You are on page 1of 4

BRIEF OR NEW

T
he process of referring students to occupational
therapy often needs clarification as individual
school districts struggle to define who does and
does not qualify for this service (Drobnyk & Siciliano,
Poor Handwriting: Who 1989), Although the situation is more straightforward for
students with obvious or severe disabilities (Kauffman,
is Referred? 1988), it is less clear for students lacking a specific diagno-
sis and for those with mild disabilities, These students
may not receive the help they need, especially in their
early years of schooling. Additionally, educators some-
Judith E. Reisman
I times have difficulty understanding the value of a medi-
cally oriented profession in an educational setting, be-
Key Words: education, special. tests, by title, cause "education is often product oriented" ,and
occupational therapy tends to be process oriented"
Minnesota Handwriting Test
(American Occupational Therapy Association [AOTA],
1989, p, 147),
School occupational therapists may see only those
students who have edUCationally related problems
(AOTA, 1989; Education For All Handicapped Children
Act [Public Law 94-142], 1975), Thus, children with con-
ditions such as poor motor coordination or postural in-
stability are not referred to occupational therapy in a
school setting, The problems that occupational therapists
address must be framed in terms of academic tasks,
Handwriting problems often serve as this academi-
cally relevant route to occupational therapy (Oliver,
1990), The demands of the task are easily understood to
be within both the educator's and occupational thera-
pist's domains, While the teacher is primarily responsible
for handwriting instruction, "the therapist's role is to de-
termine underlying postural, motor, sensory integrative,
or perceptual deficits that might interfere with the devel-
opment of legible handwriting" (Stephens & Pratt, 1989,
p, 321), Intervention for identified deficits may then in-
volve both professions.
Although therapists use both standardized tests and
clinical judgment when assessing a student's needs, stan-
dardized testing is urged whenever possible (AOTA,
1989; Carr, 1989), To document progress as well as defi-
cits in handwriting, we need tests that proVide quantita-
tive as well as diagnostic information. The Denver Hand-
writing Analysis (Anderson, 1983) and the Children's
Handwriting Evaluation Scale are examples of tests that
evaluate cursive writing (Phelps, Stempel, & Speck,
1982),
Fewer tests of printing are available. Perhaps the
most commonly used test for this purpose is the Chil-
dren's Handwriting Evaluation Scale for Manuscript Writ-
ing (Phelps & Stempel, 1987), This test is advantageous in
that it applies to both conventional manuscript and
D'Nealian (Thurber, 1981) print and reports separate
scores for rate and quality of printing for first and second
Judith E. Reisman, PhD, OTR, is Assistant Professor, Program in
graders, Unfortunately, the scoring system is not well
Occupational Therapy, University of Minnesota, Box 388
UMHC, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455.
defined, Another test, the Diagnosis and Remediation of
Handwriting Problems (Stott, Moyes, & Henderson,
This article was accepted for publication December 3, 1990. 1984), has more detailed scoring instructions and pro-

The American journal of Occupational Therapy 849


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 02/02/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
vides information on specific writing faults. This test, 1. Croup I-Students in regular education class-
however, requires that students have at least 2 years of rooms who were receiving no special education
writing instruction, which generally limits its use to third and who achieved a handwriting score of > - 1.0
graders and above. SD from the mean of the entire regular education
Some broad academic assessments include the eval- group (n = 428 [out of 484 regular-education
uation of a handwriting sample that is usually elicited for students])
other purposes, such as language skills (Brigance, 1978; 2. Croup 2 - Mainstreamed students in the same
Markwardt, 1989; Woodcock &Johnson, 1989). The scor- regular classrooms who spent part of the day in
ing gUidelines tend to rely heavily on rater judgment With- special education and who did not receive occu-
out clear scoring criteria. Several other tests rate legibility pational therapy (n = 30).
through comparison with a graded series of samples 3. Croup 3 - Students in regular classrooms receiv-
(e.g., Larsen & Hammill, 1989; Zaner-Bloser, 1984). Al- ing no special education but who were identified
though this method provides a quick screen for compari- as having poor handwriting. P00r handwriting
son with a standard, the specific elements of handwriting was defined by a score on the Minnesota Hand-
are not quantified. This limits its sensitivity to change and, writing Test of at least 1.0 SD below the mean of
therefore, its usefulness to therapists. the total regular education sample (n = 56). Pick-
The Minnesota Handwriting Test (Reisman, 1987), ard and Alston (as cited by Alston & Taylor, 1987)
which proVides a more quantitative measure of handwrit- have recommended the use of - 1.0 SD as the
ing in the early grades, was developed to address some of cutoff point for identification of students with
these concerns. Because the route of referral to occupa- handwriting problems.
tional therapy usually begins with the classroom teacher, 4. Croup 4- Mainstreamed students in the same
I have assumed that the teacher's proficiency in judging regular classrooms who spent part of the day in
handwriting quality affects the decision of who will re- special education and who received occupational
ceive intervention for this perceived problem. In the pres- therapy for problems contributing to poor hand-
ent study, I compared teachers' judgment of poor hand- writing, as stated on their IEPs (n = 51). Because
writing with scores on the Minnesota Handwriting Test this study occurred at the beginning of the school
(pilot version). The study question was as follows: Is year, many students were just entering occupa-
there a significant difference between the Minnesota tional therapy. Others had received occupational
Handwriting Test scores of students referred to occupa- therapy intervention before reaching second
tional therapy for poor handwriting and the scores of grade and were carried over because it was deter-
those students who are not referred? mined that they continued to need this interven-
tion to reach the IEP goal of improved
handwriting.
Method
Subjects Instrument
All of the second graders (N = 565) in public elementary The Minnesota Handwriting Test is a near-point copy task
schools in 27 classes served as the subjects. This was a (i.e., each test paper contains the stimulus words). To
convenience sample based on the involvement of 14 oc- take the test, students copy a sample containing the
cupational therapists throughout the country who were words (in mixed order) for the common sentence, "The
willing to participate in the study. Each class contained at quick brown fox jumped over the lazy dogs." Samples are
least one special education student who was receiving given in either manuscript or D'Nealian printing style to
occupational therapy for problems contributing to poor fit the instructional style of each classroom. In the pilot
handwriting, as stated on the individualized education version of the test, the samples were not timed.
program (IEP). All of the students were in the classroom Scoring of the samples requires the application of
during the handwriting instruction periods. No attempt specific criteria and the measurement of individual let-
was made to control the amount or type of instruction the ters. Each letter may receive up to 4 points, which are
students received. Nine classes of students, therefore, awarded if the letter meets the criteria for legibility, size,
were taught the D'Nealian method of printing, whereas form, and conformity to the baseline. If a letter fails to
the remaining 18 classes were taught manuscript print- meet the legibility criterion, then scoring is discontinued
ing, such as the Zaner-Bloser (1984) style. No student for that letter and it loses all 4 points. The objective here
received additional handwriting instruction in occupa- is to give greater weight to legibility while still recognizing
tional therapy. The study population was divided into other common areas of difficulty. Points ar~ also deleted if
four groups based on Minnesota Handwriting Test scores word and letter spacing fail to meet specific criteria.
and teachers' referral or nonreferral for special education Interrater reliability on the pilot version of the scor-
intervention, as appropriate: ing directions was established with the use of four raters,

850 September 1991, Volume 45, Number 9

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 02/02/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms


who each scored 30 samples. The correlations among Table 1
pairs of raters ranged from 0.77 to 0.88. The raters' errors Post Hoc Comparisons Between Pairs of Groups of
were of two types: misinterpretation of scoring directions Second-Grade Handwriters
and the human errors of measurement and failure to Comparison Groups M Difference Scheffe F Test"
follow directions. A revised version of the scoring direc- Group 1 vs. 2 8.628 8.771
tions is being developed to address both issues. Group 1 vs. 3 27.526 145.093
Group 1 vs. 4 21.405 95338
Group 2 vs. 3 -18898 28347
Group 2 vs. 4 12.776 13399
Procedure Group 3 vs. 4 - 6.122 4203
No/e. 1 = regular education students, 2 = special education students,
After obtaining consent for participation, an occupational 3 = poor handwriters in regular education, 4 = occupational therapy
therapist or teacher read the standardized instructions to students.
a classroom of students. The students wrote their names *p <01.
on the test sheets and then copied the stimulus words.
After the test, an adult recorded the appropriate category
of group membership (e.g., regular education, special Those students in special education who were not
education) using a prearranged coding system. The stu- referred to occupational therapy also seemed appropri-
dents' names were cut from the papers to preserve their ately placed. Their scores were significantly higher than
anonymity. those of the students with poor handwriting both in regu-
Most of the tests were scored by the occupational lar education and in occupational therapy, although their
therapists in each cooperating school district. A few of the scores were still somewhat lower than those of most of
classes were scored by research assistants hired for the their classmates in regular education. Although some Stu-
study. All data were sent to me, and I then performed a dents in the regular classroom who have poor handwrit-
single-factor analysis of variance on the students' scores ing should perhaps be referred to special education, most
to determine any signiftcant differences among the are appropriately placed. Anderson (1983) noted that
groups (Abacus Concepts, 1986). This was followed by teachers often have students in their classrooms who
Scheffe post hoc analyses (Abacus Concepts, 1986) for a have poor handwriting but have no other problem that
comparison of scores of each group with every other interferes with academic performance.
group. In summary, it seems that only those students with
the poorest handwriting in the regular classroom are re-
ferred for occupational therapy. We must next examine
Results whether occupational therapy is effective in remediating
The Group 1 students (i.e., those in regular education handwriting problems. To be objective, we as occupation-
classes) had the highest mean test score, 95.8% (SD = al therapists must have quantitative measures. The Min-
3.5), whereas the Group 4 students (i.e., those receiving nesota Handwriting Test may prove to be a useful addi-
occupational therapy) had the lowest, with a mean score tion to the instruments that occupational therapists
of76.0% (SD = 16). The Group 2 students (i.e., those in employ in both assessment and research ....
special education who were not receiving occupational
therapy) had a mean score of 89.4% (SD = 9.5). The Acknowledgments
Group 3 students (i.e., those in regular classes who had
I acknowledge Julie Haugen, PhD, OTR, for statistical consultation;
poor handwriting but did not qualify for any form of Judith Bullock, OTR, and Patricia Haggerty, OTR, for ideas related
special education) had a mean score of 80.5% (SD = 5), to developing the Minnesota Handwriting Test; the occupation-
thus placing them between the special education stu- al therapists who participated as data collectors and scorers;
dents without handwriting needs and the occupational and the occupational therapy students who served as research
therapy students with handwriting needs. The differences assistants.
This study was supported in part by grants from the Ameri-
among the groups were statistically significant (F =
can Occupational Therapy Association and the University of
218.7, P < .0001). In addition, all post hoc comparisons Minnesota Undergraduate Research Opportunities Program.
were significant at the 99% level (see Table 1).

References
Discussion
Abacus Concepts. (1986). Statview 512 + [Computer pro-
Are teachers making appropriate referrals to occupation- gram]. Calabasas, CA: BrainPower.
al therapy? According to the scores obtained from the Alston, j., & Taylor, J. (1987). Handwn'ting: Theory, re-
search and practice. New York: Nichols.
Minnesota Handwriting Test, it seems that they are. Stu- American Occupational Therapy Association. (1989).
dents referred to occupational therapy achieved the low- GUidelinesfor occupational therapy services in school systems
est scores on this test. (2nd ed.). Rockville, MD: Author.

The American Journal of Occupalional Therapy 851


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 02/02/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Anderson, P. L. (1983). Denver Handwriting Analysis. No- writing readiness skills in elementary-age children. American
vato, CA: Academic Therapy. Journal of Occupational Therapy, 44, 111-116.
Brigance, A. H. (1978). Brigance Diagnostic Inventory of Phelps, J., & Stempel, L. (1987). Children's Handwriting
Early Development. North Billerica, MA: Curriculum Associates. Evaluation Scale for Manuscript Writing. Dallas: Texas Scot-
Carr, S. H. (1989). Louisiana's criteria of eligibility for occu- tish Rite Hospital for Crippled Children.
pational therapy services in the public school system. American Phelps, J., Stempel, L., & Speck, G. (1982). Children's
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 43, 503-506. Handwriting Evaluation Scale. Dallas: CHES.
Drobnyk, W., & Siciliano, S. (1989). A decision making Reisman, J. E. (1987). Minnesota Handwriting Test (Pilot
model for occupational therapy in the public schools. Fra- version). Unpublished manuscript.
mingham, MA: Therapro. Stephens, L. C, & Pratt, P. N. (1989). School work tasks and
Education for All Handicapped Children Act (Public Law vocational readiness. In P. N. Pratt & A. S. Allen (Eds.) Occupa-
94-142). (1975). 20 U.s.c., § 1401. tional therapy for children (pp. 311-334). St. Louis: Mosby.
Kauffman, N. A. (1988). Occupational therapy in the school Stott, D. H., Moyes, F. A., & Henderson, S. E. (1984). Diag-
system. In H. L. Hopkins & H. D. Smith (Eds.), Willard and nosis and Remediation ofHandwriting Problems. Burlington,
Spackman's occupational therapy (7th ed.) (pp. 707-741). Ontario: Hayes.
Philadelphia: Lippincott. Thurber, D. N. (1981). D'Nealian handwriting. Glenview,
Larsen, S., & Hammill, D. (1989). Test ofLegible Handwrit- IL: Scott-Foresman.
ing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1989). Wj-R Tests of
Markwardt, F. C. Jr. (1989). Peabody Individual Achieve- Achievement: Examiner's manual. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching
ment Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Resources.
Service. Zaner-Bloser. (1984). Zaner-Bloser Evaluation Scale:
Oliver, C. E. (1990). A sensorimotor program for improving Manuscript. Columbus, OH: Author.

Industrial Back Injury


Prevention and Management
Presented by H. Duane Saunders, M.S., P.T.

1991 Seminars
Atlanta, GA Thursday, January 24
Cleveland, OH Thursday, February 7
Las Vegas, NV Thursday, February 21
Tampa, FL Thursday, March 7
Presented by:
Bridgeport, CT Thursday, March 21
Minneapolis, MN Thursday, April 18
Educational Baltimore, MD Thursday, April 25
Detroit, MI Thursday, May 16
Opportunities St. Louis, MO Thursday, ~une 6
Philadelphia, PA Thursday, July 18
A Saunders Group Company Portland, OR Thursday, August 8
Dallas, TX Thursday, September 12
Buffalo, NY Thursday, October 17
1·800·654·8357 Denver, CO Thursday, October 31
NewOrleans, LA ..Thursday, November21
Phone For More Information San Jose, CA ... Thursday, December 12

852 September 1991, Volume 45, Number 9

Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 02/02/2018 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms

You might also like