You are on page 1of 14

NEUROSCIENCE AND

BIOBEHAVIORAL
REVIEWS

PERGAMON Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576

Are we dependent upon coffee and caffeine?


A review on human and animal data
Astrid Nehlig*
INSERM U 398, Faculté de Médecine, 11 rue Humann, 67085 Strasbourg, France
Received 13 October 1997; received in revised form 23 April 1998; accepted 17 June 1998

Abstract
Caffeine is the most widely used psychoactive substance and has been considered occasionally as a drug of abuse. The present paper
reviews available data on caffeine dependence, tolerance, reinforcement and withdrawal. After sudden caffeine cessation, withdrawal
symptoms develop in a small portion of the population but are moderate and transient. Tolerance to caffeine-induced stimulation of
locomotor activity has been shown in animals. In humans, tolerance to some subjective effects of caffeine seems to occur, but most of
the time complete tolerance to many effects of caffeine on the central nervous system does not occur. In animals, caffeine can act as a
reinforcer, but only in a more limited range of conditions than with classical drugs of dependence. In humans, the reinforcing stimuli
functions of caffeine are limited to low or rather moderate doses while high doses are usually avoided. The classical drugs of abuse lead to
quite specific increases in cerebral functional activity and dopamine release in the shell of the nucleus accumbens, the key structure for
reward, motivation and addiction. However, caffeine doses that reflect the daily human consumption, do not induce a release of dopamine in
the shell of the nucleus accumbens but lead to a release of dopamine in the prefrontal cortex, which is consistent with caffeine reinforcing
properties. Moreover, caffeine increases glucose utilization in the shell of the nucleus accumbens only at rather high doses that stimulate most
brain structures, non-specifically, and likely reflect the side effects linked to high caffeine ingestion. That dose is also 5–10-fold higher than
the one necessary to stimulate the caudate nucleus, which mediates motor activity and the structures regulating the sleep-wake cycle, the two
functions the most sensitive to caffeine. In conclusion, it appears that although caffeine fulfils some of the criteria for drug dependence and
shares with amphetamines and cocaine a certain specificity of action on the cerebral dopaminergic system, the methylxanthine does not act on
the dopaminergic structures related to reward, motivation and addiction. 䉷 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Caffeine; Dependence; Withdrawal; Tolerance; Discrimination; Drugs of abuse

1. Introduction clinical data, the possibility that caffeine withdrawal but


not abuse and dependence should be added to diagnostic
Caffeine is the most widely used psychoactive substance manuals has been considered in the United States [107].
in the world [69]. Most of the caffeine consumed comes The present paper will review the available data on coffee
from dietary sources such as coffee, tea, cola drinks and and caffeine consumption, caffeine dependence, withdrawal
chocolate. The most notable behavioral effects of caffeine and reinforcement, and try to assess in which respect
occur after low to moderate doses (50–300 mg) and are caffeine differs from drugs of abuse such as amphetamines,
increased alertness, energy and ability to concentrate. cocaine and morphine. It will also consider the reasons why
Moderate caffeine consumption leads very rarely to health caffeine could be considered a potential drug of dependence.
risks [19, 40, 109]. Higher doses of caffeine rather induce
negative effects such as anxiety, restlessness, insomnia and
tachychardia, these effects being seen primarily in a small 2. Coffee and caffeine consumption
portion of caffeine-sensitive individuals. On the other hand,
2.1. Coffee consumption
caffeine was considered in one study as a potential drug of
abuse [71] and more recently described as ‘‘a model drug of
After having been limited to the Arab world until the
abuse’’ [99]. Finally, based on a review of science and
fifteenth century, coffee consumption reached the European
world during the sixteenth century and rapidly spread
* Corresponding author. Tel: ⫹ 33-388-243357; fax: ⫹ 33-388- throughout Europe [42]. According to recent surveys,
243360; e-mail: nehlig@neurochem.u-strasbg.fr. consumption of coffee varies greatly among the different
0149-7634/99/$ - see front matter 䉷 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S0149-763 4(98)00050-5
564 A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576

Table 1 in France [173] indicate that four attitudes are positively


Content of caffeine in a cup of coffee according to the mode of preparation linked to the quantity of coffee consumed. They are, in
(data from Refs. [36, 38])
decreasing order of importance, the need for a stimulant,
Mode of preparation Volume of serving Caffeine content the preference for strong coffee, the knowledge of coffee,
and the preference for the coffee roasting shop.
Boiled 150–190 ml 111–177 mg/cup
Filter 50–190 ml 28–161 mg/cup 2.2. Caffeine consumption
Espresso 50–150 ml 74–99 mg/cup
Percolated 150–190 ml 55–88 mg/cup
Caffeine is present in a number of dietary sources
Instant 50–190 ml 19–34 mg/cup
consumed worldwide, i.e. tea, coffee, cocoa beverages,
candy bars, and soft drinks. The content of caffeine of
countries. The highest consumption (more than 10 kg/
these various food items ranges from 71–220 mg/150 ml
person/year) is encountered in all Scandinavian countries
for coffee, to 32–42 mg/150 ml for tea, 32–70 mg/330 ml
plus Austria and the Netherlands. In most western European
for cola and 4 mg/150 ml for cocoa [44]. Caffeine consump-
countries, as well as in Brazil and Costa Rica, coffee
tion from all sources can be estimated as 76 mg/person/day
consumption ranges from 6–9 kg/person/year. The lowest
but reaches 210–238 mg/day in the United States and
consumption (less than 5 kg/person/year) occurs in the
Canada and more than 400 mg/person/day in Sweden and
United States, Italy, Algeria, Nicaragua and Paraguay [42,
Finland where 80%–100% of the caffeine intake comes only
70].
from coffee [16, 17, 44, 191, 192]. In the UK, the consump-
The content of caffeine per cup of coffee also varies
tion is as high as in the later two countries but 72% is
largely according to the size of the serving, the mode of
represented by tea [44]. According to the recent survey of
preparation of the coffee (boiled, filter, percolated, espresso
Barone and Roberts [17], the daily intake of caffeine from
or instant), and the type of coffee used (Arabica or Robusta)
all sources in the United States is estimated at 3 mg/kg/
[42, 44]. Indeed, as can be seen in Table 1, the size of a cup
person, two thirds of it coming from coffee in subjects
of coffee can range from 50–190 ml and the standard
aged more than 10 years. If only consumers are taken into
content of caffeine in a cup of coffee can be as low as
account, the daily caffeine consumption reaches a value of
19 mg/cup for instant coffee and reach a maximum value
2.4–4.0 mg/kg (170–300 mg) in a 60–70 kg individual. In
of 177 mg/cup in boiled coffee.
children, the soft drinks represent 55%, chocolate foods and
From these data, the caffeine content of a cup of coffee
beverages 35%–40% and tea 6%–10% of the total caffeine
ranges from 0.7–1.1 mg/ml for boiled or filter coffee, 0.6–
intake [51].
3.3 mg/ml for espresso and 0.2–0.6 mg/ml (eventually up to
1.0 mg/ml) in instant coffee [42].
Most of the coffee consumed throughout the world is 3. Caffeine absorption and pharmacokinetics
Arabica. In most countries, Arabica represents 70%–
100% (100% in Finland and Sweden) of the whole coffee Caffeine absorption from the gastrointestinal tract is rapid
consumed. The only exceptions are France, Italy, Portugal and reaches 99% in humans about 45 min after ingestion
and the UK, where Robusta represents 42%–70% of the [13, 21–23, 132]. Caffeine absorption is also complete in
whole coffee consumption [42, 44]. The average content animals [10, 11]. Pharmacokinetics are comparable after
of caffeine is about twice as high in Robusta as in Arabica oral or intravenous administration of caffeine in humans
coffee. Indeed, the content of caffeine, expressed as percent and animals, leading to superimposable plasma curves [13].
of dry weight, ranges from 0.9%–1.2% in green Arabica Peak plasma caffeine concentration is reached between
beans and averages 1.3% in roasted Arabica beans. In 15–120 min after oral ingestion in humans and equals 8–
Robusta coffee, the content of caffeine is 1.6%–2.4% and 10 mg/l for doses of 5–8 mg/kg [14, 23]. For doses lower
2.0% of the dry weight for green and roasted beans, respec- than 10 mg/kg, the caffeine half-life ranges from 0.7–1.2 h
tively. As a consequence, in a standard 150 ml cup, the in rats and mice, 3–5 h in monkeys [24] and 2.5–4.5 h in
content of caffeine ranges from 71–120 mg/cup for Arabica humans [12]. There are no differences in the caffeine half-
coffee and from 131–220 mg/cup for Robusta [42, 191, life in young and elderly humans [22]. Conversely, the
192]. caffeine half-life is increased during the neonatal period
World coffee consumption is increasing. The average due to the lower activity of cytochrome P-450 [7] and to
consumption of coffee in 1990 ranged from 1.41 cups/day the relative immaturity of some demethylation and acetyla-
in Japan, to 1.73 cups/day in the United States and tion pathways [9, 32]. The half-life of caffeine is about 80 ^
3.87 cups/day in Germany [42]. In the United States, coffee 23 h for the full-term newborn infant [8, 125] and can be
consumption (numbers of cups/person/day) decreased in over 100 h in premature infants [154]. Thereafter, the half-
1986 and has not changed since then. In Japan, coffee life of caffeine decreases exponentially with postnatal age to
consumption has been constantly increasing over the last reach 14.4 h and 2.6 h in 3–5 and 5–6 month-old infants,
10 years, while in Germany the consumption has been stable respectively [2, 152, 154, 156]. The clearance of caffeine is
over the same period [42]. The results of a survey performed low in one-month-old infants (31 ml/kg/h), but increases to
A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576 565

a maximum value of 331 ml/kg/h at 5–6 months, compared latter regions, A2a receptors are coexpressed with enkepha-
with 155 ml/kg/h in adult humans [7]. In adult males, the lin and dopamine D2 receptors in the same kind of striatal
caffeine half-life is reduced by 30%–50% in smokers, neuronal cells [58, 149, 171]. There is direct evidence for a
compared with nonsmokers [90, 111, 137], while it is central functional interaction between adenosine A2a and
approximately doubled in women taking oral contraceptives dopamine D2 receptors. Indeed, the administration of
[155] and largely prolonged (up to 15 h) during the last adenosine A2a receptor agonists decreases the affinity of
trimester of pregnancy [3, 28, 118]. dopamine binding to D2 receptors in striatal membranes
Caffeine is metabolized through liver biotransformation [57]. The interaction between adenosine A2a receptors
into dimethylxanthines, dimethyl and monomethyl uric and dopamine D2 receptors in the striatum might underlie
acids, trimethyl- and dimethyl-allantoin, and uracil deriva- some of the behavioral effects of methylxanthines. By
tives. The metabolic pathways show multiple and separate antagonizing the negative modulatory effects of adenosine
demethylation, C-8 oxidation and uracil formation in receptors on dopamine receptors, caffeine leads to the inhi-
humans and rodents, that occur predominantly in liver bition and blockage of adenosine A2 receptors, leading to a
microsomes. The main difference between rodents and potentiation of dopaminergic neurotransmission [56, 65,
humans is that, in the rat, 40% of the caffeine metabolites 158]. The latter interaction is very interesting since it
are trimethyl derivatives, while they do not exceed 6% in could explain the adenosine receptor antagonists-induced
humans. Conversely, in humans, the 3-methyl demethyla- increase in behaviors related to dopamine [41], such as,
tion, leading to the formation of paraxanthine, represents e.g. caffeine-induced rotational behavior [67].
72%–80% of caffeine metabolism [12, 13].

5. Addiction and drug dependence


4. Mechanism of action of caffeine
Drug dependence has been defined as ‘a pattern of beha-
The effect of caffeine on the release of intracellular vior focused on the repetitive and compulsive seeking and
calcium and as an inhibitor of cyclic nucleotide phospho- taking of a psychoactive drug’ [92]. However, it is necessary
diesterases has been mainly shown in vitro at 500 micro- or to demonstrate psychoactive effects to differentiate drug
milli-molar concentrations, respectively, i.e. at doses higher dependence from other habitual or controlled behaviors,
than those usually achieved by human consumption [139, such as the daily ingestion of some types of medication
140] and cannot therefore account for most of the physio- like aspirin or vitamins. Moreover, it appears necesary to
logical effects of caffeine. One exception is the respiratory- demonstrate that the drug is positively reinforcing its own
stimulant effect of caffeine that appears to be prominently ingestion. Therefore, the following section will consider the
mediated by the inhibition of type IV phosphodiesterase criteria used for the definition of dependence.
[102]. In fact, it is now widely accepted that the main The recent diagnostic manuals from the World Health
mechanism of action of caffeine occurring at circulating Organization (WHO) [197] and the American Psychiatric
concentrations achieved after the consumption of one or Association (APA) [4, 5] proposed a new set of criteria
two cups of coffee, is the antagonism at the level of adeno- for dependence. The diagnosis of dependence requires the
sine receptors [62, 63, 139, 140]. Indeed, in animals, most fulfillment of three (non specified) of the six WHO [196] or
pharmacological effects of adenosine in the brain can be seven APA [4, 5] criteria. The seven criteria of dependence
suppressed by relatively low concentrations of circulating as proposed by the APA [5] in DSM-IV (Diagnostic and
caffeine, i.e. less than 100 mM, which are attained after the Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed.) are: (i)
consumption of one to three cups of coffee. Adenosine tolerance (not specified for severity); (ii) substance-specific
decreases the firing rate of neurones and exerts an inhibitory withdrawal syndrome (psychic or physiological, not speci-
effect on synaptic transmission and on the release of most fied for severity); (iii) substance is often taken in larger
neurotransmitters, while caffeine increases the turnover of amounts or over a longer period than intended; (iv) persis-
many neurotransmitters, including monoamines and acetyl- tent desire or unsuccessful efforts to cut down or control use;
choline [139, 140]. (v) a great deal of time spent in activities necessary to
The A1 and A2a adenosine receptors are the subtypes that obtain, use, or recover from the effects of the substance;
are primarily involved in the effects of caffeine, while the (vi) important social, occupational or recreational activities
A2b and A3 receptors play only a minor role. The A1 recep- given up or reduced because of substance use; and (vii) use
tors are negatively linked to adenyl cyclase, while the A2a continued despite knowledge of a persistent or recurrent
receptors are positively linked to the enzyme. Adenosine A1 physical or psychological problem that is likely to have
receptors are widely distributed throughout the brain with been caused or exacerbated by the substance. The six
high levels in the hippocampus, cerebral and cerebellar criteria proposed by the WHO [197] differ only slightly
cortex, and thalamus [43, 54, 77]. Conversely, A2a recep- from those of the APA, mainly by a different sequence,
tors are almost exclusively located in the striatum, nucleus slightly different formulations and the combination of
accumbens and olfactory tubercle [110, 149, 153]. In the criteria (v) and (vi) into a single one. The only possibility
566 A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576

to differentiate between substances that can lead to depen- avoidance of a preferred flavor when the latter was paired
dence is to classify them according to the number of criteria with caffeine abstinence [193]. The severity of caffeine
met and specify for severity of symptoms and frequency of withdrawal depends on the dose, and decreases in locomotor
occurrence. activity do not appear when caffeine doses lower than
The possible dependence on caffeine has been considered 67 mg/kg/day are substituted by water [59, 98]. The length
by several groups for over a decade [71, 78, 83, 84, 88, 92, of the decrease in locomotor activity depends also upon the
93, 186] and caffeine has even been postulated as a ‘poten- dose of caffeine and the duration of the treatment before the
tial model of drug of abuse’ [99]. In two recent studies using substitution by water [59, 98]. The latency to the onset of
the criteria cited above, a dependence on caffeine was caffeine withdrawal effects usually occurs within 24 h, and
shown in a subset of the general population. As a result of peaks around 24–48 h [31, 33, 59, 98, 136, 180, 193]. The
a random telephone survey in Vermont, Hughes et al. [106] caffeine withdrawal-induced behavioral changes usually
showed that 14% and 3% of the 166 caffeine users inter- last a few days [85], except for the sleep-related signs that
viewed met the criteria for moderate and severe caffeine have been shown to last up to 30 days after the onset of
dependence, respectively. After telephone screening caffeine withdrawal [180].
performed on 99 subjects in the United States, Strain et al.
[187] found 16 individuals that fulfilled four out of the seven 6.2. Characterization of withdrawal symptoms in humans
criteria cited above, and were thus considered dependent on
caffeine. The criteria met were criteria (i), (ii), (iv) and (vii) Caffeine withdrawal translates into typical symptoms.
of DSM-IV. Criteria (iii), (v) and (vi) were excluded The most often reported are headaches, feelings of weari-
because they do not apply to a substance widely available ness, weakness and drowsiness, impaired concentration,
and culturally accepted. The dependence was not related to fatigue and work difficulty, depression, anxiety, irrritability,
the daily caffeine intake which ranged from 129–2548 mg/ increased muscle tension, occasionally tremor, and nausea
day. The median daily caffeine intake for the caffeine- and vomiting, as well as withdrawal feelings [83, 105, 140,
dependent individuals was 360 mg and about 40% of them 164, 177, 186, 187]. Withdrawal symptoms generally begin
had a daily caffeine intake of 300 mg or less [187]. about 12–24 h after sudden cessation of caffeine consump-
However, in spite of the absence of current psychiatric tion and reach a peak after 20–48 h. However, in some
disorders at the time of the study in most of the individuals individuals, these symptoms can appear within only 3–6 h
(14 out of 16), 11 of the 16 persons diagnosed with ‘caffeine and last for one week [16, 108, 124, 140]. Withdrawal
dependence’ had a history of psychiatric disorders, mainly symptoms do not relate to the quantity of caffeine ingested
substance abuse disorders (10 subjects) and mood disorders daily [86–88, 96, 97, 107, 187], e.g. the recent study of
(seven subjects). The prevalence of these disorders is higher Strain et al. [187] showed that withdrawal symptoms
than that encountered in the general population, i.e. 50% occur in individuals who daily consume anything from
[117]. Moreover, the tendency to an association between 129–2548 mg of caffeine. In the last decade, two studies
caffeine, alcohol, and nicotine consumption [122], as well [107, 187] suggested that caffeine withdrawal symptoms
as between mood disorders and nicotine dependence has (but not abuse or dependence) should be added to the list
been previously reported [29, 116]. of diagnoses recognized by the American Health System
Therefore, to try to assess in which respect caffeine (DSM-IV and ICD-10).
should or should not be considered a drug of dependence, There is a strong positive correlation between caffeine
in the present review, the consequences of coffee and consumption, fasting and headaches before and after surgi-
caffeine consumption on various criteria, possibly leading cal procedures. For every increase in the usual daily
to the diagnosis of dependence, will be considered. Among consumption of 100 mg of caffeine (about a cup of coffee),
the seven criteria for drug dependence that have been cited the risk of headache immediately before and after surgery is
above, the four main factors to consider are withdrawal, increased by 12% and 16%, respectively, and correlates also
tolerance, reinforcement and dependence. with the duration of fasting [55, 144]. The risk of headaches
is reduced in individuals who drink caffeine or get substitu-
tive caffeine tablets on the day of the surgery [89, 195, 196].
6. Caffeine withdrawal Therefore, it was advised by three studies that the numerous
healthy patients who drink caffeine-containing beverages
6.1. Caffeine withdrawal in animals daily and are undergoing minor surgical procedures should
be permitted to ingest preoperative caffeine [144, 195, 196].
There are several reports showing caffeine withdrawal In their most recent study, Weber et al. [196] also showed
signs in rats, cats and monkeys. They include decreases in that in the population selected, 40%–48% of the patients
locomotor activity [59, 98], operant behavior [31, 33, 136], already suffered regular headaches (at least weekly) at the
and in the reinforcement threshold for electrical brain stimu- time they underwent surgery. Moreover, there is a relation-
lation [136]. Other studies have reported changes in the time ship between caffeine withdrawal, the development of head-
spent in various phases of slow wave sleep [180] and aches and changes in cerebral blood flow. The cerebral
A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576 567

blood flow velocities are increased during withdrawal head- doses of the drug may occur, hence leading people to
aches, significantly decreased within 30 min after caffeine consume higher doses of the drug over time.
intake in all subjects and return to baseline values after 2 h Tolerance to many behavioral effects of caffeine occurs in
[39]. This recent study confirms several previous ones that mice, cats and squirrel monkeys chronically treated with
suggested that increased blood volume may be involved in methylxanthine (for review, see [85, 100]). Thus, tolerance
caffeine withdrawal headaches [48, 94, 133, 194]. Caffeine to caffeine-induced locomotor stimulation [1, 35, 59–61,
withdrawal symptoms were even reported in newborns 98, 100], cerebral electrical activity [35, 95, 98], reinforce-
whose mothers were heavy coffee drinkers during preg- ment thresholds for electrical brain stimulation [136], sche-
nancy. The infants displayed irritability, high emotivity dule-controlled responding maintained by presentation of
and, eventually, vomiting. Symptoms begun at birth but food and electric shock [100, 115], and thresholds for
spontaneously disappeared after a few days [134]. caffeine- [199] or NMDA-induced seizures [68] has been
Recently, Smith [181] reported that in a population of 144 described. The development of tolerance to caffeine in
students that discriminated caffeine only at chance level, animals is rapid, usually insurmountable, shows cross-toler-
there was no difference in the frequency of headaches ance with other methylxanthines but not with psychomotor
between the group in which caffeine was withdrawn and stimulants such as amphetamines and methylphenidate [60,
the one receiving caffeinated coffee over the 3 day protocol. 61, 98, 100]. On the first two days after caffeine disconti-
This effect could be interpreted as an expectancy effect that nuation, depression of locomotor activity is noted with a
might not have occurred in other studies where subjects return to baseline values on the third day, consistent with
were able to discriminate caffeine. a withdrawal syndrome [59, 60, 98]. Although the exact
mechanism underlying the development of tolerance to
6.3. Relief of abstinence symptoms by caffeine caffeine remains unclear, tolerance to behavioral effects of
caffeine in animals does not seem to involve adaptative
Caffeine withdrawal symptoms disappear soon after
changes in adenosine receptors [68, 101] and may rather
absorption of caffeine. This effect is strongly linked to the
result from compensatory changes in the dopaminergic
psychological satisfaction related to the ingestion of
system as a result of chronic adenosine receptor blockage
caffeine; this is especially true for the first cup of the day.
[66].
The potential reversal of caffeine withdrawal-induced head-
In humans the tolerance to some physiological actions of
aches and other symptoms by the absorption of caffeine
caffeine has been shown to occur. This is the case for the
alone has been known for over 50 years and has been
effect of caffeine on blood pressure and heart rate [6, 45,
shown repeatedly [48, 73, 74, 86, 103]. The occurrence of
165, 169, 175], diuresis [50], plasma adrenaline and nora-
headaches on substitution of caffeinated by decaffeinated
drenaline levels, and renin activity [165], that usually devel-
coffee predicts subsequent caffeine self-administration
ops within a few days. Tolerance to some subjective effects
[103]. Caffeine content influences coffee consumption [86,
of caffeine, such as increases in tension-anxiety, jitteriness/
87, 121] and the beneficial effects of caffeine consumption
nervousness, activity/stimulation/energy, and the strength
on mood or alertness seem to incite people to drink coffee or
of drug effect [53] was recently shown to occur. Conversely,
caffeine-containing beverages [123, 164]. Heavy consumers
although tolerance to the enhancement of arithmetic skills
of coffee show a preference for coffee that contains caffeine,
by caffeine was recently shown [169], there is only limited
while those who used to drink decaffeinated coffee will
evidence for tolerance to caffeine-induced alertness and
choose either decaffeinated or caffeine-containing coffee
wakefulness [38, 76, 96, 97]. These effects are paralleled
[78, 185]. When subjects are categorized between caffeine
by the lack of tolerance of cerebral energy metabolism to
choosers and nonchoosers, caffeine choosers tend to report
caffeine, since an acute administration of 10 mg/kg caffeine
both positive subjective effects of caffeine (stimulant and
induces the same metabolic increases whether the rats have
positive effects on mood and vigilance) as well as negative
been exposed to a previous daily chronic treatment by
subjective effects of placebo (headache and fatigue), while
caffeine or saline for 15 days [138]. These data show that
caffeine nonchoosers tend to report negative effects of
every single exposure to caffeine is able to produce cerebral
caffeine (primarily anxiety and dysphoria) [53, 179, 185].
stimulant effects and this is especially true in the areas that
control locomotor activity (caudate nucleus) and the struc-
7. Tolerance to the effects of caffeine tures involved in the sleep-wake cycle (locus coeruleus,
raphe nuclei and reticular formation) [138].
Tolerance to a drug refers to an acquired change in In humans, sleep seems to be the physiological function
responsiveness of a subject repeatedly exposed to the drug most sensitive to the effects of caffeine, as detailed in a
and can be considered in two ways. First, tolerance might recent review [182]. Generally, more than 200 mg caffeine
indicate that the dose necessary to achieve the desired are needed to affect sleep significantly. Caffeine has been
euphoric or reinforcing effects will increase with time, shown to prolong sleep latency, shorten total sleep duration
thus inciting people to gradually consume more of the but to preserve the dream phases of sleep. It is not clearly
drug. Second, tolerance to the aversive effects of high established yet whether or not the difference in the
568 A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576

sensitivity to the effects of coffee on sleep could be attribu- study recently showed discrimination of caffeine at doses
table to tolerance. According to some studies, this difference as low as 10 mg for one individual, 18 mg for three subjects,
could rather reflect the interindividual sensitivity to and 56 mg for three others [82]. That study involving the
caffeine, possibly related to differences in the rate of authors themselves was replicated with subjects less
caffeine metabolism [126, 190]. Indeed poor sleepers are informed of the potential effects of the drugs under study.
reported to metabolize caffeine at a lower rate and four Data in the same range as in the previous study were
out of the 10 subjects of the study had elimination half- obtained with caffeine discrimination in one subject at
lives exceeding 4.8 h [190]. The variability in the subjects 18 mg of caffeine, in one at 32 mg, in two at 56 mg and in
response from one night to the next should also be taken into four at 100 mg caffeine. The authors suggested that in speci-
account [76, 96, 97, 127]. Nevertheless, there are some fic individuals, caffeine could be considered to affect mood
indications of a development of tolerance to sleep distur- at doses lower than those previously reported [178]. Indeed,
bances related to caffeine intake since heavy coffee drinkers some groups were able to show enhanced auditory vigilance
appear to be less sensitive to caffeine-induced sleep distur- and reaction time at 75 mg [36], 64 mg [127], or even 32 mg
bances than light coffee drinkers [38]. Likewise, tolerance to [128] of caffeine. It seems that the effects of caffeine are
sleep latency and quality to caffeine has been shown to utilized consciously or unconsciously by the various indivi-
develop over 2 days of testing in one study [200] and duals in the management of mood state, relevant to the
7 days in another [25]. However, the tolerance is not context of drink choice [64]. The discrimination of low
complete and the sleep efficiency remains below 90% of doses of caffeine is not related to the taste of caffeine
the baseline value after 7 days of caffeine treatment [25]. since at the dose of 100 mg, subjects are not able to reliably
Thus, tolerance to some of the effects linked to regular differentiate decaffeinated coffee plus lactose from decaffei-
consumption of coffee seems to occur, especially in animals. nated coffee plus 100 mg of caffeine [80]. Conversely, at
In humans, the data are less conclusive and this may under- higher doses, caffeine could be detected in coffee as high
lie individual differences in the susceptibility and tolerance concentrations relate directly to coffee biterness [79].
to caffeine-induced effects. Moreover, mechanisms of toler-
ance may be overwhelmed by the nonlinear accumulation of 8.2. Reinforcing effects of caffeine
caffeine and its main metabolites in the human body when
caffeine metabolism becomes saturable under multiple Reinforcing efficacy of a drug refers to the relative effi-
dosing conditions [45, 46]. cacy in establishing or maintaining a behavior on which the
delivery of the drug is dependent. In animals, intravenous
self-administration of caffeine has been studied after the
8. Discrimination and reinforcement of caffeine in implantation of venous catheters allowing them by pressing
humans a lever to self-administer the drug and assess behavioral
reinforcement [84]. In four studies, caffeine was shown to
8.1. Discrimination of caffeine be self-injected in all animals [47, 49, 81, 84], while three
showed that only a limited subset (25%–33%) of the
Human subjects are able to discriminate caffeine against animals self-administered caffeine [15, 37, 172]. A sporadic
placebo both when offered in capsules or in coffee. Doses of pattern of caffeine self-administration, characterized by
300 mg or higher are usually more easily detected and periods with high rates of self-injection alternating with
mainly recognized by their negative effects of jitteriness, periods of rather low intake was found in nonhuman
anxiety or nervousness, whereas the lower doses are primates [47, 81, 84]. Thus, although caffeine seems to be
detected by their lack of effect or by caffeine withdrawal able to act as a reinforcer in some conditions, there is a
symptoms. However, several studies have failed to demon- marked difference between caffeine and classical drugs of
strate behavioral effects of caffeine at doses below 200 or abuse, such as amphetamines and cocaine, that maintain
300 mg, i.e. amounts corresponding to the ingestion of two self-administration across species and conditions [81,
to three cups of coffee [18, 69]. Doses in the range of 159]. Recently, caffeine was shown to be able to reinstate
100 mg, which closely approach the caffeine content of a extinguished cocaine-taking behavior in rats. This effect
normal serving are detected poorly or at chance level only in was more marked when caffeine was given one day, rather
one study [185], by 30% or 60% of the individuals accord- than four days, following the last cocaine self-administra-
ing to two other studies, respectively [86, 103]. However, in tion session. Thus, extended withdrawal is able to increase
most of these studies, subjects were not withdrawn for a the priming effects of caffeine [170, 198]. However, it must
prolonged period from their habitual daily amounts of be remembered that these animal studies use intravenous
caffeine and thus may have been tolerant. self-administration while human caffeine consumption is
In fact, doses of caffeine below 100 mg neither induce always by oral route and it is known that the former mode
feelings of withdrawal nor negative effects, have rarely been of administration is by far more addictive than the latter one
shown to alter self-reports of mood or performance and are [93]. Thus, caffeine dose does not appear to be a very robust
usually preferred by moderate coffee drinkers [104]. One reinforcer in animals.
A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576 569

In humans, the widely recognized behavioral stimulant instant coffee for three days, the desire for coffee in the next
and mildly reinforcing properties of caffeine are probably three days largely increased in the group given caffeine
responsible for the maintenance of caffeine self-administra- tablets but remained unchanged in the group given decaffei-
tion, primarily in the form of caffeinated beverages, such as nated instant coffee, although the latter group experienced
coffee, tea and cola [84, 140]. In some studies, the choice of marked symptoms of caffeine withdrawal [97].
caffeine has been shown to be more potently controlled by The question of whether the taste of coffee and caffeine
avoiding withdrawal than by its positive effects [166, 175], may influence its intake is still a matter of debate. If water
while other data support the hypothesis that the true perfor- containing caffeine is given chronically to rats between 29
mance-enhancing effects of caffeine are responsible for its and 40 days of postnatal life, rats exposed to caffeine as
self-administration [167]. Most data showed that caffeine adults will drink more caffeinated water than tap water.
reinforcement occurs in 100% of heavy caffeine consumers Likewise the previous administration of an adenosine
(1020–1530 mg/day) that also had histories of alcohol or agonist increases caffeine intake. Thus, it seems that
drug abuse [78–80]. For moderate caffeine users (128– caffeine intake could be at least partly related to its pharma-
595 mg/day), caffeine reinforcement occurs in about 45% cological properties, although the influence of taste cannot
[88, 103, 104, 147, 148] to possibly 80%–100% of the be eliminated [143]. Coffee and caffeine would in fact have
subjects [52, 179]. two components, an appetitive and an aversive one. The
Caffeine reinforcement varies with the dose. Doses of absorption of low quantities of caffeine could favor the
caffeine encountered in tea and coffee are high enough to appetitive effect of caffeine [193], whereas higher quantities
act as reinforcers, since people look for them in case of could exacerbate its aversive effects [183]. In man, the
withdrawal symptoms [103]. Indeed, a dose of 25–50 mg gustatory response to caffeine is not influenced by previous
caffeine per cup of coffee acts as a reinforcer, while increas- exposure to a series of methylxanthines or adenosine [30,
ing doses beyond 50 or 100 mg tends to decrease the choice 135] or by caffeine deprivation [27]. However, the taste of
of caffeine or the frequency of caffeine self-administration coffee is an important aspect of caffeine consumption and
[84], and high doses of caffeine (400–600 mg in a single subjects prefer caffeine in coffee to caffeine in capsules [80].
dose) are avoided [86]. Caffeine reinforcement relates also Another possiblity is that caffeine is a constituent of
to withdrawal syndromes occurring after coffee cessation. coffee and tea which are liked for reasons independent
Indeed, subjects that consistently suffer from caffeine with- from their caffeine content. Thirst could be one factor but
drawal headaches increase their chance to select caffeinated is is probably not the main one involved in the consumption
coffee (containing 100 mg caffeine) by 2.6 [105]. Moreover, of tea or coffee, while it could contribute more to the
the choice of caffeine seems to be more potently controlled consumption of soft drinks. Liking the sensory properties
by avoiding withdrawal than it is by its positive effects of tea and coffee can also be related to the nutritional benefit
[176]. derived from the milk, cream and/or sugar added to the
Recently, Bickel et al. [20] reviewed 16 studies dealing beverage. The last possibility is the influence of situational
with the behavioral economics paradigm for the study of conditions on mood that can play an important role in rein-
drug abuse. Increasing consumption of a fixed price item forcing preferences for specific foods and beverages.
when another one becomes more expensive indicates a Indeed, coffee and tea are often consumed in social contexts
substitutive function and appears clearly with opiates, and during breaks from work [166]. Therefore, the influence
cocaine and phencyclidine, but not with caffeine. These of caffeine on the consumption of tea, coffee or soft drinks
data confirm the already-known fact that caffeine is less may be relatively subtle and depend both on the accumu-
reinforcing than amphetamines and related psychomotor lated dose over the day and the mood state in which it is
stimulants [34, 36, 112, 120, 185]. consumed. For example, if an individual is already quite
stimulated, the ingestion of a caffeine-containing beverage
8.3. Reinforcing effects of caffeine-containing drinks may lead to unpleasant effects. Indeed, it was shown
unrelated to caffeine recently that, at least in some individuals, the choice to
drink coffee is influenced by the interaction between the
The conditions under which caffeine functions as a rein- mood state before coffee and the effects anticipated based
forcer are still not clearly understood. However, the possible on the content of caffeine in the drink [26, 64].
reinforcing effects of coffee unrelated to caffeine, but related
to its smell, taste and social environment usually accompa-
nying coffee consumption should not be totally neglected in 9. Molecular mechanisms underlying drug dependence
the every day motivations for caffeine-containing or
caffeine-free coffee consumption. Indeed, in subjects with The molecular mechanisms underlying reinforcement
a habitual coffee consumption of 4–10 cups/day (mean and drug dependence were recently reviewed [174] and
intake 6 cups/day) and switched without a withdrawal the critical role of the mesolimbic dopamine system empha-
period to the consumption of 600 mg of caffeine either in sized. The mesolimbic dopamine system consists of the
tablets containing 50 mg of caffeine each or decaffeinated dopaminergic neurons originating in the ventral tegmental
570 A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576

area and ending in the nucleus accumbens. Rats self- caffeine-induced metabolic increase in the shell of the
administer amphetamines and dopamine directly into nucleus accumbens can only be recorded after the admin-
the nucleus accumbens and the ventral tegmental area istration of 10 mg/kg of caffeine, at which dose rates of
as well as in other brain regions connected to the cerebral glucose utilization are simultaneously increased in
mesolimbic dopaminergic system such as the cerebral the core of the nucleus accumbens ([138, 141] and unpub-
cortex, hippocampus and lateral hypothalamus [114, 119, lished personal data), which differs from the specific meta-
174]. bolic changes in the shell of the nucleus accumbens
The nucleus accumbens that plays a central role in the recorded with addictive drugs [160, 162, 184]. Moreover,
mechanism of drug dependence is functionally and morpho- the dose of 10 mg/kg of caffeine at which a metabolic
logically divided into a core and a shell part. The medio- increase is recorded in both the shell and the core of the
ventral shell part is related to the limbic ‘extended nucleus accumbens is about five times higher than the
amygdala’ assumed to play a role in emotional, motivational mean human daily caffeine consumption [44]. In addition
and reward functions, whereas the laterodorsal core part to the metabolic increase in both parts of the nucleus
regulates somatomotor functions [91]. The specificity of accumbens, that dose of caffeine leads to widespread cere-
cocaine, amphetamines, morphine, alcohol, D 9tetrahydro- bral metabolic increases recorded in most structures of the
cannabinol, and also nicotine, is to selectively activate the extrapyramidal motor system, many limbic and thalamic
dopaminergic neurotransmission in the shell of the nucleus regions, as well as several areas of the cerebral cortex
accumbens as compared with the caudate nucleus [159, 160, [138, 141]. Conversely, the doses of addictive drugs that
188], a property that has been related to the strong addictive elicit the increase in functional activity in the shell of the
properties of these drugs [119, 174]. Dopamine D2 recep- nucleus accumbens are rather low and activate only a very
tors are necessary for opiate rewarding, since in transgenic limited number of brain regions together with the latter
mice lacking these receptors, the motivational component of structure, such as the caudate nucleus in the case of amphe-
drug addiction is specifically suppressed while the behavior tamines, for example [163].
oriented at food rewarding is maintained [131]. Likewise, Taken together, these data show that caffeine increases
the dopamine transporter is an obligatory target of cocaine cerebral functional activity in the shell of the nucleus
and amphetamines, as these psychoactive drugs have no accumbens only at doses at which it already activates
effect on dopamine release and uptake in mice lacking the numerous other brain regions. In fact, caffeine primarily
dopamine transporter [72]. Conversely to the drugs of abuse acts on the extrapyramidal motor system leading to a release
that selectively lead to a release of dopamine in the shell of of dopamine in the caudate nucleus [145, 146] and on cere-
the nucleus accumbens [159, 160, 188], caffeine increases bral structures related to the sleep-wake cycle such as the
dopamine release in the caudate nucleus [145, 146], which reticular formation, raphe nuclei and locus coeruleus [138,
relates to the stimulatory properties of caffeine on locomo- 141]. These data are in good accordance with the facilitated
tor activity [139, 140], but does not induce any release of motor output [108, 130], the increase in wakefulness
dopamine in the shell of the nucleus accumbens when reported in humans after caffeine ingestion [108], and the
injected at doses ranging from 0.5–5.0 mg/kg [189]. This caffeine-induced dopamine release in the caudate nucleus
data is consistent with the low addictive potential of and the prefrontal cortex [145, 146, 188]. Conversely, the
caffeine. However, at the latter doses, caffeine stimulates effects of amphetamines, cocaine and nicotine on the neural
the release of dopamine in the prefrontal cortex, the terminal substrates underlying addiction are quite specific and occur
area of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system which is at doses that do not usually lead to the activation of many
consistent with its reinforcing and psychostimulant proper- other brain regions [160, 162, 184]. Moreover, cocaine,
ties [189]. amphetamines and nicotine induce the expression of the
immediate early gene c-fos in the nucleus accumbens and
structures related to the ‘extended amygdala’ while the
10. Effects of drugs of dependence and caffeine on expression of c-fos is only increased in the caudate nucleus
cerebral functional activity after caffeine [113, 151].
Thus, the effect of caffeine on the dopamine-mediated
Amphetamines, cocaine, and also nicotine, induce paral- brain reward system occurs only at high doses, i.e. 10 mg/
lel increases in the release of dopamine and the rates of kg in rats that correspond to the ingestion of 200–300 mg of
cerebral glucose utilization and blood flow in the nucleus caffeine in a 70 kg individual and lead to a plasma
accumbens [129, 160–163, 184], with a specific activation concentration of 13–16 mg/ml [138]. This dose is higher
of the shell of the nucleus, while there are no changes in than those necessary to activate the motor system and the
these parameters in the core of the nucleus accumbens sleep-wake cycle and induces a widespread increase in rates
[150, 160, 162, 163, 184]. Conversely, the acute adminis- of cerebral energy metabolism [138, 141] that also reflects
tration of caffeine does not lead to a release of dopamine the numerous side effects of the ingestion of rather high
and a metabolic increase in the shell of the nucleus accum- doses of caffeine and is already related to somewhat
bens at doses ranging from 1–5 mg/kg in the rat. The aversive effects.
A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576 571

11. Conclusion reduce its addictive potential and help people to progres-
sively quit smoking [142, 157]. However, it must be remem-
The objective of the present review was to analyze the bered that drug dependence can occur in some cases by the
possible dependence potential of coffee and/or caffeine. oral route, with alcohol, oral amphetamines, barbiturates
Specifically, three main factors were considered: withdra- and benzodiazepines.
wal, tolerance, and reinforcement. In addition, the molecu- In conclusion, it appears that, although caffeine fulfils
lar basis for the action of the classical drugs of dependence some of the criteria for drug dependence, the relative risk
and of caffeine was considered. of addiction of caffeine is quite low and, as reported
A withdrawal syndrome to caffeine has been described previously, is the lowest among seven drugs or drug classes
which does not seem to relate to the quantity of caffeine considered [75].
ingested daily. Tolerance to behaviorally-induced effects of
caffeine occurs in animals. In humans, tolerance to some
subjective effects of caffeine as well as partial tolerance to Acknowledgements
sleep seems to occur, at least in some individuals. Caffeine
does not act consistently as a reinforcer in animals and This study was suppported by a grant from the Institute
obviously in a more limited range of conditions than do for Scientific Information on Coffee (I.S.I.C), Paris, France.
classsical drugs of dependence. In humans, the reinforcing
stimuli functions of caffeine are limited to low or rather References
moderate doses that are usually present in a classical serving
of coffee or soft drink. [1] Ahlijanian MK, Taskemori AE. Cross-tolerance between caffeine
When considering the molecular basis for drug depen- and (-)-N 6-(phenylisopropyl)-adenosine (PIA) in mice. Life Sci
dence, it appears that the classical drugs of abuse such as 1986;38:577–588.
amphetamines, cocaine and nicotine induce specific [2] Aldridge A, Aranda JV, Neims AH. Caffeine metabolism in the
newborn. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1979;25:447–453.
increases in dopamine release and functional activity in [3] Aldridge A, Bailey J, Neims AH. The disposition of caffeine during
the shell of the nucleus accumbens, the key structure for and after pregnancy. Semin Perinatol 1981;5:310–314.
reward, motivation and addiction. Conversely, caffeine [4] American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and statistical
does not induce the release of dopamine and increase manual of mental disorders. Revised 3rd ed. Washington DC: Amer-
glucose utilization in the shell of the nucleus accumbens. ican Psychiatric Association, 1987.
[5] American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and statistical
Glucose utilization in the latter structure increases only at manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington DC: American
rather high doses that are usually avoided by the general Psychiatric Association, 1992.
population, probably since they activate the whole brain [6] Ammon HPT, Bieck PR, Mandalaz D, Verspohl EJ. Adaptation of
energy metabolism. Caffeine acts as amphetamines and blood pressure to continuous heavy coffee drinking in young volun-
cocaine on the dopaminergic system. Nicotine activates teers. A double-blind crossover study. Br J Clin Pharmacol
1983;15:701–706.
other brain areas, but shares with amphetamines and cocaine [7] Aranda JV, Collinge JM, Zinman R, Watters G. Maturation of
the property of specifically affecting the release of dopa- caffeine elimination in infancy. Arch Dis Child 1979;54:946–949.
mine in the nucleus accumbens. Other drugs, such as phen- [8] Aranda JV, Gorman W, Bergsteinsson H, Gunn T. Efficacy of
cyclidine, barbiturates and benzodiazepines, that can create caffeine in treatment of apnea in the low-birth-weight infant. J
withdrawal, reinforcement and tolerance, do not clearly Pediatr 1977;90:467–472.
[9] Aranda JV, MacLeod SM, Renton KW, Eade NR. Hepatic micro-
share dopaminergic mechanisms of action with ampheta- somal drug oxidation and electron transport in newborn infants. J
mines, nicotine and cocaine. Pediatr 1974;85:534–542.
Usually, the consumption of caffeine occurs by the oral [10] Arnaud MJ. Identification, kinetic and quantitative study of (2- 14C)
route and is gradual over the day, and the delay in the and (1-Me 14C)caffeine metabolites in rat’s urine by chromatographic
absorption of caffeine reduces the likelihood of a strong separations. Biochem Med 1976;16:67–76.
[11] Arnaud MJ. Comparative metabolic disposition of (1-Me 14C)caf-
dependence, unlike drugs of abuse that are most often admi- feine in rats, mice, and chinese hamsters. Drug Metab Dispos
nistered via the intravenous or inhaled form. When caffeine 1985;13:471–478.
is administered intravenously to subjects with histories of [12] Arnaud MJ. The pharmacology of caffeine. Progr Drug Res
stimulant drug abuse, the methylxanthine increases dose- 1987;31:273–313.
dependently ratings of positive mood (i.e. liking the drug) [13] Arnaud MJ. Metabolism of caffeine. In: Garattini S, editor. Caffeine,
coffee and health. New York: Raven Press, 1993:43–95.
and the frequency of identification to a stimulant like [14] Arnaud MJ, Welsch C. Theophylline and caffeine metabolism in
cocaine [168]. Likewise, cocaine specifically increases cere- man. In: Reitbrock N, Woodcock BG, Staib AH, editors. Theophyl-
bral glucose utilization when given to rats by the intrave- line and other methylxanthines. Friedr. Zurich: Vieweg, 1982:135–
nous route, but does not do so when given intraperitoneally 148.
[161]. A typical example of the difference in the addictive [15] Atkinson J, Ensslen M. Self-administration of caffeine by the rat.
Arzneimittelforschung 1976;26:2059–2061.
effects of a psychoactive stimulant is the case of nicotine, [16] Barone JJ, Roberts H. Human consumption of caffeine. In: Dews PB,
which is highly addictive when inhaled in the form of cigar- editor. Caffeine: perspectives from recent research. Heidelberg:
ettes while it is used as a gum or a transdermal patch to Springer, 1984:59–73.
572 A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576

[17] Barone JJ, Roberts HR. Caffeine consumption. Food Chem Toxicol [41] Daly JW. Mechanism of action of caffeine. In: Garattini S, editor.
1996;34:119–126. Caffeine, coffee and health. New York: Raven Press, 1993:97–150.
[18] Bättig K. The physiological effects of coffee consumption. In: Clif- [42] D’Amicis A, Viani R. The consumption of coffee. In: Garattini S,
ford MN, Wilson KC. editors. Coffee: botany, biochemistry and editor. Caffeine, coffee and health. New York: Raven Press, 1993:1–
production of beans and beverages. Westport, CT: AVI, 16.
1985:394–439. [43] Daval JL, Werck MC, Nehlig A, Pereira de Vasconcelos A. Quanti-
[19] Benowitz NL. Clinical pharmacology of caffeine. Ann Rev Med tative autoradiographic study of the postnatal development of adeno-
1990;41:277–288. sine A1 receptors and their coupling to G proteins in the rat brain.
[20] Bickel WK, Degrandpre RJ, Higgins ST. The behavioral economics Neuroscience 1991;40:841–851.
of concurrent drug reinforcers: a review and reanalysis of drug self- [44] Debry G. Coffee and health. Paris: Libbey, 1994.
administration research. Psychopharmacology 1995;118:250–259. [45] Denaro CP, Brown CR, Jacob III P, Benowitz NL. Effects of caffeine
[21] Blanchard J, Sawers SJA. The absolute bioavailability of caffeine in with repeated dosing. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 1991;40:273–278.
man. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 1983;24:93–98. [46] Denaro CP, Brown CR, Wilson M, Jacob III P, Benowitz NL. Dose-
[22] Blanchard J, Sawers SJA. Comparative pharmacokinetics of caffeine dependency of caffeine metabolism. Clin Pharmacol Ther
in young and elderly men. J Pharmacokin Biopharmaceut 1990;31:358–369.
1983;11:109–126. [47] Deneau G, Yanagita T, Seevers MH. Self-administration of
[23] Bonati M, Latini R, Galetti F, Young JF, Tognoni G, Garattini S. psychoactive substances in the monkey: a measure of psychological
Caffeine disposition after oral doses. Clin Pharmacol Ther dependence. Psychopharmacologia 1969;16:30–48.
1982;32:98–106. [48] Dreisbach RH, Pfeiffer C. Caffeine withdrawal headache. J Lab Clin
[24] Bonati M, Latini R, Tognoni G, Young JF, Garattini S. Interspecies Med 1943;28:1212–1219.
comparison of in vivo caffeine pharmacokinetics in man, monkey, [49] Dworkin SI, Vrana SL, Broadbent J, Robinson JH. Comparing the
rabbit, rat, and mouse. Drug Metab Rev 1984;15:1355–1383. reinforcing effects of nicotine, caffeine, methylphenidate and
[25] Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Caffeine use as a model of acute and chronic caffeine. Med Chem Res 1993;2:593–602.
insomnia. Sleep 1992;15:526–536. [50] Eddy NB, Downs AW. Tolerance and cross-tolerance in the human
[26] Booth DA, French JA, Wainwright CJ, Gatherer AJH. Personal subject to the diuretic effect of caffeine, theobromine and theophyl-
benefits from post-ingestional actions of dietary constituents. Proc line. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1928;33:167–174.
Nutr Soc 1992;51:335–341. [51] Ellison CR, Singer MR, Moore LL, Nguyen USDT, Garrahie E,
[27] Brauer LH, Buican B, de Wit H. Effects of caffeine deprivation on Maror JK. Current caffeine intake in young children: amount and
taste and mood. Behav Pharmacol 1994;5:111–118. sources. J Amer Dietetic Assoc 1995;95:802–804.
[28] Brazier JL, Ritter J, Berland M, Khenfer D, Faucon G. Pharmaco- [52] Evans SM, Critchfield TS, Griffiths RR. Caffeine reinforcement
kinetics of caffeine during and after pregnancy. Dev Pharmacol Ther demonstrated in a majority of moderate caffeine users. Behav Phar-
1983;6:315–322. macol 1994;5:231–238.
[29] Breslau N, Kilbey MM, Andreski P. Nicotine dependence and major [53] Evans SM, Griffiths RR. Caffeine tolerance and choice in humans.
depression: new evidence from a prospective investigation. Arch Psychopharmacology 1992;108:51–59.
Gen Psychiatry 1993;50:31–35. [54] Fastbom J, Pazos A, Ungerstedt U, Rudolphi K, Fredholm BB.
[30] Brovsic GM, Rowe MM. Methylxanthines, adenosine, and human Protective effect of adenosine and a novel xanthine derivative
taste responsivity. Physiol Behav 1992;52:559–563. propentofylline on the cell damage after bilateral carotid occlusion
[31] Carney JM. Effects of caffeine, theophylline and theobromine on in the gerbil hippocampus. Neuroscience 1990;22:813–826.
scheduled controlled responding in rats. Brit J Pharmacol [55] Fennelly M, Galletly DC, Purdie GI. Is caffeine withdrawal the
1982;75:451–454. mechanism of postoperative headaches?. Anesth Analg
[32] Carrier O, Pons G, Rey E, Richard MO, Moran C, Badoual J, Olive 1991;72:449–453.
G. Maturation of caffeine metabolic pathways in infancy. Clin Phar- [56] Ferré S, Fuxe K, von Euler G, Johansson B, Fredholm BB. Adeno-
macol Ther 1988;44:145–151. sine–dopamine interactions in the brain. Neuroscience
[33] Carroll ME, Hagen EW, Asencio M, Brauer LH. Behavioral depen- 1992;51:501–512.
dence on caffeine and phencyclidine in rhesus monkeys: interactive [57] Ferré S, von Euler G, Johansson B, Fredholm BB, Fuxe K. Stimula-
effects. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1989;31:927–932. tion of high-affinity adenosine A-2 receptors decreases the affinity of
[34] Chait LD, Uhlenhuth EH, Johanson CE. Reinforcing and subjective dopamine D-2 receptors in rat striatal membranes. Proc Natl Acad
effects of several anorectics in normal human volunteers. J Pharma- Sci USA 1991;88:7237–7241.
col Exp Ther 1987;242:777–783. [58] Fink JS, Weaver DR, Rivkees SA, Peterfreund RA, Pollack AE,
[35] Chou DT, Khan S, Forde J, Hirsh K. Caffeine tolerance: behavioral, Adler EM, Reppert SM. Molecular cloning of the rat A2 adenosine
electrophysiological and neurochemical evidence. Life Sci receptor: selective expression with D2 dopamine receptors in rat
1985;36:2347–2358. striatum. Mol Brain Res 1992;14:186–195.
[36] Clubley M, Bye CE, Henson TA, Peck AW, Riddington CJ. Effects [59] Finn IB, Holtzman SG. Tolerance to caffeine-induced stimulation of
of caffeine and cyclizine alone and in combination on human perfor- locomotor activity in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1986;238:542–
mance, subjective effects and EEG activity. Brit J Clin Pharmacol 546.
1979;7:157–163. [60] Finn IB, Holtzman SG. Pharmacological specificity of tolerance to
[37] Collins RJ, Weeks JR, Cooper MM, Good PI, Russell RR. Prediction caffeine-induced stimulation of locomotor activity. Psychopharma-
of abuse liability of drugs using IV self-administration by rats. cology 1987;93:428–434.
Psychopharmacology 1984;82:6–13. [61] Finn IB, Holtzman SG. Tolerance and cross-tolerance to theophyl-
[38] Colton T, Gosselin RE, Smith RP. The tolerance of coffee drinkers to line-induced stimulation of locomotor activity in rats. Pharmacol
caffeine. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1968;9:31–39. Biochem Behav 1988;35:477–479.
[39] Couturier EGM, Laman DM, van Duijn MAJ, van Duijn H. Influ- [62] Fredholm BB. Are methylxanthine effects due to antagonism of
ence of caffeine and caffeine withdrawal on headache and cerebral endogenous adenosine?. Trends Pharmacol Sci 1980;1:129–132.
blood flow velocities. Cephalalgia 1997;17:188–190. [63] Fredholm BB. Adenosine, adenosine receptors and the actions of
[40] Curatolo PW, Robertson D. The health consequences of caffeine. caffeine. Pharmacol Toxicol 1995;76:93–101.
Ann Int Med 1983;98:641–653. [64] French JA, Wainwright CJ, Booth DA. Caffeine and mood: indivi-
A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576 573

dual differences in low-dose caffeine sensitivity. Appetite experimental pharmacology, vol. 118. Heidelberg: Springer,
1994;22:277–279. 1996:315–341.
[65] Garrett BE, Griffiths RR. The role of dopamine in the behavioral [86] Griffiths RR, Woodson PP. Reinforcing effects of caffeine in
effects of caffeine in animals and humans. Pharmacol Biochem humans. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1988;246:21–28.
Behav 1997;57:533–541. [87] Griffiths RR, Woodson PP. Reinforcing properties of caffeine:
[66] Garrett BE, Holtzman SG. Caffeine cross-tolerance to selective studies in humans and laboratory animals. Pharmacol Biochem
dopamine D1 and D2 receptor agonists but not to their synergistic Behav 1988;29:419–427.
interaction. Eur J Pharmacol 1994;262:65–75. [88] Griffiths RR, Woodson PP. Caffeine physical dependence: a review
[67] Garrett BE, Holtzman SG. Does adenosine receptor blockade of human and laboratory animal studies. Psychopharmacology
mediate caffeine-induced rotational behavior?. J Pharmacol Exp 1988;94:437–451.
Ther 1995;274:207–214. [89] Hampl KF, Schneider MC, Ruttimann U, Ummenhofer U, Drewe J.
[68] Georgiev V, Johansson B, Fredholm BB. Long-term caffeine treat- Perioperative administration of caffeine tablets for prevention of
ment leads to a decreased susceptibility to NMDA-induced clonic postoperative headaches. Can J Anaesth 1995;42:789–792.
seizures in mice without changes in adenosine A1 receptor number. [90] Hart P, Farrell GC, Cooksley WGE, Powell IW. Enhanced drug
Brain Res 1993;612:271–277. metabolism in cigarette smokers. Brit Med J 1976;2:147–149.
[69] Gilbert RM. Caffeine as a drug of abuse. In: Gibbins RJ, Israel Y, [91] Heimer L, Zahm DS, Churchill L, Kalivas PW, Wohltmann C.
Kalant H, Popham RE, Schmidt W, Smart RG, editors. Research Specificity in the projection patterns of accumbal core and shell in
advances in alcohol and drug problems, vol. 3. New York: Wiley, the rat. Neuroscience 1991;41:89–125.
1976:49–77. [92] Heishman SJ, Henningfield JE. Stimulus functions of caffeine in
[70] Gilbert RM. Caffeine consumption. In: Spiller GA, editor. The humans: relation to dependence potential. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
methylxanthine beverages and foods: chemistry, consumption, and 1992;16:273–287.
health effects. New York: Liss, 1984:185–213. [93] Heishman SJ, Henningfield JE. Is caffeine a drug of dependence?
[71] Gilliland K, Bullock W. Caffeine: a potential drug of abuse. Adv Criteria and comparisons. Pharmacopsychoecologia 1994;7:127–
Alcohol Subst Abuse 1984;3:53–73. 136.
[72] Giros B, Jaber M, Jones SR, Wightman RM, Caron MG. Hyperlo- [94] Hirsch K. Central nervous system pharmacology of the dietary
comotion and indifference to cocaine and amphetamine in mice methylxanthines. In: Spiller GA, editor. The methylxanthine
lacking the dopamine transporter. Nature 1996;379:606–612. beverages and food: chemistry, consumption, and health effects.
[73] Goldstein A, Kaizer S. Psychotropic effects of caffeine in man. III. A New York: Liss, 1984:235–301.
questionnaire survey of coffee drinking and its effects in a group of [95] Hirsch KR, Forde J, Chou DT. Demonstration of caffeine tolerance
housewifes. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1969;10:477–488. in reticular formation neurons. Fed Proc 1983;42:882.
[74] Goldstein A, Kaizer S, Whitby O. Psychotropic effects of caffeine in [96] Höfer L, Bättig K. Cardiovascular, behavioral, and subjective effects
man. IV. Quantitative and qualitative differences associated with of caffeine under field conditions. Pharmacol Biochem Behav
habituation to coffee. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1969;10:489–497. 1994;48:899–908.
[75] Goldstein A, Kalant H. Drug policy: striking the right balance. [97] Höfer L, Bättig K. Psychophysiological effects of switching to
Science 1990;249:1513–1521. caffeine tablets or decaffeinated coffee under field conditions. Phar-
[76] Goldstein A, Warren R, Kaizer S. Psychotropic effects of caffeine in macopsychoecologia 1994;7:169–177.
man. I. Interindividual differences in sensitivity to caffeine-induced [98] Holtzman SG. Complete, reversible, drug-specific tolerance to
wakefulness. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1965;149:156–159. stimulation of locomotor activity by caffeine. Life Sci
[77] Goodman RR, Snyder SH. Autoradiographic localization of adeno- 1983;33:779–787.
sine receptors in rat brain using (3 H) cyclohexyladenosine. J [99] Holtzman SG. Caffeine as a model drug of abuse. Trends Pharmacol
Neurosci 1982;2:1230–1241. Sci 1990;11:355–356.
[78] Griffiths RR, Bigelow GE, Liebson IA. Human coffee drinking: [100] Holtzman SG, Finn IB. Tolerance to the behavioral effects of
reinforcing and physical dependence producing effects of caffeine. caffeine in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1988;29:411–418.
J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1986;239:416–425. [101] Holtzman SG, Mante S, Minneman KP. Role of adenosine receptors
[79] Griffiths RR, Bigelow GE, Liebson IA, O’Keffe M, O’Leary D, Russ in caffeine tolerance. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1991;256:62–68.
N. Human coffee drinking: manipulation of concentration and [102] Howell LL. Comparative effects of caffeine and selective phospho-
caffeine dose. J Exp Anal Behav 1986;45:133–148. diesterase inhibitors on respiration and behavior in rhesus monkeys.
[80] Griffiths RR, Bigelow GE, Liebson IA. Reinforcing effects of J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1993;266:894–903.
caffeine in coffee and capsules. J Exp Anal Behav 1989;52: [103] Hughes JR, Higgins ST, Bickel WK, Hunt WK, Fenwick JW, Gulli-
127–140. ver SB, Mireault GC. Caffeine self-administration, withdrawal, and
[81] Griffiths RR, Brady JV, Bradford LD. Predicting the abuse liability adverse effects among coffee drinkers. Arch Gen Psychiatry
of drugs with animal drug self-administration procedures: psycho- 1991;48:611–617.
motor stimulants and hallucinogens. In: Thompson D, Dews PB, [104] Hughes JR, Hunt WK, Higgins ST, Bickel WK, Fenwick JW, Pepper
editors. Advances in behavioral pharmacology, vol. 2. New York: SL. Effect of dose on the ability of caffeine to serve as a reinforcer in
Academic Press, 1979:163–208. humans. Behav Pharmacol 1992;3:218–221.
[82] Griffiths RR, Evans SM, Heishman SJ, Preston KL, Sannerud CA, [105] Hughes JR, Oliveto AH, Bickel WK, Higgins ST, Badger GJ.
Wolf B, Woodson PP. Low-dose caffeine discrimination in humans. Caffeine self-administration and withdrawal: incidence, individual
J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1990;252:970–978. differences and interrelationships. Drug Alcohol Depend
[83] Griffiths RR, Evans SM, Heishman SJ, Preston KL, Sannerud CA, 1993;32:239–246.
Wolf B, Woodson PP. Low-dose caffeine physical dependence in [106] Hughes JR, Oliveto AH, Helzer JE, Bickel WK, Higgins ST. Indica-
humans. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1990;255:1123–1132. tions of caffeine dependence in a population-based sample. In:
[84] Griffiths RR, Mumford GK. Caffeine — a drug of abuse? In: Bloom Harris LS, editor. Problems of drug dependence 1992. NIDA
FE, Kupfer DJ, editors. Psychopharmacology: the fourth generation research monographs no. 132, NIH publication no. 93-3505.
of progress. New York: Raven Press, 1995:1699–1713. Washington DC: US Government Printing Office, 1993:194.
[85] Griffiths RR, Mumford GK. Caffeine reinforcement, discrimination, [107] Hughes JR, Oliveto AH, Helzer JE, Higgins ST, Bickel WK. Should
tolerance and physical dependence in laboratory animals and caffeine abuse, dependence, or withdrawal be added to DSM-IV and
humans. In: Schuster CR, Kuhar MJ, editors. Handbook of ICD-10?. Amer J Psychiatry 1992;149:33–40.
574 A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576

[108] James JE. Caffeine and health. London: Academic Press, 1991. [132] Marks V, Kelly JF. Absorption of caffeine from tea, coffee and coca
[109] James JE, Stirling KP. Caffeine: a survey of some of the known and cola. Lancet 1973;1:827.
suspected deleterious effects of habitual use. Brit J Addict [133] Matthew RJ, Wilson WH. Caffeine consumption, withdrawal and
1983;78:251–258. cerebral blood flow. Headache 1985;25:305–309.
[110] Jarvis MF, Williams M. Direct autoradiographic localization of [134] McGowan JD, Altman RE, Kanto WP. Neonatal withdrawal symp-
adenosine A2 receptors in the rat brain using the A2-selective toms after chronic maternal ingestion of caffeine. South Med J
agonist, (3 H)CGS 21680. Eur J Pharmacol 1989;168:243–246. 1988;81:1092–1094.
[111] Joeres R, Klinker H, Heusler H, Epping J, Zilly W, Richter E. Influ- [135] Mela DJ, Mattes RD, Tanimura S, Garcia-Medina MR. Relation-
ence of smoking on caffeine elimination in healthy volunteers and in ships between ingestion and gustatory perception of caffeine. Phar-
patients with alcoholic liver cirrhosis. Hepatology 1988;8:575–579. macol Biochem Behav 1992;43:513–521.
[112] Johanson CE, Kilgore K, Uhlenhuth EH. Assessment of dependence [136] Mumford GK, Neill DB, Holtzman SG. Caffeine elevates reinforce-
potential of drugs in humans using multiple indices. Psychopharma- ment threshold for electrical brain stimulation: tolerance and with-
cology 1983;81:144–149. drawal changes. Brain Res 1988;459:163–167.
[113] Johansson B, Lindström K, Fredholm BB. Differences in the regio- [137] Murphy TL, McIvor G, Yap A, Cooksley WGF, Hallyday JW,
nal and cellular localization of c-fos messenger RNA induced by Powell IW. The effect of smoking on caffeine elimination: implica-
amphetamine, cocaine and caffeine in the rat. Neuroscience tion for its use as a semiquantitative test of liver function. Clin Exp
1994;59:837–849. Pharmacol Physiol 1988;15:9–13.
[114] Johnson SW, North RA. Opioids excite dopamine neurons by hyper- [138] Nehlig A, Daval JL, Boyet S, Vert P. Comparative effects of acute
polarization of local interneurons. J Neurosci 1992;12:483–488. and chronic administration of caffeine on local cerebral glucose
[115] Katz JL, Goldberg SR. Psychomotor stimulant effects of caffeine utilization in the conscious rat. Eur J Pharmacol 1986;129:93–103.
alone and in combination with an adenosine analog in the squirrel [139] Nehlig A, Daval JL, Debry G. Caffeine and the central nervous
monkey. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1987;242:179–187. system: mechanisms of action, biochemical, metabolic and psychos-
[116] Kendler KS, Neale MC, MacLean CJ, Heath AC, Eaves LJ, Kessler timulant effects. Brain Res Rev 1992;17:139–170.
RC. Smoking and major depression: a causal analysis. Arch Gen [140] Nehlig A, Debry G. Effects of coffee on the central nervous system.
Psychiatry 1993;50:36–43. In: Debry G, editor. Coffee and health. London: Libbey, 1994:157–
[117] Kessler RC, McGonagh KA, Zhao S, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Eshle- 249.
man S, Wittchen HU. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM- [141] Nehlig A, Lucignani G, Kadekaro M, Porrino LJ, Sokoloff L. Effects
III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the of acute administration of caffeine on local cerebral glucose utiliza-
National Comorbidity Survey. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1994;51:8–19. tion in the rat. Eur J Pharmacol 1984;101:91–100.
[118] Knutti R, Rothweiler H, Schlatter C. Effect of pregnancy on the [142] Nemeth-Coslett R, Henningfield JE, O’Keefe MK, Griffiths RR.
pharmacokinetics of caffeine. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 1981;21:121– Nicotine gum: dose-related effects on cigarette smoking and subjec-
126. tive ratings. Psychopharmacology 1987;92:424–430.
[119] Koob GF. Drugs of abuse: anatomy, pharmacology and function of [143] Newland MC, Brown K. Oral caffeine consumption by rats: the role
reward pathways. Trends Pharmacol Sci 1992;13:177–184. of flavor history, concentration, concurrent food, and an adenosine
[120] Kosman ME, Unna KR. Effects of chronic administration of the agonist. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1992;42:651–659.
amphetamines and other stimulants on behavior. Clin Pharmacol [144] Nikolajsen L, Larsen KM, Kierkegaard O. Effect of previous
Ther 1968;9:240–254. frequency of headache, duration of fasting and caffeine abstinence
[121] Kozlowski LT. Effect of caffeine on coffee drinking. Nature on perioperative headache. Brit J Anaesth 1994;72:295–297.
1976;264:354–355. [145] Okada M, Kiryu K, Kawata Y, Mizuno K, Wada K, Tasaki H,
[122] Kozlowski LT, Henningfield JE, Keenan RM, Lei H, Leigh G, Jeli- Kaneko S. Determination of the effects of caffeine and carbamaze-
nek LC, Pope MA, Haertzen CA. Patterns of alcohol, cigarette, and pine on striatal dopamine release by in vivo microdialysis. Eur J
caffeine and other drug use in two abusing populations. J Subst Pharmacol 1997;321:181–188.
Abuse Treat 1993;10:171–179. [146] Okada M, Mizuno K, Kaneko S. Adenosine A1 and A2 receptors
[123] Kuznicki JT, Turner LS. The effects of caffeine on caffeine users and modulate extracellular dopamine levels in rat striatum. Neurosci Lett
non-users. Physiol Behav 1986;37:397–408. 1996;212:53–56.
[124] Lane JD. Effects of brief caffeinated-beverage deprivation on mood, [147] Oliveto AH, Hughes JR, Higgins ST, Bickel WK, Pepper SL, Shea
symptoms, and psychomotor performance. Pharmacol Biochem PJ, Fenwick JW. Forced-choice versus free-choice procedures:
Behav 1997;58:203–208. caffeine self-administration in humans. Psychopharmacology
[125] Le Guennec C, Billon B. Delay in caffeine elimination in breast-fed 1992;109:85–91.
infants. Pediatrics 1987;79:264–268. [148] Oliveto AH, Hughes JR, Pepper SL, Bickel WK, Higgins ST. Low
[126] Levy M, Zylber-Katz E. Caffeine metabolism and coffee attributed doses of caffeine can serve as reinforcers in humans. In: Harris LS,
sleep disturbances. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1983;33:770–775. editor. Problems of drug dependence, 1990, NIDA research mono-
[127] Liebermann HR, Wurtmann RJ, Emde GG, Coviella ILG. The graph no. 105. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office,
effects of caffeine and aspirin on mood and performance. J Clin 1991:442.
Psychopharmacol 1987;7:315–320. [149] Ongini E, Fredholm BB. Pharmacology of adenosine A2 receptors.
[128] Liebermann HR, Wurtmann RJ, Emde GG, Roberts C, Coviella ILG. Trends Pharmacol Sci 1996;17:364–372.
The effects of low doses of caffeine on human performance and [150] Orzi F, Morelli M, Fieschi C, Pontieri FE. Metabolic mapping of the
mood. Psychopharmacology 1987;92:308–312. pharmacological and toxicological effects of dopaminergic drugs in
[129] London ED, Connolly RJ, Szikszay M, Wamsley JK, Dam M. experimental animals. Cerebrovasc Brain Metab Rev 1993;5:95–
Effects of nicotine on local cerebral glucose utilization in the rat. J 121.
Neurosci 1988;8:3920–3928. [151] Pagliusi SR, Tessari M, DeVevey S, Chiamulera C, Pich EM. The
[130] Lorist MM, Snel J, Kok A. Influence of caffeine on information reinforcing properties of nicotine are associated with a specific
processing stages in well rested and fatigued subjects. Psychophar- patterning of c-fos expression in the rat brain. Eur J Neurosci
macology 1994;113:411–421. 1996;8:2247–2256.
[131] Maldonado R, Salardi A, Valverde O, Samad TA, Roques BP, [152] Paire M, van Lieferinghen P, Desvignes V, Dubray C, Raynaud EJ,
Borelli E. Absence of opiate rewarding effects in mice lacking dopa- Lavarenne J. Cinétique de la caféine au cours des premiers mois de
mine D2 receptors. Nature 1997;388:586–589. la vie et implications pratiques. Sem Hôp Paris 1988;64:1813–1817.
A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576 575

[153] Parkinson FE, Fredholm BB. Autoradiographic evidence for G- [174] Self DW, Nestler EJ. Molecular mechanisms of drug reinforcement
protein coupled A2-receptors in rat neostriatum using (3 H)-CGS and addiction. Ann Rev Neurosci 1995;18:463–495.
21680 as a ligand. Naunyn-Schmiedeberg’s Arch Pharmacol [175] Shi J, Benowitz NL, Denaro CP, Sheiner LB. Pharmacokinetic-phar-
1990;342:85–89. macodynamic modeling of caffeine: tolerance to pressor effects. Clin
[154] Parsons WD, Neims AH. Prolonged half-life of caffeine in healthy Pharmacol Ther 1993;53:6–14.
term newborn infants. J Pediatr 1981;98:640–641. [176] Shuh KJ, Griffiths RR. Caffeine reinforcement: the role of withdra-
[155] Patwardhan RV, Desmond PV, Johnson RF, Schenker S. Impaired wal. Psychopharmacology 1997;130:320–326.
elimination of caffeine by oral contraceptive steroids. J Lab Clin [177] Silverman K, Evans SM, Strain EC, Griffiths RR. Withdrawal
Med 1980;95:603–608. syndrome after the double-blind cessation of caffeine consumption.
[156] Pearlman SA, Duran C, Wood MA, Maisels MJ, Berlin CM. New Engl J Med 1992;327:1109–1114.
Caffeine pharmacokinetics in preterm infants older than 2 weeks. [178] Silverman K, Griffiths RR. Low-dose discrimination and self-
Dev Pharmacol Ther 1989;12:65–69. reported mood effects in normal volunteers. J Exp Anal Behav
[157] Pickworth WB, Bunker EB, Henningfield HE. Transdermal nicotine: 1992;57:91–107.
reduction of smoking with minimal abuse liability. Psychopharma- [179] Silverman K, Mumford GK, Griffiths RR. Enhancing caffeine rein-
cology 1994;115:9–14. forcement by behavioral requirements following drug ingestion.
[158] Pollack AE, Fink JS. Adenosine antagonists potentiate D2 dopamine- Psychopharmacology 1994;114:424–432.
dependent activation of fos in the striatopallidal pathway. [180] Sinton CM, Petitjean F. The influence of chronic caffeine adminis-
Neuroscience 1995;68:721–728. tration on sleep parameters in the cat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav
[159] Pontieri FE, Tanda G, Dichiara G. Intravenous cocaine, morphine, 1989;32:459–462.
and amphetamine preferentially increase extracellular dopamine in [181] Smith AP. Caffeine dependence: an alternative view. Nature Medi-
the ‘shell’ as compared with the ‘core’ of the rat nucleus accumbens. cine 1996;2:494.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1995;92:12304–12308. [182] Snel J. Coffee and caffeine. Sleep and wakefulness. In: Garattini S,
[160] Pontieri FE, Tanda G, Orzi F, Dichiara G. Effects of nicotine on the editor. Coffee, caffeine and health. New York: Raven Press,
nucleus accumbens and similarity to those of addictive drugs. Nature 1993:255–290.
1996;382:255–257. [183] Steigerwald ES, Rusiniak KW, Eckel DL, O’Regan MH. Aversive
[161] Porrino LJ. Functional consequences of acute cocaine treatment conditioning properties of caffeine in rats. Pharmacol Biochem
depend on route of administration. Psychopharmacology Behav 1989;31:579–584.
1993;112:343–351. [184] Stein EA, Fuller SA. Selective effects of cocaine on regional
[162] Porrino LJ, Domer FR, Crane AM, Sokoloff L. Selective alterations cerebral blood flow in the rat. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1992;
in cerebral metabolism within the mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic 262:327–334.
system produced by acute cocaine administration in rats. Neuropsy- [185] Stern KN, Chait LD, Johanson CE. Reinforcing and subjective
chopharmacology 1988;1:109–118. effects of caffeine in normal human volunteers. Psychopharmacol-
[163] Porrino LJ, Lucignani G, Dow-Edwards D, Sokoloff L. Correlation ogy 1989;98:81–88.
of dose-dependent effects of acute amphetamine administration on [186] Strain EC, Griffiths RR. Caffeine dependence: fact or fiction?. J Roy
behavior and local cerebral metabolism in rats. Brain Res Soc Med 1995;88:437–440.
1984;307:311–320. [187] Strain EC, Mumford GK, Silverman K, Griffiths RR. Caffeine
[164] Richardson NJ, Rogers PJ, Elliman NA, O’Dell RJ. Mood and dependence syndrome. Evidence from case histories and experimen-
performance effects of caffeine in relation to acute and chronic tal evaluations. JAMA 1994;272:1043–1048.
caffeine deprivation. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1995;52:313– [188] Tanda G, Pontieri FE, DiChiara G. Cannabinnoid and
320. heroin activation of mesolimbic dopamine transmission by
[165] Robertson D, Wade D, Workman R, Woosley RL, Oates JA. Toler- a common m1 opioid receptor mechanism. Science 1997;
ance to the humoral and hemodynamic effects of caffeine in man. J 276:2048–2050.
Clin Invest 1981;67:1111–1117. [189] Tanda G, Loddo P, Frau R, Acquas E, Dichiara G. Effect of intra-
[166] Rogers PJ, Richardson NJ. Why do we like drinks that contain venous caffeine on limbic and cortical dopamine transmission in the
caffeine?. Trends Food Sci Technol 1993;4:108–111. rat: a microdialysis study. Proceedings of the 6th International
[167] Rogers PJ, Richardson NJ, Dernoncourt C. Caffeine use: is there a Symposium on Adenosine and Adenine Nucleotides, Ferrara, Italy,
net benefit for mood and psychomotor performance?. Neuropsycho- May 19–24, 1998.
biology 1995;31:195–199. [190] Tiffin P, Ashton H, Marsh R, Kamali F. Pharmacokinetic and phar-
[168] Rush CR, Sullivan JT, Griffiths RR. Intravenous caffeine in stimu- macodynamic responses to caffeine in poor and normal sleepers.
lant drug abusers: subjective reports and physiological effects. J Psychopharmacology 1995;121:494–502.
Pharmacol Exp Ther 1995;273:351–358. [191] Viani R. Composition of coffee. In: Garattini S, editor. Caffeine,
[169] Satoh H, Tanaka T. Comparative pharmacology between habitual coffee and health. New York: Raven Press, 1993:17–41.
and non-habitual coffee-drinkers: a practical class exercise in phar- [192] Viani R. Caffeine consumption. Proceedings of the Caffeine Work-
macology. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 1997;52:239–240. shop, Thai FDA and ILSI Bangkok, Thailand, 1996.
[170] Schenk S, Worley CM, McNamara C, Valadez A. Acute and [193] Vitiello M, Woods SC. Caffeine: preferential consumption by rats.
repeated exposure to caffeine: effects on reinstatement of extin- Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1977;3:147–149.
guished cocaine-taking behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology [194] von Borstel RW, Wurtman RJ, Conlay LA. Chronic caffeine
1996;126:17–23. consumption potentiates the hypotensive action of circulating
[171] Schiffman SN, Jacobs O, Vanderhaeghen JJ. Striatal restricted adenosine. Life Sci 1983;32:1151–1158.
adenosine A2 receptor (RDC8) is expressed by enkephalin but not [195] Weber JG, Ereth MH, Danielson DR. Perioperative ingestion of
by substance P neurons: an in situ hybridization histochemistry caffeine and postoperative headache. Mayo Clin Proc
study. J Neurochem 1991;57:1062–1067. 1993;68:842–845.
[172] Schuster CR, Woods JH, Seevers MH. Self-administration of central [196] Weber JG, Klindworth JT, Arnold JJ, Danielson DR, Ereth MK.
stimulants by the monkey. In: Sjoqvist F, Tottie M, editors. Abuse of Prophylactic intravenous administration of caffeine and recovery
central stimulants. New York: Raven Press, 1969:339–347. after ambulatory surgical procedure. Mayo Clin Proc
[173] SECED. Rapport descriptif de la consommation de café, Paris, 1997;72:621–626.
France, 1980. [197] World Health Organization (WHO). The ICD-10 classification of
576 A. Nehlig / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23 (1999) 563–576

mental and behavioural disorders. World Health Organization: caffeine-induced seizures in rats. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med
Geneva, 1994. 1988;188:185–190.
[198] Worley CM, Valadez A, Schenk S. Reinstatement of extinguished [200] Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos A, Roehrs T, Lipschutz L, Timms V, Roth
cocaine-taking behavior by cocaine and caffeine. Pharmacol T. Effects of caffeine on alertness. Psychopharmacology
Biochem Behav 1994;48:217–221. 1990;100:36–39.
[199] Yasuhara M, Levy G. Rapid development of functional tolerance to

You might also like