You are on page 1of 10

Advanced Review

The development of stereotyping


and exclusion
Kelly Lynn Mulvey, Aline Hitti and Melanie Killen∗

This article reviews the developmental science literature on stereotyping and


exclusion, with a focus on gender, race, and ethnicity. Stereotyping of others,
which is defined as the attribution of traits to individuals based on group
membership, is often used to justify exclusion of others in social group contexts.
This review includes a focus on the links between these two constructs. Research on
stereotyping and exclusion has drawn on several theoretical traditions, including
social domain theory, social identity developmental theory, and subjective group
dynamics theory, which are also discussed in the context of the research
findings. Key findings on stereotyping include categorization and classification in
relationship with decreased in-group bias, and the role of stereotypes in encoding
information. Findings on exclusion include the use of available information to make
judgments, preferences for in-group members who are normative and out-group
members who are deviant, the increased importance, with age, of group functioning
in exclusion decisions, and decreased negative evaluation of in-group members
who partake in exclusionary behaviors. Though little research has explicitly studied
the links between stereotyping and exclusion from groups, this review describes
the current literature in both areas and suggests future directions for research. 
2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. WIREs Cogn Sci 2010 1 597–606

S tereotyping emerges in childhood and contributes


to processes related to inclusion and exclusion.
This is because exclusion from groups is often justified
childhood and play a role in how children and
adults reason about and interact with others. Social
psychologists focus on stereotypes as the cognitive
on the basis of stereotypic expectations.1–3 Individuals components influencing intergroup behaviors. In this
who do not fit the stereotypic expectations of a review,8 due to space limitations, we will focus on
group (and the group identity) are vulnerable to cognitive and social-cognitive aspects of stereotyping
being rejected and excluded by the group. Because and exclusion rather than including emotional and
exclusion is related to negative social consequences affective dimensions as well.
in childhood and adulthood, it is important to Stereotypes play a fundamental role in discrim-
understand how and why stereotyping emerges in inatory actions, such as exclusion from peer groups,
development, and the links to processes of exclusion. which children and adolescents experience throughout
This review of the developmental science literature their lives. Inclusion and exclusion in an intergroup
will focus on stereotyping and exclusion in childhood context, which is defined as an interaction with mem-
and adolescence, and provide a foundation for bers of an out-group3 occurs when children relate to
understanding issues of prejudice in development. their peers in different ways based on their group mem-
Stereotyping reflects cognitive categorizing bership, such as their ethnicity or gender.1 Although
about the world. Cognitive structures include one’s not necessarily detrimental, exclusion can have long-
perception of beliefs, knowledge, and expectations term negative impacts on children who are excluded;
about different social groups,4–7 which develop during for instance, exclusion can lead to long-lasting strug-
gles with depression, anxiety, and social withdrawal.1
Research on stereotyping in childhood has
∗ Correspondence to: mkillen@umd.edu typically centered on gender as well as race/ethnicity.
Department of Human Development, 3304 Benjamin Building, Although there is a long history of studying stereotypes
University of Maryland, College Park, MD, 20742, USA about gender and race/ethnicity in adult contexts,9
DOI: 10.1002/wcs.66 less is known about the connection between the

Vo lu me 1, Ju ly /Au gu s t 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 597


Advanced Review wires.wiley.com/cogsci

development of stereotyping and exclusion based on which includes actions that are contingent on socially
gender and race/ethnicity in childhood. The study agreed upon rules that are alterable and that, in their
of the development of stereotyping and exclusion absence, will cause no direct harm to be inflicted on
in childhood is based on developmental theories another; or the psychological domain, which includes
of cognition, including social-cognition and moral actions related to personal choices and preferences
judgment. Beginning with Piaget (1932/1965), the (for a review of theory refer to Smetana (2006)). A
field has differentiated into several recent frameworks. key element of social domain theory, which holds
For this review, we will focus on two theoretical relevance for the study of stereotypes and exclusion,
models, social domain theory10,11 and social identity is the central focus on children’s reasoning and
theory.12 the recognition that children do distinguish, from
very early ages, between behaviors and acts which
inherently center on issues such as fairness and justice
THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS and those which promote group functioning and
OF STEREOTYPING AND EXCLUSION social interactions (for a review, see Killen, Sinno,
RESEARCH and Margie (2007)). This theory has been applied to
Piaget (1932/1965) conducted foundational research the topic of stereotyping and exclusion in several ways,
on the development of social cognition, which he as described below, with the predominant focus being
defined as the thought process required for making the forms of reasons (e.g., moral, social–conventional,
judgments about the morally correct action in a psychological) children and adolescents use to justify
specific situation. A lack of a moral viewpoint or reject exclusion decisions, and how stereotyping
inevitably leads to prejudice, given that acting on is explained by children using social–conventional
prejudicial beliefs, such as those often embodied in reasons (e.g., traditions, customs, norms). Moreover,
stereotyping, violates moral principles of equality and exclusion and stereotyping research has centered on
fairness.13 Thus, Piaget’s research on the development contextual variations, stemming from social domain
of moral principles is relevant for understanding theory, to understand when and how these constructs
the emergence of stereotyping and exclusion. Piaget, emerge in development.
relying on an interview methodology, determined While moral principles are required to reject
that children’s morality develops as an outcome of exclusion based on stereotyping, group identity
social and cognitive abilities. As children begin to emerges during childhood, which provides a com-
interact with others and form peer relationships, they peting consideration for children, one that has the
develop more complex understandings of equality, potential to enhance stereotyping of others. A cen-
reciprocity, and respect. Piaget’s work, in particular, tral theoretical framework which bears on the study
his focus on the importance of peer relationships in
of stereotypes, exclusion, and social groups is social
the development of moral reasoning in children, bears
identity theory.12 According to social identity theory,
on the study of stereotyping and exclusion as these
individuals strive to maintain their in-group identity
arise in the context of peer interaction and intergroup
by viewing their own social group more positively
relations.
than other social groups, and to identify with social
Social domain theory11,14 builds from this
groups with positive social status. Attempts to see
early theory with its focus on social cognition but
deviates from the global stage model by (1) providing the in-group in increasingly positive ways can lead
a domain-specific account of social development to prejudice toward members of out-groups.12 Self-
and (2) demonstrating the emergence of morality in categorization theory (SCT Turner et al.,16 ) extends
early childhood. Extensive research has determined this concept of self-identification on the basis of
that children’s social reasoning is organized by cognitive grouping.2 Therefore, people place them-
three domains, the moral, social–conventional, and selves in a group that they view as most similar
psychological, which co-occur in development and to themselves based on some classification label,
do not reflect an ordered hierarchical, stage-like which is cognitively contrasted with another classi-
trajectory.11,15 Research driven by this theory has fication. Such self-categorization emphasizes positive
shown that children from a very young age (3 years) similarities between individuals of the in-group, thus
construct their thinking about the social world by promoting in-group bias, while also focusing on the
differentiating their experiences based on either the negative differences of the out-group which may lead
moral domain, which includes actions that have to out-group prejudice. This process, then, creates
implications for someone else’s welfare, fairness, opportunities for the development of stereotypes and
justice, and rights; the social–conventional domain, acts of exclusion based on group membership.3

598  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. Vo lu me 1, Ju ly /Au gu s t 2010


WIREs Cognitive Science Development of stereotyping and exclusion

STEREOTYPING AND recalling counter-stereotypic information is highly


CATEGORIZATION AS PRECURSORS correlated with both children’s racial stereotyping and
OF EXCLUSION their classification skills, and not related to a child’s
poor attention, or lower intelligence.19 Therefore,
Research, specifically, on stereotyping has framed children’s retrieval of information about a category
stereotypes as linked to the ability to cognitively or group is influenced by stereotypes and their ability
organize the social world into categories and examines to classify an individual into one or multiple groups.
how stereotypes are used to rationalize behavior with Yet, while children become capable of classification,
respect to these categories.17–19 In a study on in-group stereotyping persists into adulthood.2,22 Thus, other
favoritism and out-group prejudice with 4–7 year explanations are required to explain the trajectory of
olds, Aboud20 found correlations between children’s
stereotyping through development.
classification skill and their scores on the Preschool
Stereotype research with adults suggests that
Racial Attitude Measure.21 Children who had higher
people use stereotypes as recall cues.23 Stereotypes are
multiple classification skills, and were thus able to
used when both encoding and retrieving information
classify stimuli into multiple categories, displayed
thus increasing the number of associative paths to
lower in-group favoritism.20 Thus, this study indicated
stereotypic information.24 Similar processes may be at
that, in fact, children’s ability to classify may help
work in children’s stereotyping processes as well. The
to mitigate the development of stereotypes. This is
relation between stereotypes and memory in children,
because children were able to simultaneously attribute
however, has mostly been studied in the context
both positive and negative attributes to the same child,
of children’s eye-witness testimony. Young children’s
decreasing out-group negativity.
encoding, storage and retrieval of information about
Bigler and Liben18 tested a similar hypothesis
events are highly influenced by stereotypes and
considering the effects of teaching children (5–10 years
of age) multiple classification skills on gender interviewer suggestibility.25 Two studies that assess
stereotyping and information processing. Further the effects of stereotypes on children’s testimony
supporting Aboud’s20 finding that classification skill propose that it is the schematic nature of stereotypes
may decrease stereotyping, prior to the intervention, that helps children remember them.25,26 Stereotypes
a significant positive correlation was found between a provide information about individuals, which assist
child’s classification skills and their egalitarian gender children in organizing their memory of specific
attitudes.18 After intervention, results showed that categories or groups. Research on recollection of
those children who acquired multiple classification categories has shown that children do use schemas
skills and who were trained specifically on social to help them remember, and are more likely to
categories improved the most on their egalitarian remember thematic categories based on functionality
responses. than taxonomic lists.27
In a measure assessing participants’ memory This thematic nature of stereotypes facilitates the
for occupations that were gender counter-stereotypic, encoding of information collected from experiences
those who were better classifiers remembered more or the environment, and helps with retrieving
counter-stereotypic information than those who were information from memory. Stereotypes may also add
not. This did not differ between those who received to information that has not been perceived and
training classifying social categories or those who were misrepresent information that has been perceived.28 It
trained with non-social categories, thus suggesting is worth noting that the relationship between memory
that multiple classification skills help children to and stereotypes is twofold; while on the one hand,
process counter-stereotypic information even if they stereotypes influence a child’s memory, the processes
have stereotypic attitudes. In another study assessing and strategies used to remember also lend themselves
individual differences in memory of race-related to schematic biases that in turn invoke stereotypes.
stereotype-consistent versus stereotype-inconsistent These memory processes to some extent facilitate the
information, Bigler and Liben19 found another use of stereotypic information in children’s judgments.
correlation between 4- and 9-year-old children’s However, memory as well as judgment is also driven
classification skills and their ability to process specific by the inferences that people make based on what they
information. know. This has been documented when adolescents
Using a classification task that included race, evaluate peer retribution: in a study which showed
gender, and age as possible categories Bigler and that when participants were asked to make judgments
Liben19 found that children’s ability to classify was about punishment based on ambiguous information,
negatively correlated with their stereotypic responses. adolescents were more likely to rate punishment
They also concluded that the problem children had for stereotype-consistent transgressions (e.g., jocks

Vo lu me 1, Ju ly /Au gu s t 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 599


Advanced Review wires.wiley.com/cogsci

breaking the sound system at a party) as more fair than what role stereotypes and stereotypic information
stereotype-inconsistent transgressions (e.g., computer plays in this process.
club members breaking the sound system at a party).29 Recent research on exclusion has focused
Drawing on previous literature concerning on children’s social and moral cognition, drawing
people’s concepts of intelligence and its impact on on social domain theory11,15 to investigate the
self-judgment, Dweck et al.30 , Dweck and Leggett,31 reasoning used by children in making inclusion and
Levy and Dweck32 applied the idea of the lay person’s exclusion decisions. The methods used in social
static (entity) versus dynamic (incremental) theory domain research provide a platform for testing
to children’s stereotyping. They hypothesized those children’s judgments about social and moral issues
who adopt an entity theory and view their social in an intergroup context.13 Participants in this line
structures as fixed are more likely to stereotype, of research are asked to judge and reason about
while those who adhere to an incremental theory hypothetical scenarios of fairness and justice in
and understand their social structures to be malleable which the target’s group membership is manipulated.
are less likely to stereotype. Examining this theory in Children and adolescents have been shown to elicit
adults and 11–13 year olds,32,33 findings showed that the use of stereotypes when making their judgments
entity theorists who believed that people’s attributes about inclusion and exclusion1 as well as peer
are fixed displayed higher levels of stereotyping retribution,29,39 attributing intentions of others,40
than incremental theorists who thought of people’s explaining intergroup discomfort,41 and initiating
attributes as malleable. Studies about the impact of cross-group and cross-race friendships.42,43
people’s lay theories on stereotype formation based When asked to judge prototypic or straightfor-
on realistic groups have been conducted only with ward exclusion scenarios based on group membership,
adults, however, and limited research exists with such as gender and race/ethnicity, preschool aged
children.33,34 children are more likely to evaluate exclusion as
Recent research has suggested that these implicit wrong using equality and fairness reasons than using
theories about intelligence are one component of a social–conventional or personal justifications or rely-
broader phenomenon referred to as essentialism.35 ing on stereotypes.1,44,45 In complex or ambiguous
Essentialism, the tendency to assume shared invisible situations, however, children and adolescents will rely
traits are held by all members of a group,36,37 may also on stereotypes.1 For instance, Theimer, Killen, and
be at play in some instances of stereotyping,33,38 which Stangor (2001) analyzed European American chil-
can lead to exclusion. A focus on essentialist beliefs dren’s (mean age ∼ 6 years) evaluation of inclusion in
appears to impact the development of stereotypes as an activity based on gender-stereotypic expectations
well as prejudice in certain contexts because essential- of peer activities (e.g., doll-playing, truck-playing).
ism involves generalizing characteristics of an entire Results showed that in the absence of additional infor-
group to an individual member of the group. Such mation about experience with the activity, participants
generalizations can function to create stereotypes.38 chose to include the child who fit the stereotype of the
activity more often and used stereotypic reasoning in
justifying their answers (e.g., ‘Boys don’t know how
EXCLUSION AND GROUP NORMS to play with dolls and dolls are only for girls’ or ‘Girls
are quiet and they don’t like to play with trucks’).
Another explanation for the continuation of stereotyp- In continuation of this research, Killen et al.44
ing into adolescence and adulthood lies with the role investigated age differences (3–5 year olds) in assessing
that stereotyping plays in perpetuating group iden- children’s use of stereotypes in reasoning about similar
tity, group norms, and exclusion.1,3 Moreover, social inclusion scenarios as those used in Theimer, Killen,
conventional reasoning, identified by social domain and Stangor (2001) and counter-probed children
theory, often serves to justify stereotyping. Social iden- about their inclusion decision offering them an
tity theory proposes that enhancing the group requires alternative reason to include the child they did not
in-group favoritism and out-group negativity.3 Out- pick, thus testing their conviction in their decision.
group negativity may generate stereotyping as well as Probing revealed that most children who initially used
prejudice and bias. Children frequently base decisions stereotypic information to make their decision did
on who to include based on what they believe about change their minds to focus on moral judgment when
others, which is often founded upon the stereotypes given the opportunity (after a probe by the interviewer
that have developed as described above. Studying the in which the fairness of turn-taking was mentioned),
social reasoning used by children as they make inclu- while those who initially focused on the moral aspects
sion and exclusion decisions helps to unveil precisely of the situation did not change to consider the

600  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. Vo lu me 1, Ju ly /Au gu s t 2010


WIREs Cognitive Science Development of stereotyping and exclusion

stereotypic argument as often (after a probe by the or distribution of resources. This was compared
interviewer in which the stereotype about who plays with scenarios where information about individual
with toys was mentioned). However, the study did merit was given. In ambiguous scenarios, adolescents
show that younger children were more likely than relied on whatever information was available to
older children to choose the stereotypic child prior to make judgments, thus stereotypes served as a source
probing, and were more likely to base their judgment of information. Thus, Horn’s research reveals how
on stereotypic expectations. complexity, such as multiple considerations, and
Exclusion based on stereotypes has been studied ambiguity, such as a lack of individuating information,
with older children as well as in multifaceted contribute to stereotyping by adolescents regarding
scenarios, where children must weigh multiple types exclusion from social cliques.
of information in making exclusion decisions. Killen More recently, social domain theory on exclu-
and Stangor (2001) interviewed first, fourth, and sion has focused on race and ethnicity.47 A set of
seventh graders, using similar gender-stereotypic studies has examined other variables that contribute
activity-based scenarios (e.g., boys joining a ballet to why and how children and adolescents justify or
club and girls joining a baseball team), however reject exclusion, including social experience, such as
they also included multifaceted scenarios, where intergroup contact and cross-race friendships. These
children were given information about the skill variables stem from both Piagetian theory with his
level of the different possible targets for inclusion emphasis on peer interaction and social experience
and exclusion. They found that children rejected contributing to social knowledge, as well as social
straightforward exclusion and used moral reasoning psychological theory regarding intergroup contact. In
in judging exclusion as wrong. The children generally Allport’s17 foundation theory on the nature of preju-
chose the non-stereotyped child in the equal skill dice, he hypothesized that a set of conditions need to
context multifaceted situation, which suggests active be met for intergroup contact to reduce prejudice;48,49
inclusivity. Surprisingly, in-group favoritism was one of the strongest predictors for prejudice reduction
not found. Additionally, the researchers found that is cross-race friendships.49
most children only used one type of justification Crystal et al.50 tested whether intergroup con-
(social–conventional or moral). tact in the form of cross-race friendships was related
As expected, age-related differences were doc- to evaluations of interracial peer exclusion. Using
umented. Specifically, in the unequal skill context, three contexts that varied in intimacy (friendship,
both first and seventh graders favored the stereotyp- sleepover, dating), the findings indicated that both
ical child, but fourth graders did not. Most children ethnic majority and minority participants in fourth,
used group functioning (social–conventional reason- seventh, and tenth grade viewed interracial exclusion
ing) for justifying the unequal skill situation, but as more wrong when they had a higher level of inter-
first and fourth graders used more moral reasoning group contact, which was a measure that included
than did seventh graders. Finally, the study showed contact in classrooms, schools, and neighborhoods.50
gender differences, with girls showing a greater con- In another study, researchers demonstrated that inter-
cern for inclusivity. Overall, the findings indicated group contact in the form of cross-race friendships was
that, with age, group functioning (in the form of significantly related to the use of stereotypes by White
social–conventional reasoning) was provided as a European-American students in fourth, seventh, and
basis for exclusion. tenth grade to explain interracial peer discomfort.41
Horn46 built from this line of research, capturing In this study, participants were asked why it was that
the explicit use of stereotypes in making moral there might be discomfort in interracial interactions.
judgments about peer relationships in adolescence.46 Students with low reported levels of cross-race friend-
Using ambiguity in scenarios presented to ninth and ships were more likely to explain racial discomfort in
eleventh graders that focused on peer cliques (e.g., terms of stereotypes such as lack of shared interests
the ‘cheerleaders’ excluded a ‘gothic,’ the ‘jocks’ and levels of aggressiveness by the out-group than
excluded a ‘techie’), Horn46 found that such lack students with high levels of cross-race friendships.41
of information influenced the use of stereotypes by Finally, a comparison was made for how
adolescents. In this study when judging a scenario ethnic majority and minority US participants (fourth,
where only group membership information was seventh, tenth grades) evaluated interracial peer
provided, participants were more likely to rely on encounters in which the purported reason for
either purely moral concerns (‘It’s unfair to exclude the exclusion by the excluder was either ‘non-race’ such
gothic’) or only stereotypic information (‘A techie is as lack of shared interests, or ‘race-based’ such as
not athletic’) when making decisions about exclusion explicitly referring to race. Although all participants

Vo lu me 1, Ju ly /Au gu s t 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 601


Advanced Review wires.wiley.com/cogsci

used moral reasons, such as a lack of fairness and prejudicial attitudes toward the out-group and move
unequal treatment to evaluate race-based exclusion, them into the ethnic prejudice phase.54 Children ages
there were differences between the majority and 4–5 years old, according to SIDT, are likely in the
minority samples regarding the ‘non-race’ reasons ethnic preference phase and may be engaging in out-
for exclusion with minority participants viewing it group derogation.54 The theory proposes that the
as more wrong than majority participants (and using development of out-group derogation is dependent
more moral reasons). For example, minority students on several factors: the level of identification that a
viewed a White student not having lunch with a Black child has with his/her in-group, the extent to which
student because the Black student ‘did not like soccer’ prejudice is an in-group norm, and the extent to
as less wrong than did ethnic majority students, which the in-group members perceive the members of
who viewed this type of decision as a ‘personal the out-group as a threat.54–56
choice’ (in a manner similar to how same-race peer Nesdale et al.54 assessed SIDT’s prediction that
encounters would be evaluated). Ethnic minority children would actively dislike out-group members
students referred to reasons such as empathy and when the in-group had a norm of exclusion as
perspective taking, as well as reasons referencing a well as when out-group threat was present. Their
lack of fairness to evaluate exclusion as wrong. findings, which revealed that group norms of exclusion
Age-related differences emerged regarding the do lead to children developing more prejudicial
context. With age, participants viewed exclusion from attitudes and stereotypes toward the out-group,
a sleepover party as wrong, rejecting parental pressure provide support for much of SIDT.54 However, they
to keep a party same-race; at the same time, with found age-related changes not accounted for by SIDT:
age, participants viewed exclusion from a friendship when the in-group held a norm of exclusion, older
encounter as legitimate, accepting peer pressure to children expressed neutrality toward the out-group,
conform to same-race dating expectations. As a not outright dislike.54 Research outside of SIDT has
whole, these findings provide information regarding revealed that, with age, children are less likely to
how stereotypic expectations emerge, and are related attribute negative traits to the out-group.57 Thus, these
to evaluations of intergroup exclusion. Focusing on findings reveal that there may be greater complexity
group functioning becomes more salient for children, to individuals’ experiences and decision making with
with age, and particularly by adolescence. In fact, the regard to in- and out-groups.
roots of group identity begin in early childhood. Children use stereotypic information when mak-
As children begin to associate with social groups, ing decisions about how much they like in- and
they develop preferences for different groups based out-group members and, therefore, whether they will
on behaviors or traits of individuals within those include or exclude these members. Nesdale53 con-
groups, thus intergroup relations find a foothold in ducted a study focused explicitly on how children use
stereotypic beliefs which children form about other stereotypic information in making judgments about
groups.2,12,51,52 A theoretical framework that has how much they like or dislike in-group members,
looked at this process is social identity development with a focus on ethnic groups. Specifically, 8-, 10-,
theory (SIDT), which, while focused on ethnic and 12-year-old children were told stories about in-
prejudice, provides a foundation for thinking about and out-group members of different ethnic groups
intergroup contact and conceptions of the in-group (Australians and Vietnamese) who displayed behavior
and out-group in relation to the self. SIDT proposes that was either stereotype-consistent or stereotype-
that children move through four phases as they inconsistent for their ethnic group. Not only did
develop ethnic prejudice: undifferentiated, ethnic older children have better memory recall for the in-
awareness, ethnic preference, and ethnic prejudice.53 group member stereotype-inconsistent information,
SIDT is relevant for studying many forms of exclusion but also they liked the out-group member (who
(ethnic or otherwise) because of its analysis of group expressed stereotype-inconsistent information) better
relationships in the ethnic preference phase. than the in-group member who expressed stereotype-
Specifically, according to SIDT, as children inconsistent information. Younger children still pre-
enter the ethnic preference phase, they exhibit a ferred the in-group member, even when this member
concern with maintaining membership in the in- expressed stereotype-inconsistent information. Thus,
group and a strong focus on the identity of that children do actively use stereotypes in making deci-
in-group.54 SIDT would predict, then, that children sions about who they like.
in the ethnic preference phase would adhere tightly This finding leads directly to the work of Abrams
to in-group norms, including any in-group norms of and Rutland, who have looked explicitly at how
exclusion, which may lead to both stereotypes and one makes decisions about including or excluding

602  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. Vo lu me 1, Ju ly /Au gu s t 2010


WIREs Cognitive Science Development of stereotyping and exclusion

in- and out-group members who adhere to or resist of intergroup bias. Instead, it seems that with age,
group norms.3 Their work, which has focused on children are even more focused on group dynamics
the intersection of stereotyping and exclusion in an and their concern with being loyal to group norms
intergroup context, is founded upon the framework increases, which may perpetuate stereotypes and lead
of subjective group dynamics (SGD). SGD focuses on to greater exclusion. This finding is in contrast to
group identity and the relationship between judgments other SGD work which found that older children
about members of one’s in-group and judgments about showed decreased racial prejudice and stereotyping,
in-groups and out-groups generally.58,59 Specifically, due to public self-focus. In other words, older children
SGD examines children’s reasoning about in- and out- expressed less intergroup bias because of their percep-
group members who express deviant and normative tion that adults would not approve of such beliefs.62
attitudes toward the group.58 Thus, while adults can have a powerful impact on the
Research has found that children prefer individ- explicit expression of prejudicial attitudes, children
uals who express normative, or loyal, ideas about seem poised to continue exclusive behavior based on
the in-group, regardless of their group affiliation adherence to group norms.
and likewise express greater dislike for individuals
who deviate from the in-group norms.59 Additionally,
research on SGD has shown that, in moral situations, CONCLUSION
children use intergroup bias, which often has its foun- In conclusion, age-related changes regarding stereo-
dations in stereotypes, for group-based evaluations, typing and exclusion are multiple. A few readily
but not morality-based evaluations.60 SGD has also identifiable patterns are that: (1) with age, explicit
revealed that, with age, children adhere more to group stereotypes diminish in straightforward contexts but
norms and rely more on group functioning in decision are activated in complex exclusion situations; (2) with
making.60 Thus, SGD brings to the study of exclu- age, multiple forms of reasoning are brought to bear
sion a focus on the salience of group identity and on exclusion decisions, including moral, conventional,
an understanding of how individuals evaluate in- and and psychological reasons; (3) social identity increases
out-group members who are either loyal or deviant with age and contributes to in-group bias; (4) with age,
toward group norms. an understanding of group dynamics becomes more
Specific findings from within this theoretical complex and knowledge enables children to differen-
framework illuminate exactly how children use tiate in-group identity and social norms which bear
information about groups, specifically group norms, on exclusion; (5) group identity can both foster and
in making decisions about who to exclude. In one hinder stereotyping, depending on the context; and
study, focused on summer school-based groups, which (6) social experience in the form of intergroup contact
are minimal where children have not had time to and cross-group friendships can reduce prejudice and
form strong bonds or relationships with their in- increase tolerance.
group members, Abrams et al.59 found that children While much research has been conducted investi-
(6–7 year olds and 10–11 year olds) showed a strong gating the developmental trajectory of stereotypes and
in-group bias, even though the groups were minimal. exclusion, there are many gaps to be filled. Further
Additionally, older children were even more focused analysis is needed to understand what drives chil-
on differentiating their responses to loyal and deviant dren’s use of group functioning in their judgments.
in- and out-group members than younger children. Can it be explained by a stereotypic understanding
Thus, older children seem better able to consider that only people of the same social category can
multiple categories, which lead to a greater dislike function well together in a group, or are there other
for group members who deviate from their own in- reasons? Although the research reviewed touches on
group norms and greater like for group members some studies that look at developmental differences
who support in-group norms. Older children, then, in in moral reasoning, there is currently no line of work
some ways are able to overcome their conceptions of that tracks the frequency or contextual relevance of the
the out-group, which may be based on stereotypes, use of stereotypes developmentally. Further research
and value individual out-group members who express is necessary to document children’s use of stereotypes
deviant views, thus supporting the in-group. in moral reasoning and how this changes cognitively
Building on this study, researchers found, con- and contextually from early childhood to adolescence.
trary to earlier suggestions, that multiple classification This review has mapped the importance of
skill is not related to intergroup bias.61 In other words, categorization and classification skills to children’s
while children, with age, did develop better multiple use of stereotypes and their egalitarian attitudes.
classification abilities, it did not reduce their level More research is needed to investigate how and when

Vo lu me 1, Ju ly /Au gu s t 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 603


Advanced Review wires.wiley.com/cogsci

these skills bear on children’s conceptualization of and stereotypes are encoded, stored, and retrieved from
theorizing about social exclusion. memory.
It has been found that stereotypes add to or While exclusion literature has drawn extensively
distort children’s recollection of information, regard- on minimal groups and ethnic groups, more work
less of if the information is factual or false. The should be done drawing out exclusion based on
schematic process involved in remembering stereo- gender, religion, language, and culture, as these are
types and retrieving them from memory does provide areas of continued relevance in the increasingly global
some insight into the role of memory in invoking world. By investigating and classifying the reasoning
stereotypes in children. However, developmental sci- used by children and adolescents when making inclu-
entists can draw from the extant and vast research sion and exclusion decisions, researchers can identify
on stereotypes and memory processes with adults. how children interpret these social experiences, and
Social psychologist have conducted research with a thereby reveal the areas for change and intervention.
plethora of samples of university students to investi- Ultimately, the application of most of the research out-
gate the associative-network model and the schema- lined above should be in the development of effective
pointer tag model and how they can be applied to curricula to promote positive intergroup experiences
remembering expectancy-congruent and expectancy- and to reduce or diminish stereotypes, bias, and dis-
incongruent information,28 or similarly stereotype- crimination. A greater awareness of the connections
consistent and stereotype-inconsistent information. between stereotyping and exclusion will provide an
Creating age relevant measures and testing these mem- empirical database for the development of effective
ory processes in children can help further clarify how interventions designed to reduce prejudice.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
We acknowledge support from the National Science Foundation BCS #0840492.

REFERENCES
1. Killen M, Sinno S, Margie NG. Children’s experiences 7. Ruble DN, Martin CL, Berenbaum S. Gender devel-
and judgments about group exclusion and inclusion. opment. In: Eisenberg N, ed. Handbook of Child
In: Kail RV, ed. Advances in Child Development and Psychology: Vol. 3, Personality and Social Develop-
Behavior. New York: Elsevier; 2007, 173–218. ment. 6th ed. New York: Wiley Publishers; 2006.
2. Nesdale D. Social identity processes and children’s 8. Macrae CN, Stangor C, Hewstone M. Stereotypes and
ethnic prejudice. In: Bennett M, Sani F, eds. The Devel- Stereotyping. New York: The Guilford Press; 1996.
opment of the Social Self . New York: Psychology Press;
2004, 219–245. 9. Dovidio JF, Glick P, Rudman L, eds. Reflecting on the
Nature of Prejudice: Fifty Years After Allport. Malden,
3. Abrams D, Rutland A. The development of subjective MA: Blackwell; 2005.
group dynamics. In: Levy SR, Killen M, eds. Inter-
group Relations and Attitudes in Childhood Through 10. Killen M, Lee-Kim J, McGlothlin H, Stangor C. How
Adulthood. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2008, children and adolescents evaluate gender and racial
47–65. exclusion. Monographs of the Society for Research
in Child Development, Vol. 67(4). Oxford: Blackwell
4. Bigler RS, Liben L. A developmental intergroup the- Publishers; 2002.
ory of social stereotypes and prejudice. In: Kail R, ed.
Advances in Child Psychology. New York: Elsevier; 11. Turiel E. The Development of Social Knowledge:
2006, 39–90. Morality and Convention. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press; 1983.
5. McKown C, Weinstein RS. The development and
consequences of stereotype consciousness in middle 12. Tajfel H, Turner JC. An integrative theory of inter-
childhood. Child Dev 2003, 74:489–515. group conflict. In: Austin WG, Worchel S, eds. The
Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Monterey,
6. Ruble DN, Martin CL. Gender development. In:
CA: Brooks-Cole; 1979, 33–47.
Damon W, Eisenberg N, eds. Handbook of Child Psy-
chology. 5th ed., Vol. 3. New York: Wiley; 1998, 13. Killen M, Margie NG, Sinno S. Morality in the context
933–1016. of intergroup relationships. In: Killen M, Smetana JG,

604  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. Vo lu me 1, Ju ly /Au gu s t 2010


WIREs Cognitive Science Development of stereotyping and exclusion

eds. Handbook of Moral Development. Mahwah, NJ: 29. Horn SS, Killen M, Stangor C. The influence of group
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2006, 155–183. stereotypes on adolescents’ moral reasoning. J Early
14. Turiel E. The development of morality. In: Damon Adolesc 1999, 19:98–113.
W, ed. Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3 Social, 30. Dweck CS, Chiu C, Hong Y. Implicit theories and their
Emotional, and Personality Development. 5th ed. New role in judgments and reactions: a world from two
York: Wiley; 1998, pp. 863–932. perspectives. Psychol Inq 1995, 6:267–285.
15. Smetana JG. Social-cognitive domain theory: consis- 31. Dweck CS, Leggett EL. A social-cognitive approach
tencies and variations in children’s moral and social to motivation and personality. Psychol Rev 1988,
judgments. In: Killen M, Smetana JG, eds. Handbook of 25:109–116.
Moral Development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum 32. Levy SR, Dweck CS. The impact of children’s static
Associates; 2006, 119–154. versus dynamic conceptions of people on stereotype
16. Turner JC, Hogg MA, Oakes PJ, Reicher SD, formation. Child Dev 1999, 70:1163–1180.
Wetherell M. Rediscovering the social group: A self- 33. Levy SR, Stroessner SJ, Dweck CS. Stereotype forma-
categorization theory. Oxford, England: Basil Black- tion and endorsement: the role of implicit theories.
well; 1987. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998, 74:1421–1436.
17. Allport GW. The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: 34. Levy SR, Plaks JE, Hong YY, Chiu CY, Dweck CS.
Addison Wesley, 1954. Static vs. dynamic theories and the perception of
18. Bigler RS, Liben LS. Cognitive mechanisms in children’s groups: different routes to different destinations. Pers
gender stereotyping: theoretical and educational impli- Soc Psychol Rev 2001, 5:156–168.
cations of a cognitive-based intervention. Child Dev 35. Bastian B, Haslam N. Psychological essentialism
1992, 63:1351–1363. and attention allocation: preferences for stereotype-
19. Bigler RS, Liben LS. A cognitive-developmental consistent versus stereotype-inconsistent information.
approach to racial stereotyping and reconstructive J Soc Psychol 2007, 147:531–541.
memory in Euro-American children. Child Dev 1993, 36. Gelman S. The Essential Child: Origins of Essentialism
64:1507–1518. in Everyday Thought. New York: Oxford University
20. Aboud FE. The formation of in-group favoritism and Press; 2003.
out-group prejudice in young children: are they distinct 37. Rothbart M, Taylor M. Category labels and social
attitudes? Dev Psychol 2003, 39:48–60. reality: do we view social categories as natural kinds?
21. Williams JE, Best DL, Boswell DA. The measurement In: Semin GR, Fiedler K, eds. Language and Social
of children’s racial attitudes in the early school years. Cognition. London: Sage; 1992, 11–36.
Child Dev 1975, 46(2):494–500. 38. Arthur AE, Bigler RS, Liben LS, Gelman SA, Ruble DN.
Gender stereotyping and prejudice in young children: a
22. Rutland A. The development and self-regulation of
developmental intergroup perspective. In: Levy S, Killen
intergroup attitudes in children. In: Bennett M, Sani F,
M, eds. Intergroup Attitudes and Relations in Child-
eds. The Development of the Social Self . New York:
hood Through Adulthood. Oxford: Oxford University
Psychology Press; 2004, 247–265.
Press; 2008, 66–86.
23. Bodenhausen G. Stereotypic biases in social decision
39. Pitner RO, Astor RA, Bendenishty R, Haj-Yahia MM,
making and memory: testing process models for stereo-
Zeira A. Effects of group stereotypes on adolescents’
type use. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988, 55:726–737.
reasoning about peer retribution. Child Dev 2003,
24. Sherman JW, Frost LA. On the encoding of stereotype- 74:413–425.
relevant information under cognitive load. Pers Soc
40. McGlothlin H, Killen M. How social experience is
Psychol Bull 2000, 26:26–34.
related to children’s intergroup attitudes. In: Degner J,
25. Leichtman MD, Ceci S. The effects of stereotypes Dunham Y, eds. European Journal of Social Psychol-
and suggestions on preschoolers’ reports. Dev Psychol ogy: Special Issue: Children’s Intergroup Attitudes. In
1995, 31:568–578. press.
26. Memon A, Holliday R, Hill C. Pre-event stereotypes 41. Killen M, Kelly MC, Richardson C, Crystal DS, Ruck
and misinformation effects in young children. Memory MD. European-American children’s and adolescents’
2006, 14:104–114. evaluations of interracial exclusion. Group Processes
27. Lucariello J, Nelson K. Slot-filler categories as mem- Intergroup Relat. In press.
ory organizers for young children. Dev Psychol 1985, 42. Killen M, Rutland A. Social Exclusion, Prejudice, and
21:272–282. Morality in Children’s Lives. Oxford: Blackwell Pub-
28. Stangor C, McMillan D. Memory for expectancy- lishers. In press.
congruent and expectancy-incongruent information: a 43. McGlothlin H, Edmonds C, Killen M. Children’s and
review of the social and social developmental literature. adolescents’ decision-making about intergroup peer
Psychol Bull 1992, 111:42–61. relationships. In: Quintana S, McKown C, eds. The

Vo lu me 1, Ju ly /Au gu s t 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 605


Advanced Review wires.wiley.com/cogsci

Handbook of Race, Racism, and the Developing Child. 53. Nesdale D. Developmental changes in children’s ethnic
New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2007, 424–451. preferences and social cognition. J Appl Dev Psychol
44. Killen M, Pisacane K, Lee-Kim J, Ardila-Rey A. Fair- 1999, 20:501–519.
ness or stereotypes? Young children’s priorities when 54. Nesdale D, Maass A, Durkin K, Griffiths J. Group
evaluating group exclusion and inclusion. Dev Psychol norms, threat and children’s racial prejudice. Child
2001, 37:587–596. Dev 2005, 76(3):652–663.
45. Theimer CE, Killen M, Stangor C. Young children’s 55. Nesdale D, Durkin K, Maass A, Griffiths J. Threat,
evaluations of exclusion in gender-stereotypic peer con- group identification and children’s ethnic prejudice.
texts. Dev Psychol 2001, 37:18–27. Soc Dev 2005, 14:189–205.
46. Horn SS. Adolescents’ reasoning about exclusion from 56. Nesdale D, Griffith J, Durkin K, Maass A. Empathy,
social groups. Dev Psychol 2003, 39:71–84. group norms and children’s ethnic attitudes. J Appl Dev
47. Killen M, Henning A, Kelly MC, Crystal D, Ruck M. Psychol 2005, 26:623–637.
Evaluations of interracial peer encounters by majority 57. Aboud FE, Levy SR. Interventions to reduce preju-
and minority U.S. children and adolescents. Int J Behav dice and discrimination in children and adolescents.
Dev 2007, 31:491–500. In: Oskamp S, ed. Reducing Prejudice and Discrimi-
48. Pettigrew TF, Tropp LR. Allport’s intergroup contact nation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates;
hypothesis: its history and influence. In: Dovidio JF, 2000, 269–293.
Glick P, Rudman L, eds. Reflecting on the Nature 58. Abrams D, Rutland A, Cameron L. The development of
of Prejudice: Fifty Years After Allport. Malden, MA: subjective group dynamics: children’s judgments of nor-
Blackwell; 2005, 262–277. mative and deviant in-group and out-group individuals.
49. Tropp LR, Prenovost MA. The role of intergroup Child Dev 2003, 74:1840–1856.
contact in predicting children’s inter-ethnic attitudes: 59. Abrams D, Rutland A, Cameron L, Marques JM.
Evidence from meta-analytic and field studies. In: Levy The development of subjective group dynamics: when
S, Killen M, eds. Intergroup Attitudes and Relations in-group bias gets specific. Br J Dev Psychol 2003,
in Childhood Through Adulthood. Oxford: Oxford 21:155–177.
University Press; 2008, 236–248. 60. Abrams D, Rutland A, Ferrell J, Pelletier J. Children’s
50. Crystal D, Killen M, Ruck M. It’s who you know that judgments of disloyal and immoral peer behavior: sub-
counts: intergroup contact and judgments about race- jective group dynamics in minimal intergroup contexts.
based exclusion. Br J Dev Psychol 2008, 26:51–70. Child Dev 2008, 79:444–461.
51. Aboud FE, Amato M. Developmental and socialization 61. Abrams D, Rutland A, Cameron L, Ferrell J. Older
influences on intergroup bias. In: Brown R, Gaertner SL, but wilier: in-group accountability and the develop-
eds. Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Inter- ment of subjective group dynamics. Dev Psychol 2007,
group Relations. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers; 2001, 43:134–148.
65–85. 62. Rutland A, Cameron L, Milne A, McGeorge P.
52. Horn S. Group status, group bias, and adolescents’ Social norms and self-presentation: children’s implicit
reasoning about treatment of others in school contexts. and explicit intergroup attitudes. Child Dev 2005,
Int J Behav Dev 2006, 30:208–218. 76:451–466.

FURTHER READING
Abrams D, Hogg MA, Marques JM, eds. The Social Psychology of Inclusion and Exclusion. New York: Psychology
Press; 2005.
Bennett M, Sani F. The Development of the Social Self . New York: Psychology Press; 2004.
Levy SR, Killen M, eds. Intergroup Attitudes and Relations in Childhood Through Adulthood. Oxford: Oxford University
Press; 2008.

606  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. Vo lu me 1, Ju ly /Au gu s t 2010

You might also like