You are on page 1of 31

CHAPTER 2

REVIEW OF LITERATURE

This chapter presents the review of literature related to the study. The review has been organized
into the categories of Risk Perception, Perception of Driving Tasks, Traffic Rules, Personal
Responsibility, Road Safety and Risky Driving Behaviour.

2.1 Risk Perception

Drivers’ risk perception affects road accidents. Risk perception is result of disposition, social-
cultural, economic and demographic factors. Literature review related to drivers’ risk perception
is as follows:

According to Brown and Groeger (1988) risk perception refers to the drivers’ experience of risk
in potential traffic hazards. Risk perception is determined by information of the potential hazards
in traffic environment, and on the ability of driver to perceive potential hazards resulting into
actual accidents. Inexperienced drivers overestimate their driving skills and underestimate the
risks in traffic.

Renge (1998) has described that excessive speed and low perception of risk are significantly
associated with road accidents. Young and inexperienced drivers have low risk perception
regarding inappropriate speeding as compared to experienced drivers. Rajalin (1994) has
identified that there is significant difference between young female and male drivers adopting
high speed. Speed affects the risk perception of drivers.

Kouabenan (2002) has shown that there is significant relationship between the knowledge of risk
perception and road accidents. Experienced drivers have high risk perception as compared to less
experienced drivers. Ma et al. (2008) have found that risk perception significantly indirectly
affects risky driving behavior. There is significant effect of risk perception and risk taking
attitude on risky driving behaviour.

Nordfjaern et al. (2011) have identified that Indian drivers have low risk perception towards road
accidents. Most of road accidents occur in low income countries.

19
Ponnaluri (2011) analyzed that there is negative relationship between risk perception and risk
taking behaviour. Rosenbloom et al. (2008) have described that high level of risk perception is
perceived by drivers after the training as compared to before training. Female and adult drivers
have high level of risk perception as compared to male and novice drivers. There is insignificant
effect of age and driving experience on perceived risk.

Moen et al. (2006) have shown that there is significant difference between risk perception of
male and female drivers. Female drivers are more worried and concerned about the risk as
compared to male drivers. Risk perception and risk taking behaviour have been significantly
influenced by age. Low risk perception has been found in young drivers than old drivers.

Parker et al. (1998) have pointed that there is significant association between driving behaviour
and risk perception. Cohn et al. (1995) have discussed that there is negative correlation between
risk perception and risk taking behaviour. Drivers having high risk perception show lower
probability of risk taking. Mayhew and Simpson (1995) have demonstrated that novice drivers
have inferior skill performance as compared to experienced drivers. Novice drivers possess low
skills of hazard perception, attention control, timesharing and regulation.

Hoare (2007) has described that driving behaviour is affected by different variables such as
optimism bias, age differences, cross-cultural difference gender differences, driving experience,
exposure, and seatbelt usage. It is also identified that dimensions of risk perception is
significantly influenced by driving behaviour, optimism bias, age differences, driving experience
and exposure.

Deery (1999) has described that novice drivers are unable to identify the risk situations as
compared to experienced drivers. Novice drivers have low risk perception in comparison to
experienced drivers. Novice driver perceives high risks in specific risk situation than experienced
drivers. There is significant difference between novice and experienced drivers regarding risk
perception. Hesketh and Godley (2002) have identified that speed perception is an important
ability for safe driving. Road accidents depend on perception of speed, distance and making a cut
in front incoming vehicle. Hakes and Viscusi (1997) have found that individual risk perception is
affected by education, income, health status and gender.

20
2.2 Driving Tasks

Drivers’ perception of driving tasks is related to road accidents. Road accidents are result of
three factors of driving tasks such as human, vehicle and environment. Available literature on
driving tasks is as follows:

Deery and Love (1996) have identified that there is significant difference between drivers’ age
regarding driving style and driving skill. Driving training improves driving skills but many
deficiencies have been found in driving training. Driving skill is concerned with driving task
such as use of steering wheel and time taken to respond in traffic hazards. Driving style is
concerned with decision making such as over time, driving speed and tailgating. Driving style
and driving skill significantly affect drivers’ safety level.

Naing et al. (2008) have observed that there are three level of driving tasks such as human,
vehicle and environment. At human level, there are many factors which are associated with
driving task such as loss of consciousness, acute medical condition, falling asleep, inattention,
distraction, cell phone use, emotion, careless, mood and aggressive driving. The vehicle level
includes high frequency driving tasks such as speed, acute technical failure and tire burst. The
environment level driving tasks includes traffic density, traffic flow, slippery road conditions,
temporary problems and adverse weather conditions. It is found that 64.6 per cent road accidents
occur due to lack of vigilance.

Hauer (1999) has explained that drivers’ attention decreases in stable driving situations such as
driving on a straight road and going around a bend, and intersections when drivers change
directions or overtake. Barkley (2004) has described that 72.9 percent distractions occur in
stabilized traffic situations. 11.1 percentage road accidents occur while changing direction of
vehicle and 6.7 per cent occur while overtaking the vehicle. Road accidents occur when, simple
driving situations allow driver to get distracted and perform another task at the same time.

Keskinen et al. (1998) have described that male drivers are more inattentive in accident situation
as compared to female drivers. It is also found that road travelers are more likely to be involved
in road accidents due to inattention in age group of less than 25 years and between 45 and 64
years. Bailly and Chapon (2006) have found that there is significant association between

21
inattention and driving experience regarding road accidents. It is seen that inexperienced drivers
are more sensitive to occurrence of road accidents.

Sagberg and Bjornskau (2006) have described that there is a significant difference between
novice male drivers and general drivers regarding hazard perception of driving tasks. Fuller
(2005) identified that speed influences the driving tasks condition and play an important role in
driving behaviour. Most of drivers follow high speed at turning point due to overestimating their
steering skills.

2.3 Traffic Rules

Traffic rules play an important role in reduces road accidents. Traffic rules have been
implemented by government for saving life of road users. Road accidents can be minimized by
strictly enforcement of law.

Tahir et al. (2012) have described that 25 percent of road traffic accidents cases occur due to
carelessness and 22 percent due to driving vehicle on wrong side. 45 percent cases of road traffic
accident occur with motorcycle, 34 percent with cars, and 2 percent with trucks and 1percent
with bus. Zegeer et al. (2012) have analyzed that most of road accidents occur due to poor traffic
law of enforcement. O.G. et al. (2009) have found that road accidents occur due to old age;
overtaking, and poor maintenance of vehicles. Driving after drinking and breaking the speed
limits are causes of road accidents. Sundstrom et al. (2012) have analyzed that there is significant
difference among ambulance drivers’ performance regarding overtaking. Bener et al. (2005)
have identified that most of drivers do not follow the traffic rules while driving.

Banik et al. (2011) have shown that drivers’ behaviour is responsible for increase in number of
road accidents due to exceeding the capability of driving and overtaking. Hassen et al. (2011)
have identified that 66 per cent of road traffic accidents occur due to overtaking and overloading.
Afukaar et al. (2003), Vanlaar and Yannis (2006) and Brookland et al. (2010) have observed that
speeding is responsible to increase 50 per cent of road traffic injuries. Yao and Wu (2011) have
identified that driving vehicle on wrong side is associated with road accidents.

Sabbour and Ibrahim (2010) have observed that driving styles, excessive speed, ignoring road
signs, driving opposite direction, non use of safety belt, risky conditions and fast driving are

22
significantly associated with male drivers. Parker et al. (1995) have described that road accidents
are the result of rules violation rather than errors. Bone and Mowen (2006) have described that
there is a significant relationship between leniency in regulation and road accidents. Highly
educated drivers show the negative attitude towards road safety as compared to less educated
drivers.

Clarke et al. (2002) have found that drivers’ higher education is not panacea to reduce road
accidents. Poor driving skills and lack of knowledge of traffic laws are identified as the causes of
fatal accidents. Rebellious attitudes and decision-making skills of young drivers’ are contributory
factors of road accidents.

Beirness (1993) has observed that seat belts significantly reduce the risk of drivers’ injury or
fatality while driving. Gibbons et al. (2006) has identified that 35 percent of road accidents occur
to drivers who do not use seat belt while driving.

Jorgensen and Polak (1993) have demonstrated that driving speed of drivers depends on
characteristics, attitude and ability of risk perception, knowledge of information regarding the
speed limit, probability of detection by law enforcement, severity of traffic penalties, and the
potential of time saving or loss. Low skilled drivers overestimate their abilities and drive fast.

Goodwin et al. (2006) have observed that positive driving behaviour is necessary rather than
encouraging drivers to obey traffic signals. It is estimated that 20 percent of road accidents
injuries can be reduced by camera enforcement. Reason et al. (1990) have described that drivers’
attitudes have been classified on the basis of errors and violations of rules.

Parker et al. (1995) examined that there is significant relationship among errors, violations, and
forgetfulness. Young and male drivers violate traffic rules that results into financial loss and loss
of life. It is also found that most of road accidents occur due to rules violation as compared to
error. Parker and Stradling (1998) described that there is a significant difference between male
and female drivers regarding violation of traffic rules. 40 percent of male drivers more violate
the traffic rules as compared to female drivers who violate traffic rules in 20 percent cases.
Different variables included in traffic rules violation are red lights crossing, tailgating, excessive
speed limit and racing.

23
2.4 Road Safety

Rate of road accidents is increasing every day. It is has noted that large numbers of people are
getting killed every year in road accidents and some of them become sufferer of life threating
problems such as mental disturbance, loss of money and loss of hand or legs. India has a very
huge population of road travellers such as two wheelers and four wheelers on roads. Public must
know about road safety. It can be achieved by implementation of road safety awareness
programs.

Silas et al. (2011) have observed that poor road conditions and maintenance, and culture are
contributory factors in road accident deaths. Atubi et al. (2012) have identified that the length of
roads, presence of road safety corps and population significantly influence road traffic accidents.
Road traffic accidents significantly increase with increase in length of roads (km). Large area is
likely to be involved in greater number of accident than small area due to higher traffic volumes.
Kardara et al. (1997) have observed that most of road accidents fatalities occur with drivers than
passengers and pedestrians. Assum (1997) has described that there is significant difference
between drivers’ attitude regarding traffic safety and speeding.

2.5 Personal Responsibility

Drivers’ personal responsibility plays an important role in reducing road accidents. It is


responsibility of every driver to follow traffic rules. Literature regarding drivers’ personal
responsibility is as follows:

Sliupas (2009) has demonstrated that most of road accidents occur due to vehicle breakdown.
The lengths of road section, average daily traffic and road barriers are strongly associated with
road accidents. Kircher and Andersson (2013) have pointed that 40 percent of trucks do not have
any seat belts installed. Most of truck drivers do not take proper rest after long drive. Tahir et al.
(2012) have described that speeding, carelessness, and wrong side driving are significantly
associated with road accidents. 25 per cent of road accidents occur due to careless driving and 22
per cent due to driving on wrong side. 45 percent cases of road accidents are associated with
motorcycle, 34 percent with cars, 2 percent with trucks, and 1 percent with buses. Lee et al.
(2011) have observed that there is significant difference between male and female drivers
regarding use of mobile phone. 36 percent of male drivers use mobile phone as compared to 30

24
percent female drivers while driving. 48 percent of young drivers use mobile phone during
driving as compared to 30 percent old drivers. Roehler et al. (2013) have observed that most of
drivers and passengers do not wear the helmet while driving. Some drivers feel uncomfortable
after wearing the helmet.

Zhao et al. (2009) have identified that male drivers are more involved in road accidents due to
driving old vehicle, without using seat belt and speeding. Male drivers take more risk during
night time on urban link. Hejar et al. (2005) have identified that there is insignificant relationship
between car drivers’ injuries and not using seat belt.

Lee et al. (2011) have found that 95 percent of road traffic deaths can be reduced by using seat
belts by drivers and passengers. Most of drivers do not use seat belt during driving. 91 percent of
car drivers wear seat belt while driving. 12 per cent of drivers feel discomfort and do not find it
necessary to wear seat belt during driving. There is significant difference between male and
female drivers to wear seat belt. 47 percent of male drivers wear seat belt as compared to 38
percent female drivers. Male drivers say that people should be free to choose whether to wear
seat belt or not as compared to female drivers. It is also identified that 67 percent of young
drivers do not wear seat belt while driving.

Rumar (1990) has identified that 50 per cent of car drivers stop vehicle at intersections marked
with a stop sign. It is also found that most of drivers stop cars too late at stop sign than at blinker.
There is insignificant difference in percentage of drivers who stop or slow down at stop sign or at
red blinker. Daily and Martin (1988) have described that many factors influence choice of travel
mode such as infrastructure or availability of public transport, economics and psychological
factors include perceptions of safety. Majority of public use public transport as compared to
private vehicle and most of students use own vehicle for travelling.

2.6 Risky Driving Behaviour

Risky driving attitude of drivers associate with road accidents. Risky driving behaviour can be
modified by enforcement of law. Literature has been reviewed related to risky driving behaviour
that is following:

25
Ma et al. (2008) have described that drivers’ attitude towards rules violation and speeding
significantly affect risky driving behavior. Risky driving behavior significantly indirectly
affected by risk perception. There is insignificant difference between taxi and bus drivers
regarding risk perception, attitude and driving behavior. Aggressive behaviour and normal
behaviour significantly affect road accidents in first two years. Low level of education does not
significantly affect crash rates. Taxi and bus drivers show positive attitude towards rules
violation and speeding. Risk perception and risk taking attitude significantly affect risky driving
behavior. Less concerned drivers have positive attitude towards rules violation and speeding.

Harre et al. (2000) have described that license is a ticket of freedom for drivers. Goodwin et al.
(2006) have identified that behavioural factors are responsible to increase road accidents. These
factors reduce capabilities of drivers on long term basis such as inexperience, aging, disease,
disability and alcoholism that result into increasing road accidents.

Hassen et al. (2011) have defined that most of drivers use mobile phone while driving. There are
three elements of drivers’ attitude as feeling, thought and behaviour. Behaviour can be measured
by drivers’ reaction in certain situation. Petridou and Moustaki (2002) have identified that there
are many factors such as drowsiness, fatigue, eating, acute stress and acute alcohol, intoxication
and temporary distraction affect drivers’ performance.

McEvoy et al. (2006) have observed that risk of crash increase with use of mobile phone. Drivers
are more likely to be injured in road accidents due to using mobile phone while driving.
Mangiaracina and Palumbo (2007) have described that smoking is more likely to be the cause of
road accidents than using mobile phone while driving.

Brison (1990) has identified that there is significant difference between smokers and non-
smokers regarding car accidents. Sacks (1994) have observed that smokers are 1.5 time more
involved in road accidents than non-smokers. It is found that drivers are distracted from driving
tasks as they engage in different activities such as using mobile phone, eating, drinking,
smoking, talking with passengers, and using audio equipment.

Gulliver and Begg (2004) have described that driver’ activities of eating and drinking while
driving increase crash risk.

26
Jenness et al. (2002) have identified that drivers’ performance is significantly affected by
different distraction activities like eating or operating CD player while driving. Bener et al.
(2005) have identified that careless driving is one of the most contributing factors in road traffic
accidents. 35 percent road accidents occur due to careless driving. Excessive speed is the second
factor in road accident after careless driving. May (2004) has identified that one third of fatal and
serious accidents occur due to excessive and improper speed. Banik et al. (2011) defined that
drivers are responsible for occurrence of accidents due to exceeding the capability of driving and
vehicle speed.

Laapotti and Keskinen (2004) have described that there is significant difference between male
and female drivers regarding controlling vehicle. 70 percent time, male drivers are found losing
control over vehicle. Most of male drivers show risky driving habits. Shope et al. (2001) have
found that young drivers are more likely to be involved in crashes fatality with property damage.
Young drivers’ lack of experience and maturity causes them to take more risk and overestimate
their driving skills.

Cooper et al. (1995) have shown that sixteen year old drivers are more often involved in error
fatalities. These drivers are involved in high speed crashes due to loose control or ran off road.
Gayer et al. (2000) have found that excessive speed reduces driver’s ability to negotiate curves or
objects on roadway, extends stopping distance, and increases distance a driver travels when
encountering dangerous situation. Speeding is main contributing factors in traffic crashes and
fatalities.

Miller and Taubman (2010) have observed that there is significant relationship between
speeding and crash involvement. It is also found that addition of speed increases the potential of
being involved in fatal motor vehicle accident.

Mayhew and Simpson (1995) have identified that there is significant difference between novice
drivers and experienced drivers regarding risky driving. Novice drivers show positive attitude
towards risky driving behaviour as compared to experienced drivers. The novice drivers have
low risk perception skills and do not know how to operate vehicle on road to minimize
possibility of a crash.

27
Regan et al. (1998) found that novice drivers’ abilities and skills can be improved through
training. Gulian et al. (1989) have underlined that drivers who show aggressiveness are more
involved in road accidents as compared to drivers with low aggressiveness. Iversen (2004) has
found that there is positive correlation between risk-taking attitudes and risky driving behavior.
Risk taking attitude predicts risky driving behaviour. There are many dimensions included in risk
taking attitude such as rule violations and speeding, careless driving, and drinking and driving.

Forester et al. (1984) have explained that speed is affected by drivers’ income, vehicle mileage,
age, motorcycle registration, and speed limit. Speed is positively related to income, mileage, age
and price of fuel; but negatively related to motorcycle registration and speed limits. Speed is
significantly and positively related to total motor vehicle fatalities. Francis et al. (2004) have
observed that driving behavior is significantly associated with safety attitudes and risk
perception. There is significant correlation between road traffic safety attitude and aggressive
driving behaviors and fast driving.

Bamberg et al. (2003) have described that risk perception is negatively correlated with risk-
taking behaviors. High risk perception of drivers shows low probability of taking part in that
behavior. Ying et al. (2010) have found that novice and inexperienced drivers are involved in
risky driving and talking on a cellular phone in some situations. Cellular phone tasks
significantly decrease driving performance for both novice and experienced drivers. Experienced
drivers are more benefitted from training as compared to novice drivers.

Klauer et al. (2006) have observed that 22 percent of crashes occur due to tasks distractions.
Dialing a hand-held cellular phone increases the risk of crash or near crash by 2.8 times, and
talking on a handheld device increases risk by 1.3 times. Sabey and Taylor (1980) described that
human factors contribute 95 percent in road accidents. Driving behavior is the most important
human factor in road accident. Driver, vehicle and environment factors play an important role in
road accidents.

Lawton et al. (1997) identified that most of road accident occur due to fault of drivers. It is also
observed that experienced drivers’ skills performance is better as compared to novice drivers’
performance. Novice drivers have low skills ability as their fail to identify hazard situation and
distraction while driving.

28
Hall (1997) has identified that risk-taking does not violate traffic laws but increases the risk of
being involved in a crash. Three dimensions of risk perception are emotion-based risk
perception, cognition risk perception and concern risk perception. Ulleberg and Rundmo (2003)
suggested that there is minor affect of risk perception on risky behaviour. It implies that risk
perception is significantly related to risky driving behavior and risk-taking attitudes. It is also
found that speeding attitudes significantly contribute in risky driving behavior.

Hijar et al. (2003) has described that male drivers are 2.6 times more likely to suffer from road
traffic injury than female drivers. Drivers in age groups 21 to 25 years take more risk while
driving. Mayhew and Simpson (1995) have described that proportion of old drivers are more
involved in road accident at intersections when entering traffic or crossing a main road.

Preusser et al. (1984) have found that novice drivers’ crash involvement can be minimized
through legal restrictions such as night driving curfews, minimum licensing age and alcohol
purchase age. Reimer et al. (2008) found that there is significant difference in preference for
cellular phone keeping by old age drivers and young drivers while driving.

Sarkar and Andreas (2004) have shown that driving training decreases errors and increases
vehicle control. Caird et al. (2008) have identified that use of a hand free device and any cellular
phone while driving is significantly associated with drivers’ distraction and unsafe driving
behaviour. It is also found that road user behaviour as drivers, pedestrian, cyclist, and passenger
is significantly affected by traffic rules and regulations. Drivers are involved in traffic crashes
due to physiological or mental states. The probability of road accidents increase when distracted
driving combines with bad road conditions and non-enforcement of traffic laws. It is described
that most of road accidents are occurred by drivers due to using unmaintained vehicle.

Teye-Kwadjo (2011) has found that road drivers’ behaviour and motor vehicles are influenced
by key factors such as physical and social environments. Poor roadway lighting affects visibility
of pedestrians. Weather conditions such as winter/snow make roads challenging. The values,
culture and religious beliefs in the social environment affect road user behaviour. Road accidents
are considered just punishments of God. It is also found that drivers’ characteristics and
environmental/situational factors increase road accidents. Different factors included in drivers’
characteristics are poor traffic risk perception, fatalism, aggressive driving, feelings of

29
invulnerability, inadequate driver training, and risk-taking tendency. The situational factors such
as bad road infrastructure and equipment, nature and bad condition of passenger-carrying
vehicles, and ineffective traffic law enforcement increase road accidents.

Stanton and Salmon (2009) have discussed that inadequate road infrastructure or poor
environmental conditions significantly affect road user behaviour and their performance. Factors
included in inadequate conditions are confusing layout, misleading signage, poor road surface
related conditions, weather conditions, lighting conditions, time of day, misleading or
inappropriate rules, and regulations.

Naing et al. (2008) have highlighted that lack of attention increases road accidents. Three types
of attention are identified such as inattention, distraction and competition for attention. Variables
included in inattention are lost in thought, distraction due to using mobile phone and third is
related to focusing on driving task like searching for road signs.

Waylen and McKenna (2002) have identified that there is significant difference between male
and female drivers regarding distraction. Male drivers are more likely to be involved in road
accident on turning point, during night and overtaking as compared to female drivers. It is also
found that female drivers are more involved in road accidents at intersection as compared to
male drivers.

McKenna and Farrand (1999) have found that there is significant difference between experienced
and inexperienced drivers regarding suffering considerable interference to hazard perception
skills in presence of a secondary driving task. Drummond (2000) has shown significant
difference between experienced drivers and novice drivers regarding detecting hazard situation.
It is found that poor hazard perception is significantly associated with risk of fatal or serious
crashes but not minor crashes.

Van and Landstrom (2006) have found that sleepiness or tiredness is a serious problem for road
accidents. Most of road accidents occur by drivers’ tiredness. One-third drivers occasionally
have to fight sleepiness while driving and 8 percent drivers occasionally feel head dozing while
driving. It is also found that one-fifth of drivers stop vehicle due to sleepiness.

30
De et al. (2008) have described that there is a positive correlation between traffic density and
road accident. Road accidents increase as traffic density increase. It is also found that heavy
traffic create problems and decrease safety as it blocks vision and reduces drivers’ ability to
change lanes.

Gullone and Moore (2000) have indicated that there is significant difference between old and
young drivers to be involved in crashes. It is found that experienced and inexperienced drivers
have significant difference regarding to be involved in road accidents. Factors included in road
accident are speeding, reckless driving, and alcohol. Risky driving behaviour such as close
following, dangerous overtaking, and failing to allow sufficient time to merge traffic lanes are
responsible for road accidents.

Hartos et al. (2001) have found that less driving experience increases risky driving behaviours
and road accidents among youths. Road traffic accident rates for newly licensed youngsters
remains high for the first few years. It is found that the rate of road traffic accidents is higher for
adolescents who begin to drive at young ages. Inexperience and immature drivers have higher
probability of being involved in road accident as compared to experience and old drivers.
Williams (1995) has described that youngsters are more likely to drive at night than older adults,
which increases risk of being involved in a road traffic accident and related injuries.

Harre et al. (2000) have described that young drivers are involved in road traffic accidents as
compared to old drivers. Young drivers take more risk during driving. It is found that youngsters
are more likely to engage in variation of unsafe driving behaviours such as tailgating, violating
traffic signs and signals, allowing too little time to merge, and speeding than older drivers.
Waylen and McKenna (2008) have found that there is significant difference between adolescent
male and female drivers regarding risky attitudes in reference to speeding and tailgating.

Wilson and Stimpson (2010) have identified that young drivers are more likely to be involved in
road accidents due to distracted driving than older drivers. Distracted driving behaviour is found
in young drivers. Drivers who engage in distracted driving are more likely to be involved in risky
driving behaviour. Bianchi and Summala (2002) have described four categories of risky driving
behaviour such as aggressive driving, substance use and driving, distracted driving and

31
committing moving violations while driving. Errors and misjudgments are not considered in risk
categories.

Elvik et al. (2004) have identified that excess or inappropriate speed is the main contributing
factors in serious vehicle accidents. It is found that there is positive correlation between speed
and risk. Elliot et al. (2005) have found that age and experience of drivers significantly affect
road accidents. Inappropriate speed on bad road condition is the strongest predictor of severe
crashes irrespective of the age and experience of drivers.

Lajunen et al. (2003) have described that most of road accidents are committed by young drivers
due to excessive speed of vehicle. Young drivers do not have the fear of loss of life. Most of road
accidents occur under the 25 years age. Excessive and inappropriate speeds have been identified
as the greatest contributing factor in single vehicle accident for young drivers.

Campbell and Stradling (2003) have found that the highest proportion of drivers in age groups of
21 to 29 years violate speed limit. Male drivers in the age groups of 16 to 25 years have been
identified to drive at very high speed. Speeding is common behaviour in young drivers. Fildes
and Leening (1991) found that speed choice is influenced by road width and number of lanes.
Drivers prefer high speeds on wide roads, multiple lanes, and clear road markers.

Warren (1982) has identified that different characteristics of road such as distance of visibility,
surface condition and aggregate, slope and bend, and cut influence drivers’ speed preference
under different environmental conditions. The aggregate and surface stability of roads
significantly affect crash rates.

Goldenbeld and Van (2007) have found that drivers are unable to identify road conditions or
hazards situation during speeding, and loose control on vehicle that result into road accidents. It
is also found that drivers have to take road condition problems to adjust vehicle speed in
different road environment.

Patton et al. (1995) has described that risk of crash increase two times when speed of vehicle
increase above 5km/hour of the speed limit. It is found that there is significant correlation
between excess speed limit and road accidents. Groeger (2006) has described that low driving
experience significantly affects a drivers’ ability to identify and control vehicle. Drivers’ age also

32
influence road accidents. Most of vehicle accidents occur with young drivers. Problematic tasks
for researcher that drivers experience increase with age and it is difficult to distinguish the
influence of experience on driving behaviour distinct from age.

Horswill et al. (2008) have found that drivers’ inability to judge road conditions increase risk of
crash involvement. There is significant relationship between poor road conditions and hazards,
and not making appropriate adjustments in driving behaviour. Most of road accidents are result
of low drivers’ experience and lack of perceptual training in hazard detection.

Braitman et al. (2008) have found that young and inexperienced drivers are more likely to be
involved in accidents as they fail to identify other vehicles at intersections. Renge (1998) has
described that driving experience significantly influences hazard awareness and perceived speed
low risk. It is also found that driving experience strongly influences on hazard detection ability,
and speed preference between newly licensed, novice and experienced drivers, and driving
instructors.

Young et al. (2008) described that aged drivers are more likely to be injured than younger drivers
in a road accident. Higher crash rate occurs with aged drivers due to poor visual, tiredness and
poor body activeness. Beck et al. (2002) have found that young drivers are more likely to be
involved in road accident as compared to older drivers. Young drivers are more involved in
crashes from fatality to property damage. They have limited driving experience and immaturity
and take more risk and overestimate their driving skill.

Blatt and Furman (1998) have found that there are many factors involved in road accidents such
as road design, limited sight distance due to hills and curves, mostly two-lane roads, and two-
way traffic. Accident rate increases due to high speed, less seat belt use, and delays in discovery
and arrival of emergency support.

Grantham (2000) has identified that drivers’ performance is affected by excessive speed.
Speeding reduces drivers’ ability to negotiate curves or objects at the road, extends the stopping
distance, and increases the distance a driver travels when encountering a dangerous situation.
Speeding is the most dominant contributing factors in road accident and fatalities. It is found that
speed limit is main technique to control vehicle operating speed but speed limits are not effective
in all driving situations. Many factors are involved in road accidents such as road design,

33
infrastructure, intelligent-vehicle and road related technologies, drivers’ behaviour particularly
old and new drivers.

Forester et al. (1984) described that speed is positively related to income, vehicle mileage, age,
and speed limit but negatively related to vehicle registration and speed limits. It is seen that age
and speed are significantly related to road traffic accidents.

Prabhakar et al. (1996) have identified that male drivers show greater risky driving behaviour
and involvement in road accidents as compared to female drivers. McCartt (2003) identified that
male drivers are significantly more likely to engage in speeding, drink-driving, and violating
traffic rules as compared to female drivers. Moore (1995) has observed that male drivers are
more likely to be involved in speeding and tailgating as compared to female drivers.

Begg and Langley (2001) have described that there is significant difference between young and
older drivers regarding risky diving behaviour. Dobbeleer et al. (2009) have explained that
drivers’ behaviour play an important role in road accidents. There are many factors such as over
speed, right of way offences, drunken driving, red light offences, traffic in opposite side of the
road, distraction and fatigue and illegal overtaking. It is also found that distraction and fatigue
affect driving skill and behaviour.

Home and Reyner (2000) found that 20 percent of road accidents occur due to drivers’ tiredness.
It is found that a large numbers of road accidents fatalities occur each year due to drivers’
sleepiness while at the wheel. Bone and Mowen (2006) have identified that road accidents occur
due to different factors such as health of a person, lack of attention and public, not following the
road rules, unsafe conditions on road area, unexpected acts of road users, failure of vehicles, and
poor maintenance of vehicle.

Boufous et al. (2010) have described that concentration and free from distraction reduce the road
accidents. Most of road accidents occur due to distraction problem of drivers. It is observed that
drivers’ concentration is districted by passengers. Dunbar et al. (2002) have found that most of
road accident occurs due to violation of traffic rules. Bike drivers mostly violate traffic rules as
compared to others motors vehicle drivers. If driver provides proper space to others drivers who
are overtaking, accidents can be avoided easily. Drivers can prevent road accidents by
concentrating more on the potential hazards.

34
Williams (2003) have described that there is significant relationship between tailgating and road
accidents. Responsibility of drivers to maintain a safe distance can avoid road accidents.
Tailgating increases irritation and anxiety of drivers. Sacks and Nelson (1994) have found that
poor road light conditions play an important role in increasing accidents at night. Visibility of
drivers in case of poor road light condition decrease concentration of drivers in night. It is also
found that road accident is not only reduced by supply of proper lighting condition but also by
introducing speed limit.

Evans and Wasielewski (1983) have described that most of drivers engage in discussion with
passenger or conductor while driving. Drivers’ conversation with passenger or conductor affects
drivers’ performance that result into road accidents. Summala et al. (1996) have described that
most of bus drivers face problem when they turn vehicle in high speed and create problem to
passenger and others. A large numbers of drivers take short cut and overtake others vehicle to
reach destination.

Wood (2002) has found that most of road accidents occur due to fault of drivers. Drivers are
unable to recognize hazard situation such as flooded roads and hanging wires on road. Transport
department (2010) has described that many factors have been included in road safety to minimize
the risk, such as handling of vehicle, adequate training and education for safe driving and
maintenance of the vehicle.

Salthouse (2005) have identified that there are many factors responsible to increase road
accidents such as drivers’ health problem, distraction and violating traffic rules, road condition
and vehicle problem and low risk perception drivers.

Lawton et al. (1997) have described that there is significant difference between ordinary and
deliberate violations. Ordinary violation consists of behaviours such as violate speed limit and
red lights, and deliberate violations focus on aggressive behaviours such as volume of horn or
aggressively following another vehicle.

Juhnke (1995) has discussed that drivers’ attitude influence risk taking behaviour regarding rule
violations and speeding. Low risk perception is related to road accident. It is found that there is
significant difference between male and female drivers regarding risky driving behaviour. Yagil
(1998) has identified that there is a significant difference in drivers’ attitude regarding traffic law

35
and traffic violation on the basis of gender and age. Young and male drivers violate traffic laws
as compared to female and older drivers. Assum (1997) and Yagil (1998) have described that
male drivers and young drivers have positive attitude towards violation and risky driving.
Drivers who take more risk while driving have low risk perception.

Cohn et al. (1995) have described that drivers’ risk perception is negatively correlated with risk
taking behaviour. Finn and Bragg (1986) have identified that there is significant difference
between young male drivers’ in age group 18 to 24 years and old male drivers in age group 38 to
50 years regarding risk perception. Young drivers have low risk perception as compared to old
drivers. Different habits of young drivers such as tailgating, driving at night, speeding, and
driving after drinking play an important role in road accidents.

According to Assum (1997), drivers’ attitudes is divided into two groups such as general
attitudes towards traffic safety and special attitudes towards speed, drinking and driving,
consideration for other road users and responsibility. It is also noticed that drivers’ attitude affect
road accident risk. Ulleberg and Rundmo (2003) have found that there is significant relationship
between drivers’ risk perception and driving behaviour regarding controlling attitudes.

2.7 Research Gap

The extensive literature review has been done to highlight drivers’ behaviour towards road
accidents. The study is designed to fill the research gap. But it was hard to find studies based on
drivers’ behaviour. After literature review, it is found that most of studies are based on secondary
data. A single study has not been found related to drivers’ behaviour in India. Adequate numbers
of studies are not available related to drivers’ perception of risk and driving tasks. Many
programs regarding road safety are organized by government to aware public but these programs
have failed due to low education of public and poor enforcement of law. No study has been
found that establish relationship between drivers’ perception of risk and driving tasks and safety
attitude. To fill that gap in literature the presents entitled “A study of road safety behaviour in
Haryana” is conducted.

36
2.8 References:

Afukaar, F. K. (2003). Speed control in developing countries: Issues, challenges and


opportunities in reducing road traffic injuries. Injury Control and Safety Promotion, 10(1-
2), 77-81.

Assum, T. (1997). Attitudes and road accident risk, Accident Analysis and Prevention, 2, 153-59.

Atubi and Augustus, O. (2012). Determinants of road traffic accident occurrences in Lagos state:
Some lessons for Nigeria. International Journal of Humanities and Social Science, 2(6),
252-259.

Bailly, B. and Chapon, A. (2006). Less trouble specifies the selectivity the attention: Conscience
situation the conduit at attention the conductor. Transport, 40-42.

Bamberg, S., Ajzen, I., and Schmidt, P (2003). Choice of travel mode in the theory of planned
behavior: The roles of past behavior, habit, and reasoned action. Basic Applied Social
Psychology. 25(3), 175-187.

Banik, B. K., Chowdhary, M. A. I., Hossain, E., and Mojumdar, B. (2011). Road accident and
safety study in Sylhet Region of Bangladesh. Journal of Engineering Science and
Technology, 6(4), 493-505.

Barkley, R. A. (2004). Driving impairments in teens and adults with attention hyperactivity
disorder. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 27(2), 233-260.

Beck, K.H., Hartos, J., and Simons-Morton, B. (2002). Teen driving risk: The promise of
parental influence and public policy. Health Education & Behavior, 29, 72-84.

Begg, D. and Langley, J. (2001). Changes in risky driving behavior from age 21 to 26 years,
Journal of Safety Research, 32, 491-499.

Beirness, D. J. (1993). Do we really drive as we live? The role of personality factors in road
crashes. Alcohol, Drugs and Driving, 9, 129-143.

37
Bener, A. and Crundall, D. (2005). Road traffic accidents in the United Arab Emirates compared
to Western countries. Advances in Transportation Studies an international Journal,
Section A 6.

Bianchi, A., and Summala, H. (2002). Moral judgment and drivers’ behavior among Brazilian
students. Psychological Report, 91, 759-866

Blatt, J., and Furman, S. M. (1998). Resident location of drivers involved in fatal crashes.
Accident Analysis & Prevention, 30(6), 705-711.

Bone, S.A., and Mowen, J.C. (2006). Identifying the traits of aggressive and distracted drivers: a
hierarchical trait model approach. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 5, 454-464.

Boufous, S., Ivers, R., Senserrick, T., Stevenson, M., Norton, R., and Williamson, A. (2010).
Accuracy of self-report of on-road crashes and traffic offences in a cohort of young
drivers: The drive study. Injury Prevention, 16, 275-277.

Braitman, K. A., Kirley, B.B., McCartt, A. T., and Chaudhary, N. K. (2008). Crashes of novice
teenage drivers: Characteristics and contributing factors. Journal of safety research, 39
(1), 47-54.

Brison, R. (1990). Risk of automobile accidents in cigarette smokers. Canadian Journal of


Public Health, 81(2), 102-106.

Brookland, R., Begg, D., Langley, J. and Ameratunga, S. (2010). Risk perception and risky
driving behaviours of adolescents and their parents: New Zealand drivers study. Injury
Prevention, 16, A1-A2 89.

Brown, I.D. and Groeger, J.A. (1988). Risk perception and decision taking during the transition
between novice and experienced driver status, Ergonomics, 31, 585-97.

Caird JK, Willness CR, Steel P, and Scialfa, C (2008). A meta-analysis of the effects of cell
phones on driver performance. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 40, 1282-1293.

38
Campbell, M., and Stradling, S.G. (2003). Factors influencing driver speed choices. Behavioural
Research in Road Safety XIII. London: Department for Transport.

Clarke, D. D., Ward, P., and Truman, W. (2002). In-depth accident causation study of young
drivers. Retrieved from http://www.trl.co.uk/static/dtlr/pdfs/TRL542.pdf

Cohn, L.D., Macfarlane, S., Yanez, C. (1995). Risk-Perception: Differences between adolescents
and adults, Health Psychology, 14(3), 217-222.

Cooper, P.J., Pinili, M., and Chen, W. (1995). An examination of the crash involvement rates of
novice drivers aged 16 to 55. Accident Analysis and Prevention 27, 89–104.

Daily, J.M., and Martin, J.H. (1988). Situational analysis of bus riders and non-riders for
different transportation methods. Logistic Transportation Review, 24(2), 185-199.

De Waard D., Kruizinga, A., and Brookhuis, KA (2008). The consequences of an increase in
heavy goods vehicles for passenger car drivers’ mental workload and behaviour
Prevention, 40(2), 818-828.

Deery, H. A. (1999). Hazard and Risk Perception among young novice drivers. Safety Research,
30(4), 225-236.

Deery, H.A., and Love, A.W. (1996). The effect of a moderate dose of alcohol on the traffic
hazard perception profile of young drivers. Addiction, 91, 815-827.

Dobbeleer, W.D., Nathanail, T. and Adamos, G. (2009). Pan-European road safety campaign:
Campaign Design, Belgian Road Safety Institute.

Drummond, AE (2000). Paradigm lost! Paradigm gained? An Australian’s perspective on the


novice driver problem. In: Proceedings of the novice driver conference, 1st-2nd June
2000, Bristol, available at www.dft.gov.uk

Dunbar, G., Lewis, V., and Hill, R. (2002). Parent-child interaction and road behaviour: An
exploratory study. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 601-622.

39
Elliott, M. A., Armitage, C. J. and Baughan, C. J. (2005). Exploring the beliefs underpinning
drivers’ intentions to comply with speed limits. Transportation Research, Traffic
Psychology and Behaviour, 8, 459-479.

Elvik, R., Christensen, P., and Amundsen, A. (2004). Speed and Road Accidents: An evaluation
of the power model. Oslo: The institute of transport economics enhancement, retrieved
from http://trid.trb.org/view.aspx?

Evans, L, and Wasielewski P. (1983). Risky driving related to driver and vehicle characteristics
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 15(2), 121-136.

Fildes, B. N., Rumbold, G., and Leening, A. (1991). Speed behaviour and drivers’ attitude to
speeding. General Report No. 16. Hawthorn, Victoria: VIC Roads.

Finn, P., and Bragg, W. E. (1986). Perception of the risk of an accident by young and older
drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 18(4), 289-298.

Forester, T. H., McNown, R., and Singell, L. D. (1984). A cost-benefit analysis of the 55 mph
speed limit. Southern Economic Journal, 50, 631-641.

Francis, J., Eccles, M., Johnston, M., Walker, A., Grimshaw, J., Foy, R., Kaner, E., Smith, L.,
and Bonetti, D. (2004). Constructing questionnaire based on the theory of planned
behaviour. Centre for Health Services Research, UK.

Fuller, R. (2005). Towards a general theory of driver behaviour. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 37, 461-472.

Gayer, T., Hamilton, J.T. and W.K. Viscusi (2000). Private values of risk tradeoffs at superfund
sites: housing market evidence on learning about risk, Review of Economics and
Statistics, 82, 439-51.

Gibbons, F.X., Etcheverry, P.E., Stock, M.L., Gerrard, M., Weng, C., Kiviniemi, M., and
O’Hara, R.E. (2010). Exploring the link between racial discrimination and substance use:
What mediates? What buffers? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 785-
801.

40
Goldenbeld, C., and Van Schagen, I. (2007). The credibility of speed limits on 80km/h rural,
roads: The effects of road and personality characteristics. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 39, 1121–1130.

Goodwin, A.H., Waller, M.W., Foss, R.D., and Margolis, L.H. (2006). Parental supervision of
teen drivers in a graduated licensing system. Traffic Injury Prevention, 7, 224-231.

Grantham, W. B. (2000). Analysis of principal factors contributing to fatal crashes in


Mississippi. Masters Abstracts International, 39(1), 256.

Groeger J.A. (2006). Youthfulness, inexperience and sleep loss: The problems young drivers
face and those they pose for us. Injury Prevention 12(1), 19-24.

Gulian, E.,Matthews, G.,Glendon, A. I., Davies, D. R., and Debney, L. M. (1989). Dimensions of
driver stress. Ergonomics, 32, 585-602.

Gulliver, P. and Begg, D. (2004). Influences during adolescence on perceptions and behaviour
related to alcohol use and unsafe driving as young adults. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 36, 773-781.

Gullone, E., and Moore, S. (2000). Adolescent risk-taking and the five factor model of
personality. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 393-407.

Hakes, J. K. and Viscusi, W. K. (1997). Mortality risk perceptions: A Bayesian reassessment.


Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 15(2), 135-150.

Hall, J. and West, R. (1997). The role of personality and attitudes in traffic accident risk. Applied
Psychology: An International Review, 46 (3), 253-264.

Harre, N., Brandt, T., and Dawe, M. (2000). The development of risky driving in adolescence.
Journal of Safety Research, 31, 185-194.

Hartos, J. L., Eitel, P., and Simons-Morton, B. (2001). Do parent-imposed delayed licensure and
restricted driving reduce risky driving behavior among newly licensed teens? Prevention
Science, 2, 113-121.

41
Hassen, A., Godesso, A., Abebe, L., and Girma, E. (2011). Risky driving behaviours for road
traffic accident among drivers in Mekele city, Northern Ethiopia. BioMed Central
Research Notes, 4, 535.

Hauer, E. (1999). Safety and the choice of degree of curve. Transportation Research Record, 22-
27

Hejar Abdul Rahman, N. A. M. Z., Kulanthayan, Subramaniam, Law Teik Hua (2005). Car
occupant's accidents and injuries among adolescents in a state in Malaysia. Proceedings
of the Eastern Asia Society for Transportation Studies, 5, 1867-1874.

Hesketh B, Godley ST (2002). A comparison of time estimations in driving with target-only in


motion, self-only in motion, and self-and-target in motion. Ecological Psychology, 14,
111-125

Hijar, M., Vazquez-Vela, E., and Arreola-Risa, C. (2003). Pedestrian traffic injuries in Mexico:
A country update. Injury Control and Safety Promotion, 10(1-2), 37-43.

Hoare, I. A. (2007). Attitudinal factors related to driving behaviors of young adults in Belize: An
application of the precaution adoption process model. University of South Florida, South
Florida.

Home, J. and Reyner, L. (2000). Sleep Related Vehicle Accidents, Sleep Research Laboratory,
Loughborough University.

Horswill, M. S., Marrington, S. A., McCullough, C. M., Wood, J., Pachana, N. A., and
McWilliam, J. (2008). The hazard perception ability of older drivers. The Journals of
Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63, 212-218.

Iversen, H. (2004). Risk-taking attitudes and risky driving behavior. Transportation Research
Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 7(3), 135-150.

Jenness, J.W., R. J. Lattanzio, M. O’Toole, and N. Taylor (2002). Voice-activated dialing or


eating a cheeseburger. Which is more distracting during simulated driving? Paper

42
presented at the 46th annual meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society,
September 23–27, in Pittsburg, PA.

Jorgensen, F. and Polak (1993). Measuring car drivers’ skills: an economist’s view. Accident
Analysis and Prevention; 25, 555-559.

Juhnke, G. A., Sullivan, T. J. and Harman, A. E. (1995). Attitude changes in DWI offenders: A
study of short-term treatment program, Journal of Addictions and Offender Counseling,
15, 51-58.

Kardara, M., and X, Kondakis (1997). Road traffic accidents in Greece: Recent trends (1981-
1991). European Journal of Epidemiology, 13, 765-770.

Keskinen, E., Ota, H. and Katila, A. (1998). Older drivers fail in intersection: Speed
discrepancies between older and younger male drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention,
30, 323

Kircher, K., and Andersson, J. (2013). Truck drivers’ opinion on road safety in Tanzania-A
questionnaire study. Traffic Injury Prevention, 14(1), 103-111.

Klauer SG, Dingus TA, Neale VL, Sudweeks JD, and Ramsey DJ. (2006). The impact of driver
inattention on near-crash/crash risk: An analysis using the 100-car naturalistic driving
study data (No. DOT HS 810 594.). Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic Safety
Administration.

Kouabenan, D. R. (2002). Occupation, driving experience, and risk and accident perception.
Journal of Risk Research, 5(1), 49-68.

Laapotti, S., and Keskinen, E. (2004). Has the difference in accident patterns between male and
female drivers changed between 1984 and 2000? Accident Analysis and Prevention, 36,
577-584.

Lajunen, T., Parker, D., and Summala, H. (2003). The Manchester Driver Behaviour
Questionnaire: A cross-cultural study. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 942, 1-8.

43
Lawton, R.J., Parker, D., Manstead, A.S.R., and Stradling, S.G. (1997). The role of affect in
predicting social behaviors: The case of road traffic violations. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 27, 1258-1276.

Lee, L., and NatCen, A. H. (2011). Attitudes to road safety: Analysis of driver behaviour module,
2010 NatCen omnibus survey. London: Department for Transport.

Ma, M., Yan, X., Huang, H., and Abdel-Aty, M. (2008). Occupational driver safety of public
transportation: Risk perception, attitudes, and driving behavior. Journal of the
Transportation Research Board, 89.

Mangiaracina G and Palumbo L. Smoking (2007). While driving and its consequences on road
safety. Accident Analysis and prevention, 19(3), 253-67.

May, G. (2004). Europe’s automotive sector at the crossroads. Foresight, 6(5), 302-312.

Mayhew, D.R., and Simpson, H.M. (1995). The role of driving experience: Implications for
training and licensing of new drivers. Occasional report, Insurance Bureau of Canada.

McCartt, A.T., Shabanova, V.I., and Leaf, W.A. (2003). Driving experience, crashes and
citations of teenage beginning drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35, 311-320.

McEvoy SP, Stevenson MR, and Woodward M (2006). Phone use and crashes while driving: A
representative survey of drivers in two Australian states. Medical Journal of Australia,
185(11-12), 630-634.

McKenna FP, Farrand, P. (1999). The role of automaticity in driving. In: Grayson GB (ed)
Behavioural research in road safety IX, Report No. PA3524/99, Transport Research
Laboratory (TRL), Crowthorne, UK

Miller, G., and Taubman-Ben-Ari, O. (2010). Driving styles among young novice drivers- The
contribution of parental driving styles and personal characteristics. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 42, 558-570.

44
Moen, B.-E., and Rundmo, T. (2006). Perception of transport risk in the Norwegian public. Risk
Management, 8(1), 43-60.

Moore, V. M., Dolinis, J. and Woodward, A. J. (1995). Vehicle speed and risk of a severe crash.
Epidemiology, 6, 258-262.

Naing, C., Hill, J., Maguire, M., Schick, S., Eggers, A., and Pastor, C. (2008). Driving task-
related factors.

Nordfjaern, T., Jørgensen, S., and Rundmo, T. (2011). A cross-cultural comparison of road
traffic risk perceptions, attitudes towards traffic safety and driver behaviour. Journal of
Risk Research, 14(6), 657-684.

O.G. Akanbi, Eharles-Owaba, O. E., and Olueye, A. E. (2009). Human factors in traffic
accidents in Logos, Nigeria. Disaster Prevention and Management, 18(4).

Parker, D and Stradling, S.G. (1998). Influencing driver attitudes and behaviour. Department of
Environment, Transport and the Regions.

Parker, D., Lajunen, T., and Stradling, S. (1998). Attitudinal predictors of interpersonally
aggressive violations on the road, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology
and Behaviour. 1(1), 11-24.

Parker, D., Reason, J. T., Manstead, A. S. R., and Stradling, S. G. (1995). Driving errors, driving
violations and accident involvement. Ergonomics, 38(5), 1036-1048.

Patton, J.H., Stanford, M.S. and Barratt, E.S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt
Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768-774.

Petridou, E., and Moustaki, M. (2002). Human factors in the causation of road traffic crashes.
European Journal of Epidemiology, 16, 819-826.

Ponnaluri, R. V. (2011). Road traffic crashes and risk groups in India: Analysis, interpretations,
and prevention strategies. IATSS Research, 35, 104-110.

45
Prabhakar, T., Lee, S. H. V. and Job, R. F. S. (1996). Risk taking, optimism bias and risk utility
in young drivers. Proceedings of the Road Safety Research and Enforcement Conference,
61-68.

Preusser, D. F., Williams, A. F., Zador, P. L., and Blomberg, R. D (1984). The effect of curfew
laws on motor vehicle crashes, Law and Policy, 6, 115-28.

Rajalin, S. (1994). The connection between risky driving and involvement in fatal accidents.
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 26(5), 555-562.

Reason, J., Manstead, A., Stradling, S., Baxter, J., and Campbell, K. (1990). Errors and
violations on the roads: A real distinction? Ergonomics, 33, 1315-1332.

Regan, M., Deery, H.A., and Triggs, T. J. (1998). Training for intentional control in novice car
drivers: A simulator study. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
42nd Annual Meeting, Chicago, 1408–1413.

Reimer B, Mehler B, Coughlin, J. F. (2008). A comparison of the effect of a low to moderately


demanding cognitive task on simulated driving performance and heart rate in middle aged
and young adult drivers. Cyber worlds, 493-500.

Renge, K. (1998). Drivers’ hazard and risk perception, confidence in safe driving, and choice of
speed. IATSS Research 22(2), 103-110.

Roehler, D. R., Sann, S., Kim, P., Bachani, A. M., Campostrini, S., and Florian, M., (2013).
Motorcycle helmet attitudes, behaviours and beliefs among Cambodians. International
Journal of Injury Control and Safety Promotion, 20(2), 179-183.

Rosenbloom, T., Shahar, A., Elharar, A., and Danino, O. (2008). Risk perception of driving as a
function of advanced training aimed at recognizing and handling risks in demanding
driving situations. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 40, 697-703.

Rumar, K. (1990). The basic driver error: Late detection. Ergonomics, 33, 1281-1290.

Sabbour, S. M., and Ibrahim, J. M. (2010). Driving behaviour, driver style and road traffic
accidents among young medical group. Injury Prevention, 16, A1-A289.

46
Sabey, B.E. and Taylor, H. (1980). The known risks we run: the highway. Department of
Transport, TRRL SR567, Transport and Road Research Laboratory. Crowthorne, Berks.

Sacks, JJ and Nelson, DE (1994). Smoking and injuries: An overview. Preventive Medicine,
23(4), 515-520.

Sagberg F., and Bjornskau, T. (2006). Hazard perception & driving experience among novice
drivers, Accident Analysis and Prevention, 38(2), 407-414

Salthouse, T. A. (2005). Relations between cognitive abilities and measures of executive


functioning. Neuropsychology, 19, 532-545.

Sarkar, S., and Andreas, M. (2004). Acceptance of and engagement in risky driving behaviors by
teenagers. Adolescence, 39, 687-700. Retrieved from http://www.highbeam. com

Shope, J.T., Waller, P.F., Raghunathan, T.E., and Patil, S.M. (2001). Adolescent antecedents of
high-risk driving behavior into young adulthood: Substance use and parental influences.
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 33, 649-658.

Silas O. A and Adoga, A. A. (2011). Road traffic accident deaths as seen in a Tertiary Health
Centre Jos University Teaching Hospital (JUTH), Jos, North central, Nigeria. Journal of
Clinical Pathology and Forensic Medicine, 2(2), 13-15.

Sliupas, T. (2009). The impact of road parameters and the surrounding area on traffic accidents,
Transport, 24(1), 42-47.

Stanton NA, and Salmon PM (2009). Human error taxonomies applied to driving: A generic
driver error taxonomy and its implications for intelligent transport systems. Safety
Science 47, 227-237

Summala, H.., Pasanen, E., Rasanen, M., and Sievanen, J. (1996). Bicycle accidents and drivers’
visual search at left and right turns. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 28(2), 147-153.

Sundstrom, A., and Albertsson, P. (2012). Self and peer-assessments of ambulance drivers'
driving performance. IATSS Research.

47
Tahir, N., Naseer, R., Khan, S. M., Macassa, G., and Waseem, Hashmi (2012). Road traffic
crashes managed by Rescue 1122 in Lahore, Pakistan. International Journal of Injury
Control and Safety Promotion, 19(4), 347-350.

Teye-Kwadjo (2011). Risk perception, traffic attitudes and behaviour among pedestrians and
commercial minibus drivers in Ghana: A case study of manya krobo district, Norwegian
University of Science and Technology.

Transport, D. (Ed.). (2010). The drivers' handbook. Australia.

Ulleberg, P., and T. Rundmo (2003). Personality, attitudes and risk perception as predictors of
risky driving behavior among young drivers. Safety Science, 41(5), 427-443.

Van Den Berg J, and Landstrom U. (2006). Symptoms of sleepiness while driving and their
relationship to prior sleep, work and individual characteristics. Transport Research, 9,
207-226.

Vanlaar, W., and Yannis, G. (2006). Perception of road accident causes. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 38, 155-161.

Vanlaar, W., and Yannis, G. (2006). Perception of road accident causes. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 38, 155-161.

Warren, D. L. (1982). Speed zoning and control. In Synthesis of Safety Research Related to
Traffic Control and Roadway Elements, Vol. 2 (Chapter 17). Report No. FHWA-TS-82-
233. Washington, DC: Federal Highway Administration.

Waylen, A. and McKenna, F. P. (2002). Cradle Attitudes – Grave Consequences. The


development of gender differences in risky attitudes and behaviour in road use. UK,
Basingstoke: AA Foundation for Road Safety.

Waylen, F.P., and McKenna, F.P. (2008). Risky attitudes toward road use in pre-drivers.
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 40, 905-911.

48
Williams, A.F. (1995, June). Magnitude and characteristics of the young driver crash problem in
the United States. Paper presented at the First Annual International Symposium of the
Youth Enhancement, retrieved from http://trid.trb.org/view.aspx?

Williams, A.F. (2003). Teenage drivers: Patterns of risk. Journal of Safety Research, 34, 5-15.

Wilson, F.A., and Stimpson, J.P. (2010). Trends in fatalities from distracted driving in the United
States, 1999 to 2008. American Journal of Public Health, 100, 2213-2219.

Wood, J. M. (2002). Age and visual impairment decrease driving performance as measured on a
closed-road circuit. Human Factors, 44, 482-494.

Yagil, D. (1998). Gender, and age-related differences in attitudes toward traffic laws and traffic
violations, Transportation Research, 1, 123-135.

Yao, L., and Wu, C. (2011). Traffic safety of e-bike riders in china: Safety attitudes, risk
perception and aberrant riding behaviours. Transportation Research Board.

Ying Wang, Zhang, W., Reimer, B., Lavallie, M., Lesch, R. M. F., Horrey, W. J. (2010). The
effect of feedback on attitudes toward cellular phone use while driving: A comparison
between novice and experienced drivers. Traffic Injury Prevention, 11, 471-477.

Young, M.S., J.M. Mahfoud, Walker, G.H., Jenkins, D.P., and Stanton, N.A. (2008). Crash
dieting: The effects of eating and drinking on driving performance‖, Accident Analysis &
Prevention, 40(1), 142-148.

Zegeer, C. V. and Bushell, M. (2012). Pedestrian crash trends and potential counter measures
from around the world. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 44(1), 3-11.

Zhao, X. G., He, X. D., Wu, J. S., Zhao, G. F., Ma, Y. F., and Zhang, M. (2009). Risk factors for
urban road traffic injuries in Hangzhou, China. Arch Orthopnea Trauma Surgery, 129,
507-513.

49

You might also like