 = satisfied, and 5 = very satisfied (Oh, Fiore, and
Jeong 2007) and the other on a 5-point scale with 1 = terrible
. . . 5 = delightful (Maddox 1985; Oh, Fiore, and Jeong 2007).
Findings
Both revisit intention and referral intention each had two Sample and Destinations
items anchored on a 5-point scale with 1 = very unlikely, 2 =
unlikely, 3 = neutral, 4 = likely, and 5 = very likely (Oh, Table 1 summarizes sample characteristics. A gender balance
Fiore, and Jeong 2007). was evident and people in the age categories of 26–35 and
56–65 had slightly stronger representations than the others.
Nearly two-thirds of the sample reported their annual house-
Surveys hold income of US$100,000 or less. The majority were mar-
As the focus of our study was to assess the role of slow travel ried, either with children (44.2%) or without children
modes in attaining travel goals, we attempted to test the (16.2%), followed by single (31.2%). Whites represented
model across a broad spectrum of tourist destinations so as to almost three-quarters, and more than 95% were taking vaca-
increase generalizability of our results. To this end, we tion or leisure trips at least once a year. About a half of the
selected four popular destinations attracting a variety of trav- respondents reported having visited the destination within
elers with potentially diverse travel motivations and goals. the past three months and the remaining half within the past
They were Hawaii, Las Vegas, Yellowstone National Park six months, with more than 90% of all traveling for vacation
(“Yellowstone”), and Martha’s Vineyard (“Martha’s”). We or visiting friends and family members. Only 15.7% traveled
intentionally avoided choosing destinations potentially alone and the remaining majority traveled with family mem-
biased excessively toward slow tourism orientation in order bers, friends, or others. Finally, 81% had visited the same
to assess the role of slow tourism conservatively. We col- destination more than once.
lected data electronically by establishing a website contain- The destinations selected in this study seemed to cover
ing the research questionnaire for each destination and possibilities of both slow and fast tourism activities. The
inviting randomly American consumers from a national con- respondents rated the destination on a fast- to slow-paced
sumer panel supplied by QualtricsTM. We required all partici- tourism scale anchored with (n) 1 = suitable for very slow
pants to have traveled to the destination at least once in the paced tourism (36), 2 = suitable for slow-paced tourism
past six months, and the data confirmed this 100% through a (180), 3 = suitable for both slow- and fast-paced tourism
screening question. The questionnaire included two atten- (606), 4 = suitable for fast-paced tourism (170), and 5 = suit-
tion-filtering questions to check on the level of participant able for very fast-paced tourism (76). The response distribu-
involvement; all participants (100%) answered the questions tion was essentially normal, with the mean of 3.07 and
correctly. We piloted the questionnaire with a sample of standard deviation of .862 (skewness = .23; kurtosis = .54).

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


212 Journal of Travel Research 55(2)

Table 1.  Sample Characteristics (n = 1,068). One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Duncan’s mul-
Frequency Percent
tiple range test indicated that Hawaii was neutral (mean =
2.98), Las Vegas was in favor of fast-paced tourism (3.64),
Sex and both Martha’s (2.82) and Yellowstone (2.81) were
 Male 531 49.7
slightly in favor of slow tourism (F = 64.35, p <.01).
 Female 537 50.3

Similarly, they answered another question, “While visiting
Age ______ last time, how fast or slow paced was your overall
  25 or younger 165 15.4 travel behavior?” on a scale (n) with 1 = very slow (20), 2 =
 26-35 308 28.8 slow (302), 3 = neutral (433), 4 = fast (268), and 5 = very fast
 36-45 158 14.8 (45) (skewness = .18; kurtosis = –.51). The respondents
 46-55 146 13.7 reported that they engaged in slightly fast paced tourism
 56-65 191 17.9
behavior in Las Vegas (mean = 3.45) but in slightly slow
  66 or older 100 9.4

paced tourism behavior at the other three destinations
Annual household income (Hawaii = 2.92; Martha’s = 2.83; Yellowstone = 2.86) (one-
  Less than $50,000 288 27.7 way ANOVA F = 31.61, p < .01). These results indicated that
 $50,000-$75,000 226 21.2 none of these sample destinations was extremely skewed
 $75,000-$100,000 235 22.0 toward either fast or slow tourism characteristics.
 $101,001-$125,000 117 11.0
 $125,001-$150,000 66 6.2
 $150,001-$175,000 60 5.6 Measurement Quality
  $175,001 or more 76 7.1
  The quality of our first-order measurement appeared to be
Marital status sound as shown in Table 2 reporting the confirmatory factor
 Single 333 31.2 analysis results. We dropped several measurement items
  Single with dependents 47 4.4 through preliminary analyses for reasons such as unaccept-
  Married with no children 173 16.2 ably low (<.40) or high cross loadings and low correlations
  Married with children 472 44.2
with the remaining items of the construct. The finalized mea-
 Other 43 4.0

surement model reported in Table 2 resulted in good fit with
Ethnic background RMSEA = .046, NNFI = .99, and CFI = .99 (χ2 = 1,362.67, df
 White 791 74.1 = 417); all parameters were statistically significant (p <.05)
 Hispanic/Latino 82 7.7 with no notable sign of improper solutions (Boomsma 2008).
  Asian/Pacific Islander 98 9.2 The factor loadings were .73 or higher, exhibiting satisfac-
  Black/African American 69 6.5 tory within-construct convergent validity (Anderson and
 Other 28 2.6
Gerbing 1988; Bagozzi and Yi 1988). Cronbach’s alphas of

Average yearly travel frequency for vacation/leisure
internal consistency for the item sets of the constructs ranged
  Less than once a year 66 6.2 from .76 (engagement) to .92 (self-reflection), which was
  Once a year 258 24.2 satisfactory. Similarly, the composite reliabilities (ρη) of the
  2-3 times a year 526 49.3 constructs were at least .76 (engagement) and the amount of
  More than 2-3 times a year 218 20.4 variance extracted (ηVE) was at least .61 (both novelty-seek-
  ing and engagement), each exceeding the suggested mini-
Last time of visiting destination
mums (Bagozzi and Yi 1988; Fornell and Larcker 1981).
  Within the last 3 months 510 47.8
  4-6 months ago 558 52.2
These results substantiated convergent validity of the con-
  structs. Discriminant validity of the constructs was assessed
Primary reason for last visit to destination by comparing the shared variance between each pair of con-
 Business 35 3.3 structs against the amounts of variance extracted for the two
 Vacation 827 77.4 constructs (Bagozzi and Yi 1988). None of the shared vari-
  Visiting friends or family members 146 13.7 ance was larger than the amount of variance extracted for any
  Attending conference or workshop 23 2.2
construct, thereby demonstrating between-constructs dis-
 Other 37 3.5

criminant validity.
Travel party size
 Alone 168 15.7
  With someone else (family, friends, 900 84.3
Role of Slow Tourism
etc.) Assured of measurement integrity at the first order, we mod-
   
eled both slow tourism motivations and goals as second-
First time visiting the destination 203 19.0
order constructs respectively in order to (1) derive a set of

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


Oh et al. 213

Table 2.  Measurement Scales and Results of the Measurement Model.


Measures Mean SD Loadings Error

Slow tourism motivations


  Relaxation (α = .86; ρ(η) = .86; η(VE) = .67)
   I wanted to relax mentally. 4.12 .85 .87 .25
   I wanted to avoid interpersonal pressure and stress from my daily routine. 4.04 .89 .77 .41
   I wanted to experience peace and calm. 4.10 .90 .82 .33
  Self-reflection (α = .92; ρ(η) = .92; η(VE) = .74)
   I wanted to gain a new perspective on my life. 3.64 1.00 .85 .28
   I wanted to reflect more upon myself. 3.66 .99 .89 .21
   I wanted to feel inner harmony. 3.74 1.00 .86 .25
   I wanted to develop my personal and spiritual values. 3.51 1.12 .85 .29
  Escape (α = .80; ρ(η) = .81; η(VE) = .69)
   I wanted to escape from the hustles and bustles of my daily life. 4.17 .88 .90 .18
   I wanted to get away from everyday routines. 4.27 .77 .75 .44
  Novelty-seeking (α = .83; ρ(η) = .83; η(VE) = .61)
   I wanted to enjoy activities that offered thrills. 3.78 1.04 .74 .45
   I wanted to have opportunities to meet new people and do new things. 3.76 .99 .77 .40
   I wanted to seek adventure on this trip. 3.79 1.01 .83 .30
  Engagement (α = .76; ρ(η) = .76; η(VE) = .61)
   I wanted to feel that I totally belonged to the environment. 3.85 .90 .79 .37
   I wanted to immerse into local culture. 3.84 .96 .77 .41
  Discovery (α = .88; ρ(η) = .88; η(VE) = .65)
   I wanted to learn about something new. 3.99 .88 .79 .38
   I wanted to expand my knowledge of what the destination offered. 4.07 .85 .83 .31
   I wanted to obtain new perspectives from this travel. 3.99 .87 .83 .31
   I wanted to experience different things on this trip. 4.17 .82 .77 .41

Travel modes
  Fast pace travel (α = .79; ρ(η) = .79; η(VE) = .66)
   I tried to see the destination as much as possible in a limited amount of time. 3.81 1.04 .80 .37
   I tried to do more things at this destination under a time constraint. 3.34 1.05 .82 .32
  Slow pace travel (α = .69; ρ(η) = .70; η(VE) = .54)
   I took the tome to travel at my own pace. 4.15 .77 .74 .45
   I slowed down in my overall pace of travel. 3.65 .96 .73 .46

Slow tourism goals
  Self-enrichment (α = .90; ρ(η) = .90; η(VE) = .74)
   I felt much better about myself as a result of this trip. 3.85 .84 .86 .27
   This trip helped me enrich my overall perspective about myself. 3.78 .90 .88 .22
   I felt fulfilled about myself as a result of this trip. 3.92 .88 .85 .28
  Revitalization (α = .88; ρ(η) = .89; η(VE) = .79)
   I felt refreshed after visiting Hawaii. 4.23 .78 .91 .17
   This travel to Hawaii reenergized me. 4.12 .83 .87 .24

General tourism experience outcomes
  Satisfaction (α = .88; ρ(η) = .88; η(VE) = .78)
   How satisfied were you with your travel experience at Hawaii last time?a 4.50 .73 .88 .22
   Overall, how did you feel about your last trip to Hawaii?b 4.44 .70 .88 .22
  Future intention (α = .90; ρ(η) = .90; η(VE) = .82)
   How likely are you to travel to Hawaii again for similar purposes?c 4.49 .78 .88 .22
   How likely are you to consider Hawaii as an option for your future trips?c 4.44 .81 .93 .13
  Referral intention (α = .86; ρ(η) = .86; η(VE) = .75)
   How likely are you to recommend Hawaii to others?c 4.51 .75 .89 .21
   How likely are you to talk about Hawaii positively when you are asked?c 4.55 .73 .84 .29

Note: The model fit: χ2 = 1,362.67, degree of freedom (df) = 417; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = .046; nonnormed fit index (NNFI) = .99; comparative fit
index (CFI) = .99. All parameter estimates are statistically significant (p < .01).
a
Measured on a 5-point scale with 1 = very dissatisfied, 2 = dissatisfied, 3 = neutral, 4 = satisfied, and 5 = very satisfied.
b
Measured on a 5-point scale with 1 = terrible, . . . , 5 = delightful.
c
Measured on a 5-point scale with 1 = very unlikely, 2 = unlikely, 3 = neutral, 4 = likely, and 5 = very likely; all the other items were measured on a 5-point scale with
1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, and 5 = strongly agree.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


214 Journal of Travel Research 55(2)

results commensurate with the model proposed in Figure 1 Second, the other rival model considered was one specify-
and (2) draw more definite conclusions on the proposed ing travel modes to affect general tourism outcomes only indi-
model. Not only was such a model with second-order factors rectly through slow tourism goal attainment. This model was
conceptually identical to the proposed model but it would motivated particularly by the three insignificant direct paths
also avoid potentially mixed conclusions due to the multiple from slow mode to the general outcomes from the proposed
paths from motivations to travel modes and from travel model results in the fact that their first-order correlations were
modes to goals when modeled at the first order of individual all statistically significant and sizable (.48, .42, and .48,
subdimensions. The partial second-order structural model respectively). This model, which constrained the two direct
reflecting Figure 1 fit the data aptly, with RMSEA = .065, paths from travel modes to general tourism outcomes to zero,
NNFI = .98, and CFI = .98 (χ2 = 2580.87, df = 469). While resulted in fit indices slightly inferior to those for the proposed
these fit indices were slightly penalized for the increased model, with RMSEA = .066 (±.003), NNFI = .97, and CFI =
model complexity (i.e., the enlarged χ2-to-df ratio) due to the .98 (χ2 = 2655.54, df = 471). A χ2-difference test comparing
second-order factors included, they were still acceptable. In this to the proposed model indicated that this model was sig-
particular, the 90% confidence interval of RMSEA ranged nificantly worse than, and therefore should be rejected in favor
tightly from .063 to .067. The parameter estimates con- of, the proposed model (Δχ2 = 74.67, df = 2, p < .01).
formed to expectations. The second-order loadings of both
motivations and goals were all statistically significant (p
Discussion
<.05), and their standardized values ranged from .67 to .87
for the motivations and .84 and .88 for the goals. Goal-driven consumption behavior theory seems to provide
The structural parameter estimates were generally consis- a cogent framework for explaining the slow tourism phe-
tent with our expectations as summarized in Table 4. Slow nomenon. As Bagozzi and Dholakia (1999) conceptualized,
motivation led the traveler to choosing both fast (β = .09, t = travelers may set the focal-level goals for slow tourism
2.35) and slow (β = .68, t = 17.5) travel modes, yet choosing (motivation) and strive for maximizing attainment of some
the latter far more prevalently. Motivation also positively superordinate goals through optimal choices of, for example,
affected goal attainment (β = .62, t = 11.45). While slow their tempo, rhythm, and pace of travel. Our data show that
travel mode contributed significantly to slow goal attainment in a slow tourism context, motivations lead to choosing both
(β = .32, t = 5.72), fast travel mode did not (β = –.05, t = fast and slow travel modes and also positively contribute to
–1.79). Fast travel mode was negatively associated with all attaining slow tourism goals. As expected, slow travel mode
three criterion constructs (p <.01), that is, satisfaction (β = contributes much more powerfully to slow tourism goals
–.23), return intention (β = –.18), and referral intention (β = than fast travel mode; the contribution of fast travel mode to
–.21), whereas slow travel mode’s relationships with them slow tourism goal attainment was marginally negative.
all were essentially null (p > .10). Attaining slow tourism Nonetheless, fast travel mode appears to be a significant,
goals contributed significantly and positively to the tourist’s direct determinant of general tourism experience outcomes,
satisfaction, return intention, and referral intention (p <.01). while slow travel mode influenced them only indirectly
The model explained little in the variance of fast travel mode through slow tourism goal attainment. These indirect effects
(1%), 46% in slow travel mode, 76% in slow tourism goal were all statistically significant and positive (β = .20, t = 4.99
attainment, 43% in satisfaction, 30% in return intent, and on satisfaction; β = .16, t = 4.69 on return intent; β = .21, t =
43% in referral intent, respectively. 4.99 on referral intent). Thus, the slow tourism process (i.e.,
We considered two possible models rivaling the proposed slow tourism motivation → slow travel mode → slow tour-
model. First, just as motivation could make the tourist strive ism goal attainment) appears to predict the criterion vari-
for goals, so could it do for general tourism experience out- ables, establishing both nomological and criterion validities
comes. Testing such a premise required allowing direct paths of the model. Combined, these results appear to provide a set
from motivation to tourist satisfaction, future return inten- of empirical evidence supporting for the sheer presence of
tion, and referral intention. This fully recursive model the slow tourism phenomenon.
resulted in essentially the same goodness of fit with RMSEA Interestingly, our data show that fast travel mode has a
= .064 (±.003), NNFI = .98, and CFI = .98 (χ2 = 2516.53, df direct, negative effect on the three criterion variables (see
= 466). The three newly added parameters were all statisti- Table 4). These criterion variables have been widely used to
cally significant, but all in the negative direction (–.61 for gauge performances of tourist destinations in various
satisfaction, –.56 for return intent, and –.48 for referral research programs including those of mass tourism. Most
intent). These negative signs were inconsistent with the first- slow tourism researchers characterize such mass tourism as
order correlations shown in Table 3, suggesting the operation fast paced travels and, thus, fast travel mode could be posi-
of potential suppression effects (Maassen and Bakker 2001). tively associated with these general tourism outcomes.
Hence, this model was rejected in favor of the proposed However, that was not the case in our data. Traveling in a fast
model. pace may not only interfere with attainment of slow tourism

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


Oh et al. 215

Table 3.  Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviation of the Model Constructs.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
 1. Relaxation 1.00  
 2. Self-reflection 0.72 1.00  
 3. Escape 0.78 0.57 1.00  
 4. Novelty seeking 0.34 0.60 0.39 1.00  
 5. Engagement 0.59 0.76 0.56 0.67 1.00  
 6. Discovery 0.55 0.64 0.53 0.70 0.74 1.00  
 7. Fast travel –0.05 0.15 –0.09 0.22 0.04 0.04 1.00  
 8. Slow travel 0.71 0.59 0.58 0.28 0.52 0.48 –0.14 1.00  
 9. Self-enrichment 0.56 0.74 0.47 0.55 0.64 0.56 0.10 0.60 1.00  
10. Revitalization 0.66 0.58 0.60 0.42 0.55 0.56 –0.16 0.68 0.71 1.00  
11. Satisfaction 0.34 0.31 0.32 0.24 0.35 0.39 –0.25 0.48 0.48 0.63 1.00  
12.  Future intention 0.27 0.24 0.34 0.24 0.32 0.24 –0.19 0.41 0.42 0.52 0.64 1.00  
13.  Referral intention 0.36 0.30 0.37 0.28 0.40 0.38 –0.23 0.47 0.48 0.61 0.79 0.79 1.00
Mean 4.09 3.64 4.22 3.78 3.85 4.06 3.58 3.90 3.85 4.18 4.47 4.47 4.53
SD 0.88 1.03 0.83 1.01 0.93 0.86 1.10 0.87 0.87 0.81 0.72 0.80 0.74

Table 4.  Results of Hypothesis Tests. claim that it is not applicable to every tourism context (e.g.,
Dickinson and Lumsdon 2010). Based on the results of this
Paths β SE t Ratio
study, we argue that slowness is more than mere anti-speed
Motivation → Fast travel 0.09 0.04 2.35 (the correlation between fast and slow modes was –.14),
Motivation → Slow travel 0.68 0.02 17.50 more than refusal to the dominant logic of speed, and more
Motivation → Goal attainment 0.62 0.05 11.45 than those categorical niches of tourism (Fullagar, Markwell,
Slow travel → Goal attainment 0.32 0.10 5.72 and Wilson 2012). It is a mental, psychological, and behav-
Slow travel → Satisfaction –0.04 0.07 –0.53 ioral process embedded in a vast majority of goal-driven
Slow travel → Return intent 0.00 0.08 –0.02 consumption activities, including much of the tourism phe-
Slow travel → Referral intent –0.05 0.08 –0.79 nomenon. In other words, both fast and slow modes of action
Fast travel → Goal attainment –0.05 0.03 –1.79 taken are on the same continuum of all human behaviors,
Fast travel → Satisfaction –0.23 0.02 –6.60
conscious or unconscious, across time and space, constantly
Fast travel → Return intent –0.18 0.02 –5.10
interplaying as options flexibly chosen for maximal goal
Fast travel → Referral intent –0.21 0.02 –6.14
attainment. Indispensable, therefore, is a multidisciplinary,
Goal attainment → Satisfaction 0.63 0.04 9.95
phenomenological in-depth understanding of slowness from
Goal attainment → Return intent 0.51 0.04 7.89
Goal attainment → Referral intent 0.65 0.04 10.04
the behavioral perspective of individual and group tourists.
This approach also needs to be incorporated into the study of
Note: R2: Fast travel mode = .01; slow travel mode = .46; slow mobility and modality in the tourism context (e.g., Hannam,
tourism goal achievement = .76; satisfaction = .43; return intent = .30; Butler, and Paris 2014; Lew and McKercher 2006).
recommendation = .43.
Slow tourism motivations and goals seem to share much
commonness with general tourism motivations reported in
goals but also undermine the tourist’s satisfaction and inten- the literature. As reviewed earlier, most slow tourism–spe-
tions to return and recommend. In contrast, the slow tourism cific motivations found through our primary research
process was significantly conducive to these tourist experi- appeared to be quite similar to those used to study tourists in
ence outcomes. We suspect that the slow process might have various travel contexts. Nevertheless, these motivations
been a driving force, indeed, of tourist satisfaction and accounted for little variance in fast travel mode (1%), while
behavioral intention reported in previous studies, especially they explained 46% of variance in slow travel mode.
of mass tourism, even if its role was not identified in those Moreover, while the slow tourism process contributed posi-
studies. Hence, this issue appears to be a broadly neglected tively to the model, explaining 76% of variance in slow tour-
aspect in tourism research, calling for further investigations. ism goals and, to a lesser degree, tourist satisfaction (43%),
This study also provides evidence supporting the onto- return intention (30%), and referral intention (43%), fast
logical value of slowness in the tourism phenomenon and for travel model did not affect slow tourism goals and negatively
future theoretical efforts. Many researchers view slow tour- contributed to the three general tourism outcomes. Thus, in
ism rather categorically as a niche phenomenon or market our data the slow tourism process seems to establish both
(e.g., Lipman and Murphy 2012; Moore 2012) and even nomological and criterion validities, and the strong

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


216 Journal of Travel Research 55(2)

explanatory ability of the slow tourism process seems to with faster forms of tourism in any unit of travel. Slow travel
imply that the slow tourism phenomenon may not be just an mode in this sense was viewed as a striving process channel-
emerging concept, but it may be the phenomenon that has ing slow tourism-specific motivations toward maximizing
existed ubiquitously without being properly recognized. chance to realize slow tourism goals. Data covering four
The proposed model produced expected results in gen- popular tourist destinations with diverse fast and slow tour-
eral, but drew attention to two aspects. First, consistent with ism opportunities revealed a powerful explanatory ability of
the first-order correlations in Table 3, the relationships the slow tourism process. This study adds to the literature a
between fast travel mode and the three criterion constructs new layer of understanding about speed, mobility, temporal-
were in the negative direction. This means that although the ity, and pace, the tourist’s goal-oriented practice of time, and
tourist may adopt fast travel mode in mass tourism contexts, the implications of fast and slow travel modes. We argue that
such a choice is likely to undermine his/her satisfaction and slowness is an option and process permeated in almost all
future intentions. Second, the direct paths from slow travel tourism activities and, in fact, it may serve as both a motiva-
mode to the three criterion constructs were negative, incon- tion and goal for what Iso-Ahola (1982) called “escape” to
sistent with the correlations in Table 3. This inconsistency travel. Our proposed model and approach also extend
could be caused by either model misspecification, especially Weaver’s (2013) “enlightened mass tourism” in that we do
in the absence of specific theories, or an operation of slight not view mass and slow tourism as separate entities to syn-
suppression effects due to the correlations among the predic- thesize; rather, we showed that slow tourism might have
tors of the criterion constructs (Maassen and Bakker 2001). already existed as a quintessential part of mass tourism.
Nonetheless, these effects were essentially zero (i.e., statisti- This study leaves several issues to address future investi-
cally insignificant, with negligible effect sizes) and, thus, gations. First, goal-driven consumption theory needs addi-
they could be practically constrained to zero for correct tional investigations into other possible model specifications.
model solutions. A reestimation of the model with these For example, Bagozzi and Dholakia (1999) elaborate the
effects fixed at zero yielded essentially identical results in all goal-driven process in further detail, including five steps and
the remaining aspects of the model. The effects of slow mode feedback reactions. We adapted our goal-driven slow tour-
on the criterion constructs may be mediated completely by ism model to three main steps and feedback-like criterion
slow tourism goals, even if their first-order correlations were variables. Thus, our model may be limited in its explanatory
of moderate magnitude. power to the extent that such an abbreviated goal-driven pro-
The results provide multiple implications for destination cess omitted any key information as well as is incompatible
marketers, tourism planners, and policy makers. First, they with the original model specification of goal-driven
should not treat slow tourism as a concept independent of or consumption.
opposite to faster forms of tourism. Instead, both slow and Second, the proposed model needs more rigorous evalua-
fast tourism should be viewed as coexisting, complementary tions. One way to do so is to identify and incorporate into the
means to achieving travel goals. For that reason, they need to same model a central fast (mass) tourism process. Such an
pay attention to both aspects in their destination design and expanded model may allow various comparisons between
promotion. Second, they need to approach slow tourism the slow and fast tourism processes, thereby providing valu-
from the perspective of the traveler’s behavioral process able insights into the underlying behavioral mechanisms of
rather than defining forms of travel and type of travelers by tourism. As partially revealed in this study, the fast tourism
the characteristics of the destination visited. Either faster or process may function as a countervailing process to slow
slower modes of travel are what the tourist subjectively tourism, but still conducive to explaining tourist experience
determines to engage and they have different implications and destination performance. Model complexity may become
for various travel outcomes. Key is a tourist-centered, pro- a challenge in this case. Another way is to test the model
cess-oriented behavioral understanding of the phenomenon. across more diverse destinations that may involve varying
Third, offering speedy tourism opportunities needs addi- degrees of temporal practices during travel because of desti-
tional consideration regarding their potential consequences. nation characteristics. Inclusion of additional destinations
Although this study unveiled a damaging impact of fast- with different characteristics and target markets may offer
paced travel mode on the tourist’s experiential outcomes in more enlightening understandings of the temporal
one aspect, its effects may be reaching farther in broader phenomenon.
tourism contexts. Third, temporality, which can be practiced as either fast or
slow, necessitates clear definitions and operationalization in
the tourism context. The literature is unclear about how slow
Concluding Remarks
is really slow, and vice versa. In this study, we took a subjec-
This study provides a new perspective on the ontological sta- tive approach letting the traveler determine their own mental
tus of slowness, and slow tourism, in a broad context of tour- speed or tempo of traveling. This subjective approach, albeit
ism. Based on goal-driven consumption theory, we more direct and accurate than that by forms of transportation,
conceptualized slow tourism as a phenomenon coexisting makes it also difficult to judge and measure the actual speed.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


Oh et al. 217

Moreover, the function of temporality may not be linear. References


Both fastness and slowness may have threshold points. That Anderson, J. C., and D. W. Gerbing. (1988). “Structural Equation
is, too fast or too slow temporality, often imposed by various Modeling in Practice: A Review and Recommended Two-Step
travel situations and conditions, may not produce linear Approach.” Psychological Bulletin, 103 (3): 411-23.
effects in the expected directions; indeed, excessive skew- Bagozzi, R. P., and U. Dholakia. (1999). “Goal Setting and Goal
ness in temporality may yield adverse effects. The negative Striving in Consumer Behavior.” Journal of Marketing, 63
impact of fast travel mode on general tourism outcomes (special issue 1999): 19-32.
could be a case in point; fastness may be viewed positively as Bagozzi, R. P., and P. R. Warshaw. (1990). “Trying to Consume.”
long as it is interpreted as convenience rather than loss of Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (2): 127-40.
Bagozzi, R. P., and Y. Yi. (1988). “On the Evaluation of Structural
control or superficial engagement, or vice versa. There might
Models.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 16 (1):
also be a zone of temporality within which the traveler does 74-94.
not or cannot distinguish fast from slow speed. Adding the Baker, D. A., and J. L. Crompton. (2000). “Quality, Satisfaction
temporality dimension and the tourist’s practice of such tem- and Behavioral Intentions.” Annals of Tourism Research, 27
porality to the existing tourism mobility research will be a (3): 785-804.
fruitful avenue for future investigations. Beh, A. W., and B. L. Bruyere. (2007). “Segmentation by Visitor
Another revealing issue for future research relates to the Motivation in Three Kenyan National Reserves.” Tourism
conceptual overlap of the slow tourism process with existing Management, 28 (6): 1464-71.
tourism motivation models, as noted earlier. Although our Bettman, J. R. (1979). An Information Processing Theory of
qualitative study subjects provided motivations and goals Consumer Choice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
pertaining to slow travel modes, these motivations and goals Boomsma, A. (2008). “Teacher’s Corner: Reporting Analyses of
Covariance Structures.” Structural Equation Modeling, 7 (3):
could be viewed as a subset of various tourism motivation
471-83.
models reported in the literature. Such an overlap in results Bowen, D., and J. Clarke. (2009). Contemporary Tourist Behavior:
attests to our argument again that slow tourism may not exist Yourself and Others as Tourists. Wallingford, UK: CABI.
as a form or process independent of other forms or processes Chernev, A. (2004). “Goal Orientation and Consumer Preference
in travel. Counterintuitively, too, such an overlap implies a for the Status Quo.” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (3):
potential sharing of deep-seated human desires among vari- 557-65.
ous practices of time including slow mode in travel. Studies Cohen, E. (1979). “A Phenomenology of Tourist Experiences.”
are needed to delve into deeper levels of behavioral processes Sociology, 13, 179-201.
underlying slow, fast, or other temporal practices to refine the Crompton, J. (1979). “Motivations of Pleasure Vacations.” Annals
travel motivation process. Projective research techniques of Tourism Research, 6 (4): 408-24.
such as laddering in means end models may be instrumental. Dick, A. S., and K. Basu. (1994). “Customer Loyalty: Toward an
Integrated Conceptual Framework.” Journal of the Academy of
The practice and study of time, and temporality, promise
Marketing Science, 22 (2): 99-113.
a fertile ground for future tourism research. Traditionally, Dickinson, J., and L. Lumsdon. (2010). Slow Travel and Tourism.
time has been a concern more with economic and engineer- London: Earthscan.
ing researchers. As leisure cannot be properly conceptual- Dickinson, J., and P. Peeters. (2012). “Time, Tourism Consumption
ized independent of labor, so is the practice of leisure time and Sustainable Development.” International Journal of
away from that of work life. In that sense, slow tourism and Tourism Research, 16 (1): 11-21.
its conceptual potential signal the similar importance and Fodness, D. (1994). “Measuring Tourist Motivation.” Annals of
interest for systematic research. Future studies need to refine Tourism Research, 21 (3): 555-81.
our understanding of the slow tourism phenomenon from the Fornell, C., and D. F. Larcker. (1981). “Evaluating Structural
behavioral perspective and continue improving the measure- Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and
ment scales for both theoretical advances and practical appli- Measurement Error.” Journal of Marketing Research, 18 (1):
39-50.
cations. These efforts may be productive with “slow”
Fullagar, S., K. Markwell, and E. Wilson. (2012). Slow Tourism:
approaches. Experiences and Mobilities. Bristol, UK: Channel View.
Groenendaal, E. (2012). “Slow Tourism Initiatives: An Exploratory
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Study of Dutch Lifestyles Entrepreneurs in France.” In Slow
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect Tourism: Experiences and Mobilities, edited by S. Fullagar,
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. K. Markwell, and E. Wilson. Bristol, UK: Channel View, pp.
201-13.
Gutman, J. (1982). “A Means-End Chain Model Based on
Funding Consumer Categorization Processes.” Journal of Marketing,
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support 46 (2): 60-72.
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Hall, M. C. (2012). “The Contradictions and Paradoxes of
Caesars Hospitality Research Center Grant #6020, University of Slow Food: Environmental Change, Sustainability and the
Nevada Las Vegas. Conservation of Taste.” In Slow Tourism: Experiences and

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


218 Journal of Travel Research 55(2)

Mobilities, edited by S. Fullagar, K. Markwell, and E. Wilson. by S. Fullagar, K. Markwell, and E. Wilson. Bristol, UK:
Bristol, UK: Channel View, pp. 53-68. Channel View, pp. 170-84.
Hannam, K., G. Butler, and C. M. Paris. (2014). “Developments McCabe, S. (2009). “Who Needs a Holiday? Evaluating Social
and Key Issues in Tourism Mobilities.” Annals of Tourism Tourism.” Annals of Tourism Research, 36 (4): 667-88.
Research, 45 (2): 171-85. Milfont, T. L., and R. Fischer. (2010). “Testing Measurement
Higgins, E. T. (2002). “How Self-Regulation Creates Distinct Invariance across Groups: Applications in Cross-Cultural
Values: The Case of Promotion and Prevention Decision Research.” International Journal of Psychological Research,
Making.” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12 (3): 177-91. 3 (1): 111-21.
Howard, C. (2012). “Speeding Up and Slowing Down: Pilgrimage Moore, K. (2012). “On the Periphery of Pleasure: Hedonics,
and Slow Travel through Time.” In Slow Tourism: Experiences Eudaimonics, and Slow Travel.” In Slow Tourism:
and Mobilities, edited by S. Fullagar, K. Markwell, and E. Experiences and Mobilities, edited by S. Fullagar,
Wilson. Bristol, UK: Channel View, pp. 11-24. K. Markwell, and E. Wilson. Bristol, UK: Channel View,
Hu, L., and P. M. Bentler. (1999). “Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes pp. 25-35.
in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus Novak, T. P., D. L. Hoffman, and A. Duhachek. (2003). “The
New Alternatives.” Structural Equation Modeling, 6 (1): 1-55. Influence of Goal-Directed and Experiential Activities on
Iacobucci, D., and G. A. Churchill. (2010). Marketing Research: Online Flow Experiences.” Journal of Consumer Psychology,
Methodological Foundations (with Qualtrics Card), 10th edi- 13 (1/2): 3-16.
tion. Mason, OH: Cengage Learning. Oh, H., A. M. Fiore, and M. Jeong. (2007). “Measuring Experience
Iso-Ahola, S. E. (1982). “Toward a Social Psychological Theory Economy Concepts: Tourism Applications.” Journal of Travel
of Tourism Motivation: A Rejoinder.” Annals of Tourism Research, 46 (November): 119-32.
Research, 9:256-62. Oh, H., and C. H. C. Hsu. (2014). “Assessing Equivalence of
Kim, J. H., J. R. Ritchie, and B. McCormick. (2010). “Development Hotel Brand Equity Measures in Cross-Cultural Contexts.”
of a Scale to Measure Memorable Tourism Experiences.” International Journal of Hospitality Management, 36 (1):
Journal of Travel Research, 51 (1): 12-25. 156-66.
Klenosky, D. B. (2002). “The ‘Pull’ of Tourism Destinations: A Means- Oliver, R. (1999). “Whence Consumer Loyalty?” Journal of
End Investigation.” Journal of Travel Research, 40 (2): 385-96. Marketing, 63:33-44.
Kline, R. (2011). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation O’Regan, M. (2012). “Alternative Mobility Cultures and the
Modeling, 3rd edition. New York: Guilford. Resurgence of Hitch-Hiking.” In Slow Tourism: Experiences
Krippendorf, J. (1987). The Holiday Makers: Understanding the and Mobilities, edited by S. Fullagar, K. Markwell, and E.
Impact of Leisure and Travel. Oxford: Heinemann Professional. Wilson. Bristol, UK: Channel View, pp. 128-42.
Lam, T., and C. H. C. Hsu. (2006). “Predicting Behavioral Intention Parkins, W. (2004). “Out of Time: Fast Subjects and Slow Living.”
of Choosing a Travel Destination.” Tourism Management, 27 Time and Society, 13 (2/3): 363-82.
(4): 589-99. Parkins, W., and G. Craig. (2006). Slow Living. Sydney: University
Lee, T. H., and J. Crompton. (1992). “Measuring Novelty Seeking of New South Wales Press.
in Tourism.” Annals of Tourism Research, 19:732-51. Pearce, P. L., and U. Lee. (2005). “Developing the Travel Career
Lew, A., and B. McKercher. (2006). “Modeling Tourist Movements: Approach to Tourist Motivation.” Journal of Travel Research,
A Local Destination Analysis.” Annals of Tourism Research, 43 (1): 226-37.
33 (2): 403-23. Pearce, P.L., and J. Packer. (2013). “Minds On The Move: New
Li, M., and L. A. Cai. (2012). “The Effects of Personal Values Links From Psychology To Tourism,” Annals of Tourism
on Travel Motivation and Behavioral Intention.” Journal of Research, 40: 386-411.
Travel Research, 51 (4): 473-87. Ryan, C., and I. Glendon. (1998). “Application of Leisure
Lipman, M. B., and L. Murphy. (2012). “‘Make Haste Slowly’: Motivation Scale to Tourism.” Annals of Tourism Research,
Environmental Sustainability and Willing Workers on Organic 25 (1): 169-84.
Farms.” In Slow Tourism: Experiences and Mobilities, edited Singh, S. (2012). “Slow Travel and Indian Culture: Philosophical
by S. Fullagar, K. Markwell, and E. Wilson. Bristol, UK: and Practical Aspects.” In Slow Tourism: Experiences and
Channel View, pp. 84-98. Mobilities, edited by S. Fullagar, K. Markwell, and E. Wilson.
Lumsdon, L. M., and P. McGrath. (2011). “Developing a Conceptual Bristol, UK: Channel View, pp. 214-26.
Framework for Slow Travel: A Grounded Theory Approach.” Sirgy, M. J. (2010). “Toward a Quality-of-Life Theory of Leisure
Journal of Sustainable Tourism, 19 (3): 265-79. Travel Satisfaction.” Journal of Travel Research, 49 (2):
Maassen, G. H., and A. B. Bakker. (2001). “Suppressor Variables 246-60.
in Path Models: Definitions and Interpretations.” Sociological Snepenger, D., J. King, E. Marshall, and M. Uysal. (2006).
Methods and Research, 30 (2): 241-70. “Modeling Iso-Ahola’s Motivation Theory in the Tourism
Maddox, R. N. (1985). “Measuring Satisfaction with Tourism.” Context.” Journal of Travel Research, 45 (3): 140-49.
Journal of Travel Research, 23 (3): 2-5. Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M., and H. Baumgartner. (1998). “Assessing
Maruyama, M., and G. Parker. (2012). “‘Fast Japan, Slow Japan’: Measurement Invariance in Cross-Cultural Consumer
Shifting to Slow Tourism as a Rural Regeneration Tool in Research.” Journal of Consumer Research, 25 (June):
Japan.” In Slow Tourism: Experiences and Mobilities, edited 78-90.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016


Oh et al. 219

Uriely, N., Y. Yonay, and D. Simchai. (2002). “Backpacking Author Biographies


Experiences: A Type and Form Analysis.” Annals of Tourism
Haemoon Oh is a professor and Head of the Department of
Research, 29 (2): 520-38.
Hospitality and Tourism Management, Isenberg School of
Waller, J., and S. E. G. Lea. (1998). “Seeking the Real Spain?
Management, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Authenticity in Motivation.” Annals of Tourism Research, 25
(4): 110-29. A. George Assaf is an Associate Professor in the Department of
Weaver, D. B. (2013). “Asymmetrical Dialectics of Sustainable Hospitality and Tourism Management, Isenberg School of
Tourism: Toward Enlightened Mass Tourism.” Journal of Management, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Travel Research, doi:10.1177/0047287513491335.
Yoon, Y., and M. Uysal. (2005). “An Examination of the Effects Seyhmus Baloglu is a professor and Harrah Distinguished Chair in
of Motivation and Satisfaction on Destination Loyalty: A the William F. Harrah College of Hotel Administration, University
Structural Model.” Tourism Management, 26 (1): 45-56. of Nevada, Las Vegas.

Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at ANAHUAC on June 13, 2016

You might also like