You are on page 1of 8

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/237065878

Becoming a Self-Regulated Learner: An Overview

Article  in  Theory Into Practice · June 2002


DOI: 10.1207/s15430421tip4102_2

CITATIONS READS
1,665 21,637

1 author:

Barry J. Zimmerman
CUNY Graduate Center
167 PUBLICATIONS   30,485 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Barry J. Zimmerman on 19 December 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Barry . Zimmerman

Becoming a Self-Regulated
Learner: An Overview

N AN ERA OF CONSTANTDISTRACTIONSin the form Self-regulation researchers have sought to


of portable phones, CD players, computers, and understand students like Tracy and to provide help
televisions for even young children, it is hardly in developing key processes that she lacks, such as
surprising to discover that many students have not goal setting, time management, learning strategies,
learned to self-regulate their academic studying self-evaluation, self-attributions, seeking help or
very well. Consider the case of Tracy, a high school information,and importantself-motivationalbeliefs,
student who is infatuated with MTV. such as self-efficacy and intrinsic task interest.
An important mid-term math exam is two In recent years, there have been exciting dis-
weeks away, and she has begun to study while coveries regarding the nature, origins, and devel-
listening to popular music "to relax her." Tracy opment of how students regulate their own learning
has not set any study goals for herself-instead processes (Zimmerman& Schunk, 2001). Although
she simply tells herself to do as well as she can on these studies have clearly revealed how self-regu-
the test. She uses no specific learning strategies latory processes lead to success in school, few
for condensing and memorizing important material teachers currently prepare students to learn on their
and does not plan out her study time, so she ends up own. In this article, I discuss students' self-regula-
cramming for a few hours before the test. She has tion as a way to compensate for their individual
only vague self-evaluativestandardsand cannotgauge differences in learning, define the essential quali-
her academic preparationaccurately.Tracy attributes ties of academic self-regulation, describe the struc-
her learning difficulties to an inherentlack of mathe- ture and function of self-regulatory processes, and,
matical ability and is very defensive about her poor finally, give an overview of methods for guiding
study methods. However, she does not ask for help students to learn on their own.
from others because she is afraid of "looking stu-
pid," or seek out supplementarymaterials from the Changing Conceptions of
librarybecause she "alreadyhas too much to learn." Individual Differences
She finds studying to be anxiety-provoking,has little Since the beginning of public schooling in
self-confidence in achieving success, and sees little the United States, educators have wrestled with
intrinsic value in acquiring mathematical skill. the presence of substantial differences in individu-
al students' backgrounds and modes of learning.
Barry J. Zimmerman is Distinguished Professor at the
Graduate School and University Center of the City Some students grasped important concepts easily
University of New York. and seemed highly motivated to study, whereas

THEORYINTO PRACTICE, Volume 41, Number 2, Spring 2002


Copyright ? 2002 College of Education, The Ohio State University
Zimmerman
Becoming a Self-Regulated Learner

others struggled to understand and retain informa- in achieving these goals. Students who set specific
tion and often seemed disinterested. In the 19th and proximal goals for themselves displayed supe-
century, learning was viewed as a formal disci- rior achievement and perceptions of personal effi-
pline, and a student's failure to learn was widely cacy. Interestingly, simply asking students to
attributed to personal limitations in intelligence or self-record some aspect of their learning, such as
diligence. Students were expected to overcome their the completion of assignments, often led to "spon-
individual limitations in order to profit from the taneous" improvements in functioning (Shapiro,
curriculum of the school. Conceptions of self-reg- 1984). These effects, termed reactivity in the sci-
ulatory development at the time were limited to entific literature, implied that students' metacog-
acquiring desirable personal habits, such as proper nitive (i.e., self) awareness of particular aspects of
diction and handwriting. their functioning could enhance their self-control.
At the dawn of the 20th century, psychology Of course, self-awareness is often insufficient when
emerged as a science, and the topic of individual a learner lacks fundamental skills, but it can pro-
differences in educational functioning attracted duce a readiness that is essential for personal
widespread interest. Diverse reformers, such as change (Zimmerman, 2001).
John Dewey, E.L. Thorndike, Maria Montessori, These and related results led researchers to
and the progressive educators, suggested various attribute individual differences in learning to stu-
ways to alter the curriculum to accommodate stu- dents' lack of self-regulation. This perspective fo-
dents' individual differences, such as grouping of cused instead on what students needed to know
students homogeneously according to age or ability, about themselves in order to manage their limita-
introducing perceptual-motor learning tasks, and tions during efforts to learn, such as a dyslexic
broadeningcourse work to include trainingin practi- student's knowing to use a particular strategy to
cal skills. Laterreformersmatchedinstructionaltreat- read. Although teachers also need to know a stu-
ments to students' aptitude or attitude scores on dent's strengths and limitations in learning, their
standardizedtests (Cronbach, 1957). Despite these goal should be to empower their students to be-
notable efforts, critics charged that the curriculum come self-aware of these differences. If a student
of American schools remained too narrow and in- fails to understandsome aspect of a lesson in class,
flexible to accommodate the psychological needs he or she must possess the self-awareness and stra-
of all students. Many psychologists and educators tegic knowledge to take corrective action. Even if
discussed the adverse effects of a rigid curriculum it were possible for teachers to accommodate ev-
on students' self-images (ASCD Yearbook, 1962). ery student's limitation at any point during the
During the late 1970s and early 1980s, a new school day, their assistance could undermine the
perspective on students' individual differences be- most important aspect of this learning-a student's
gan to emerge from research on metacognition and development of a capability to self-regulate.
social cognition. Metacognition is defined as the
awareness of and knowledge about one's own Defining Self-Regulated Learning
thinking. Students' deficiencies in learning were in Process Terms
attributed to a lack of metacognitive awareness of Self-regulation is not a mental ability or an
personal limitations and an inability to compen- academic performance skill; rather it is the self-
sate. Social cognitive researchers were interested directive process by which learners transform their
in social influences on children's development of mental abilities into academic skills. Learning is
self-regulation, and they studied issues such as the viewed as an activity that students do for them-
effects of teacher modeling and instruction on stu- selves in a proactive way rather than as a covert
dents' goal setting and self-monitoring (Schunk, event that happens to them in reaction to teaching.
1989; Zimmerman, 1989). Students were asked to Self-regulation refers to self-generated thoughts,
set particular types of goals for themselves, such feelings, and behaviors that are oriented to attain-
as completing of a certain number of math home- ing goals (Zimmerman, 2000). These learners are
work problems, and to self-recordtheir effectiveness proactive in their efforts to learn because they are

65
/ Spring 2002
INTOPRACTICE
THEORY
Becominga Self-RegulatedLearner

aware of their strengths and limitations and be- trait that individual students either possess or lack.
cause they are guided by personally set goals and Instead, it involves the selective use of specific
task-related strategies, such as using an arithmetic processes that must be personally adapted to each
addition strategy to check the accuracy of solu- learning task. The component skills include: (a)
tions to subtraction problems. These learners mon- setting specific proximal goals for oneself, (b)
itor their behavior in terms of their goals and adopting powerful strategies for attaining the goals,
self-reflect on their increasing effectiveness. This (c) monitoring one's performance selectively for
enhances their self-satisfaction and motivation to signs of progress, (d) restructuring one's physical
continue to improve their methods of learning. and social context to make it compatible with one's
Because of their superior motivation and adaptive goals, (e) managing one's time use efficiently, (f)
learning methods, self-regulated students are not self-evaluating one's methods, (g) attributing cau-
only more likely to succeed academically but to sation to results, and (h) adapting future methods.
view their futures optimistically. A students' level of learning has been found to
Self-regulation is important because a major vary based on the presence or absence of these key
function of education is the development of life- self-regulatory processes (Schunk & Zimmerman,
long learning skills. After graduation from high 1994; 1998).
school or college, young adults must learn many Third, contemporaryresearch reveals that the
important skills informally. For example, in busi- self-motivated quality of self-regulated learners
ness settings, they are often expected to learn a depends on several underlying beliefs, including
new position, such as selling a product, by observ- perceived efficacy and intrinsic interest. Histori-
ing proficient others and by practicing on their own. cally, educators have focused on social encourage-
Those who develop high levels of skill position ment and extrinsic "bells and whistles" to try to
themselves for bonuses, early promotion, or more elevate students' level of motivation. Unfortunately,
attractive jobs. In self-employment settings, both self-directed studying or practicing was often derid-
young and old must constantly self-refine their ed as inherently boring, repetitive, and mind numb-
skills in order to survive. Their capability to self- ing with catchy phrases such as "Drill and kill."
regulate is especially challenged when they under- However, interviews with experts reveal a very dif-
take long-term creative projects, such as works of ferent picture of these experiences (Ericsson &
art, literary texts, or inventions. In recreational set- Charness, 1994). Experts spend approximately four
tings, learners spend much personally regulated hours each day in study and practice and find these
time learning diverse skills for self-entertainment, activities highly motivating. They vary their meth-
ranging from hobbies to sports. ods of study and practice in order to discover new
Although the relationship of self-reliance to strategies for self-improvement. With such diverse
success in life has been widely recognized, most stu- skills as chess, sports, and music, the quantityof an
dents struggle to attain self-discipline in their meth- individual's studying and practicing is a strong pre-
ods of study today as they did a century ago. What dictor of his or her level of expertise. There is also
does contemporaryresearch tell us about this desir- evidence that the quality of practicing and study-
able but elusive personal quality? First, self-regula- ing episodes is highly predictive of a learner's level
tion of learning involves more than detailed of skill (Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1997; 1999).
knowledge of a skill; it involves the self-awareness, However, few beginners in a new discipline
self-motivation,and behavioralskill to implementthat immediately derive powerful self-motivational ben-
knowledge appropriately.For example, there is evi- efits, and they may easily lose interest if they are
dence (Cleary & Zimmerman,2000) that experts dif- not socially encouraged and guided, as most music
fer from non-expertsin theirapplicationof knowledge teachers will readily attest (McPherson & Zimmer-
at crucial times during learning performances, such man, in press). Fortunately,the motivation of novic-
as correcting specific deficiencies in technique. es can be greatly enhanced when and if they use
Second, contemporary research tells us that high-quality self-regulatoryprocesses, such as close
self-regulation of learning is not a single personal self-monitoring. Students who have the capabilities

66
Zimmerman
Becoming a Self-Regulated Learner

to detect subtle progress in learning will increase learning psychologists view the structure of self-
their levels of self-satisfaction and their beliefs in regulatory processes in terms of three cyclical phas-
their personal efficacy to perform at a high level es. The forethought phase refers to processes and
of skill (Schunk, 1983). Clearly, their motivation beliefs that occur before efforts to learn; the per-
does not stem from the task itself, but rather from formance phase refers to processes that occur dur-
their use of self-regulatory processes, such as self- ing behavioral implementation, and self-reflection
monitoring, and the effects of these processes on refers to processes that occur after each learning
their self-beliefs. effort. The processes that have been studied in each
phase to date are shown in Figure 1, and the func-
Structure and Function of tion of each process will be described next (Zim-
Self-Regulatory Processes merman, 2000).
This brings us to the essential question of how
does a student's use of specific learning processes, Forethought phase
level of self-awareness, and motivational beliefs There are two major classes of forethought
combine to produce self-regulated learners? Social phase processes: task analysis and self-motivation.

iii

Performance Phase
Self-Co, ntrol
Image:ry
Self-instruction
Attention f ocusing
Task strategies

Self-Obse rvation
Self-reco>rding
Self-experinnentation ?l
ssnassscsss 1
-v
..
. 1
l

.! Forethought Phase I Self-Reflection Phase I


TaskAnalysis
0I
Self-Judgment
Goal setting Self-evaluation
Strategicplanning Causal attribution

Self-Motivation Beliefs Self-Reaction


Self-Reaction
Self-efficacy efficacy
Sel.l f-I~ I Self-satisfaction/affect
Outcome expectations Adaptivedefensive
Intrinsicinterest/value
Learninggoal orientation

Figure 1. Phases and Subprocesses of Self-Regulation. From B.J. Zimmerman and M. Campillo (in press), "Motivating
Self-Regulated Problem Solvers." In J.E. Davidson and Robert Sternberg (Eds.), The Nature of Problem Solving. New
York: Cambridge University Press. Adapted with permission.

67
THEORYINTO PRACTICE/ Spring 2002
Becominga Self-RegulatedLearner

Task analysis involves goal setting and strategic the boy could conduct a self-experiment in which
planning. There is considerable evidence of in- he studied parallel lessons alone and in the pres-
creased academic success by learners who set spe- ence of his friend to see whether his friend was an
cific proximal goals for themselves, such as asset or a liability. Self-monitoring,a covert form of
memorizing a word list for a spelling test, and by self-observation,refers to one's cognitive trackingof
learners who plan to use spelling strategies, such personal functioning, such as the frequency of fail-
as segmenting words into syllables. ing to capitalize words when writing an essay.
Self-motivation stems from students' beliefs
about learning, such as self-efficacy beliefs about Self-reflection phase
having the personal capability to learn and out- There are two major classes of self-reflec-
come expectations about personal consequences of tion phase processes: self-judgment and self-reac-
learning (Bandura, 1997). For example, students tion. One form of self-judgment, self-evaluation,
who feel self-efficacious about learning to divide refers to comparisons of self-observed performanc-
fractions and expect to use this knowledge to pass es against some standard, such as one's prior per-
a college entranceexam are more motivated to learn formance, another person's performance, or an
in a self-regulated fashion. Intrinsic interest refers absolute standard of performance. Another form
to the students' valuing of the task skill for its of self-judgment involves causal attribution, which
own merits, and learning goal orientation refers to refers to beliefs about the cause of one's errors or
valuing the process of learning for its own merits. successes, such as a score on a mathematics test.
Students who find the subject matter of history, Attributing a poor score to limitations in fixed abil-
for example, interesting and enjoy increasing their ity can be very damaging motivationally because
mastery of it are more motivated to learn in a self- it implies that efforts to improve on a future test
regulated fashion. will not be effective. In contrast, attributinga poor
math score to controllable processes, such as the
Performance phase use of the wrong solution strategy, will sustain mo-
Performance phase processes fall into two tivation because it implies that a different strategy
major classes: self-control and self-observation. may lead to success.
Self-control refers to the deployment of specific One form of self-reaction involves feelings
methods or strategies that were selected during the of self-satisfaction and positive affect regarding
forethought phase. Among the key types of self- one's performance. Increases in self-satisfaction
control methods that have been studied to date are enhance motivation, whereas decreases in self-sat-
the use of imagery, self-instruction, attentionfocus- isfaction underminefurtherefforts to learn (Schunk,
ing, and task strategies. For example, in learningthe 2001). Self-reactions also take the form of adap-
Spanish word pan for "bread,"an English-speaking tive/defensive responses. Defensive reactions refer
girl could form an image of a bread pan or self- to efforts to protect one's self-image by withdraw-
instructusing the phrase "breadpan." She could also ing or avoiding opportunities to learn and perform,
locate her place of study away from distractingnois- such as dropping a course or being absent for a
es so she could control her attention better. For a test. In contrast, adaptive reactions refer to adjust-
task-strategy,she could group the Spanish word pan ments designed to increase the effectiveness of
with associated words for foods. one's method of learning, such as discarding or
Self-observation refers to self-recording per- modifying an ineffective learning strategy.
sonal events or self-experimentation to find out This view of self-regulation is cyclical in that
the cause of these events. For example, students self-reflections from prior efforts to learn affect sub-
are often asked to self-record their time use to sequent forethoughtprocesses (e.g., self-dissatisfac-
make them aware of how much time they spend tion will lead to lower levels of self-efficacy and
studying. A boy may notice that when he studied diminished effort during subsequentlearning) (Zim-
alone, he finished his homework more quickly than merman & Bandura, 1994). In supportof this cycli-
when studying with a friend. To test this hypothesis, cal view of self-regulation, high correlations were

68
Zimmerman
Becoming a Self-Regulated Learner

found among learners' use of forethought, perfor- intervention programs in schools for children who
mance, and self-reflection phase processes (Zimmer- display lower levels of self-regulatory development
man & Kitsantas, 1999). For example, students who (Schunk & Zimmerman, 1998).
set specific proximal goals are more likely to self-
observe their performancein theses areas, more like- Teaching Students to Become
ly to achieve in the targetarea,and will display higher Self-Regulated Learners
levels of self-efficacy than students who do not set Research on the quality and quantity of stu-
goals (Bandura& Schunk, 1981). Otherstudies have dents' use of self-regulatory processes has revealed
revealed that experts display significantly higher lev- high correlations with academic achievement track
els of self-regulatory processes during practice ef- placement as well as with performance on stan-
forts than novices (Cleary & Zimmerman,2000). dardized test scores (Zimmerman& Martinez-Pons,
The self-regulation profile of novices is very 1986). There is also evidence that students' use of
distinctive from that of experts. Novices fail to en- self-regulatory processes is distinctive from but
gage in high-quality forethoughtand instead attempt correlated with general measures of ability, such
to self-regulatetheir learningreactively. That is, they as verbal ability (Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994).
fail to set specific goals or to self-monitor systemati- Although many self-regulatory processes, such as
cally, and as a result, they tend to rely on compari- goal setting and self-monitoring, are generally co-
sons with the performance of others to judge their vert, teachers are aware of many overt manifesta-
learning effectiveness. Because typically other learn- tions of these processes, such as students'
ers are also progressing,their performancerepresents self-awareness of the quality of their work and pre-
a constantly increasing criterion of success that is paredness in class (Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons,
very difficult to surpass. Furthermore,learners who 1988). Recent research shows that self-regulatory
make comparative self-evaluations are prompted to processes are teachable and can lead to increases
attributecausation to ability deficiencies (which are in students' motivation and achievement (Schunk
also normativein nature),and this will produce low- & Zimmerman, 1998).
er personal satisfaction and prompt defensive reac- Although research findings strongly support
tions. In contrast, the self-regulation profile of the importance of students' use of self-regulatory
experts reveals they display high levels of self- processes, few teachers effectively prepare students
motivation and set hierarchicalgoals for themselves to learn on their own (Zimmerman, Bonner, &
with process goals leading to outcome goals in suc- Kovach, 1996). Students are seldom given choices
cession, such as dividing a formal essay into an regarding academic tasks to pursue, methods for
introduction,a body, and a conclusion. Experts plan carrying out complex assignments, or study part-
learning efforts using powerful strategies and self- ners. Few teachers encourage students to establish
observe their effects, such as a visual organizer for specific goals for their academic work or teach
filling in key information (Zimmerman & Risem- explicit study strategies. Also, students are rarely
berg, 1997). They self-evaluate their performance asked to self-evaluate their work or estimate their
against their personal goals ratherthan other learn- competence on new tasks. Teachers seldom assess
ers' performance, and they make strategy (or meth- students' beliefs about learning, such as self-effi-
od) attributions instead of ability attributions. This cacy perceptions or causal attributions, in order to
leads to greater personal satisfactionwith theirlearn- identify cognitive or motivational difficulties be-
ing progress and furtherefforts to improve their per- fore they become problematic.
formance. Together these self-reactions enhance Contraryto a commonly held belief, self-reg-
various self-motivational beliefs of experts, such ulated learning is not asocial in nature and origin.
as self-efficacy, outcome expectations, learning Each self-regulatory process or belief, such as goal
goal orientation, and intrinsic interest. setting, strategy use, and self-evaluation, can be
Knowing the differences in the structureand learned from instruction and modeling by parents,
function of self-regulatoryprocesses between experts teachers, coaches, and peers. In fact, self-regulat-
and novices has enabled researchers to formulate ed students seek out help from others to improve

69
THEORYINTO PRACTICE/ Spring 2002
Becoming a Self-Regulated Learner

their learning. What defines them as "self-regulated" Schunk, D.H., & Zimmerman, B.J. (Eds.). (1994). Self-
is not their reliance on socially isolated methods of regulation of learning and performance: Issues and
educational applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
learning, but rather their personal initiative, perse- Schunk, D.H., & Zimmerman, B.J. (Eds.). (1998). Self-
verance, and adoptive skill. Self-regulated students regulated learning: From teaching to self-reflec-
focus on how they activate, alter, and sustain specific tive practice. New York: Guilford Press.
learning practices in social as well as solitary con- Shapiro, E.S. (1984). Self-monitoring procedures. In
texts. In an era when these essential qualities for life- T.H. Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.), Child behav-
ior assessment: Principles and procedures (pp.
long learning are distressingly absent in many
148-165). New York: Pergamon.
students, teaching self-regulated learning processes
Zimmerman, B.J. (1989). A social cognitive view of
is especially relevant. self-regulated academic learning. Journal of Edu-
cational Psychology, 81, 329-339.
Note Zimmerman, B.J. (2000). Attainment of self-regulation:
1. Correspondence concerning this article should be di- A social cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts,
rected to Barry J. Zimmerman, Ph.D. Program in P.R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of
Educational Psychology, Graduate School and Uni- self-regulation (pp. 13-39). San Diego, CA: Aca-
versity Center of the City University of New York, demic Press.
365 Fifth Ave., New York, NY 10016-4309 or Zimmerman, B.J. (2001). Theories of self-regulated
bzimmerman @gc.cuny.edu. learning and academic achievement: An overview
and analysis. In B.J. Zimmerman & D.H. Schunk
References (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic
achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed., pp.
ASCD Yearbook. (1962). Perceiving behaving becom-
1-37). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ing. Washington DC: Association for Supervision Zimmerman,B.J., & Bandura,A. (1994). Impact of self-
and Curriculum Development.
regulatory influences on writing course attainment.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of con- AmericanEducationalResearchJournal, 31, 845-862.
trol. New York: W.H. Freeman.
Zimmerman, B.J., Bonner, S., & Kovach, R. (1996).
Bandura, A., & Schunk, D.H. (1981). Cultivating com- learners: Beyond
Developing self-regulated
petence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through achievement to self-efficacy. Washington, DC:
proximal self-motivation. Journal of Personality American Psychological Association.
and Social Psychology, 41, 586-598.
Zimmerman, B.J., & Campillo, M. (in press). Motivating
Cleary, T., & Zimmerman, B.J. (2000). Self-regulation
differences during athletic practice by experts, non- self-regulated problem solvers. In J.E. Davidson &
R. Sternberg (Eds.), The nature of problem solving.
experts, and novices. Journal of Applied Sport Psy- New York: Cambridge University Press.
chology, 13, 61-82.
Cronbach, L.J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific Zimmerman, B.J., & Kitsantas, A. (1997). Developmen-
tal phases in self-regulation: Shifting from process
psychology. American Psychologist, 12, 671-684. to outcome goals. Journal of Educational Psycholo-
Ericsson, A.K., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert perfor-
mance: Its structure and acquisition. American gy, 89, 29-36.
Psychologist, 49, 725-747. Zimmerman, B.J., & Kitsantas, A. (1999). Acquiring
McPherson, G.E., & Zimmerman, B.J. (in press). Self- writing revision skill: Shifting from process to
outcome self-regulatory goals. Journal of Educa-
regulation of musical learning: A social cognitive
perspective. In R. Colwell (Ed.), Second handbook tional Psychology, 91, 1-10.
on music teaching and learning. New York: Ox- Zimmerman,B.J., & Martinez-Pons,M. (1986). Develop-
ford University Press. ment of a structuredinterview for assessing students'
Schunk, D.H. (1983). Progress self-monitoring: Effects use of self-regulatedlearningstrategies.AmericanEd-
on children's self-efficacy and achievement. Jour- ucational Research Journal, 23, 614-628.
nal of Experimental Education, 51, 89-93. Zimmerman, B.J., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1988). Con-
Schunk, D.H. (1989). Social cognitive theory and self- struct validation of a strategy model of student
regulated learning. In B.J. Zimmerman & D.H self-regulated learning. Journal of Educational
Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and aca- Psychology, 80, 284-290.
demic achievement: Theory, research, and prac- Zimmerman, B.J., & Risemberg, R. (1997). Becoming a
tice (pp. 83-110). New York: Springer Verlag. self-regulated writer: A social cognitive perspective.
Schunk, D.H. (2001). Social cognitive theory and self- ContemporaryEducational Psychology, 22, 73-101.
regulated learning. In B.J. Zimmerman & D.H Zimmerman, B.J., & Schunk, D.H. (Eds.). (2001). Self-
Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and aca- regulated learning and academic achievement: The-
demic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd oretical perspectives (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ:
ed., pp. 125-152). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Erlbaum.

70

View publication stats

You might also like