You are on page 1of 14

Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34

www.elsevier.com/locate/pateducou

Review

Cultural differences in medical communication: A review of


the literature
Barbara C. Schouten *, Ludwien Meeuwesen
Interdisciplinary Social Science Department, Research Institute for Psychology and Health, Utrecht University,
Utrecht, The Netherlands

Received 22 June 2005; received in revised form 9 November 2005; accepted 19 November 2005

Abstract

Objective: Culture and ethnicity have often been cited as barriers in establishing an effective and satisfying doctor–patient relationship. The
aim of this paper is to gain more insight in intercultural medical communication difficulties by reviewing observational studies on intercultural
doctor–patient communication. In addition, a research model for studying this topic in future research is proposed.
Methods: A literature review using online databases (Pubmed, Psychlit) was performed.
Results: Findings reveal major differences in doctor–patient communication as a consequence of patients’ ethnic backgrounds. Doctors
behave less affectively when interacting with ethnic minority patients compared to White patients. Ethnic minority patients themselves are
also less verbally expressive; they seem to be less assertive and affective during the medical encounter than White patients.
Conclusion: Most reviewed studies did not relate communication behaviour to possible antecedent culture-related variables, nor did they
assess the effect of cultural variations in doctor–patient communication on outcomes, leaving us in the dark about reasons for and
consequences of differences in intercultural medical communication. Five key predictors of culture-related communication problems are
identified in the literature: (1) cultural differences in explanatory models of health and illness; (2) differences in cultural values; (3) cultural
differences in patients’ preferences for doctor–patient relationships; (4) racism/perceptual biases; (5) linguistic barriers. It is concluded that by
incorporating these variables into a research model future research on this topic can be enhanced, both from a theoretical and a methodological
perspective.
Practice implications: Using a cultural sensitive approach in medical communication is recommended.
# 2005 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Intercultural communication; Doctor–patient communication; Medical communication; Ethnicity; Review

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22
2. Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22
3. Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
3.1. Study design and methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
3.2. Research questions and findings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
3.2.1. Doctors’ communicative behaviour . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
3.2.2. Patients’ communicative behaviour . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
3.2.3. Culture’s influence on doctor–patient communication: antecedents and outcomes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
4. Discussion and conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28

* Corresponding author at: Interdisciplinary Social Science Department, Utrecht University, P.O. Box 80140, 3508 TC, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Tel.: +31 30 2537786; fax: +31 30 2534733.
E-mail address: b.c.schouten@fss.uu.nl (B.C. Schouten).

0738-3991/$ – see front matter # 2005 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.pec.2005.11.014
22 B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34

4.1. Proposal of research model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29


4.1.1. Cognitive processes: Kleinman’s explanatory model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29
4.1.2. Cultural values: individualism–collectivism and self-construals . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30
4.1.3. Motivational processes: patient preferences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30
4.1.4. Perceptions: racism/bias . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
4.1.5. Language: linguistic barriers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
4.2. Study limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
4.3. Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32
4.4. Practice implications. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32

1. Introduction have shown that there are considerable disparities in access


to care as well as in health outcomes as a consequence of
A recent issue in the study of doctor–patient interaction is patients’ ethnic background. For example, ethnic minority
the relation between patients’ ethnic background and patients are less likely to be recommended for certain
medical communication, and the fundamental question this treatments than White patients [13,14]. Although these
raises to what extent belonging to an ethnic/cultural group disparities in health (care) are probably partly related to
influences the communication process between patients and socio-economic variables such as income, gaps in doctor–
health care practitioners. To pose this question is of vital patient communication are likely to play a crucial role as
importance, because in today’s multicultural society health well, since it is well known that this factor is positively
care practitioners are increasingly confronted with patients associated with various health-related outcomes [15].
from different cultural and ethnic backgrounds. For instance, Hence, possible gaps in intercultural medical communica-
in The Netherlands more than 18% of the population is from tion seem to place ethnic minority patients at an increased
other ethnic origin nowadays [1]. Encounters with these risk of receiving inferior care.
patients are likely to differ from meetings between doctors The overall aim of the present study is to gain more
and patients sharing the same cultural or ethnic background, insight into the effects of patients’ and doctors’ cultural/
because people from different cultures hold different beliefs ethnic backgrounds on the medical communication process.
about health, illness and communication [2,3]. These Main research question is whether there are any differences
divergent beliefs, as well as linguistic barriers that often in doctors’ and patients’ communicative behaviour between
exist between members of different cultures, confront health intercultural and intra-cultural consultations, and if so,
care practitioners with the difficult task to deliver good which differences. A second question concerns how these
quality care to a wide diversity of patients, each bringing his differences may be explained and what consequences they
own unique background to the medical encounter. have for patient outcomes, such as patient satisfaction,
Culture1 and ethnicity have often been cited as barriers in compliance and understanding. Because our focus in this
establishing an effective and satisfying doctor–patient review is on communicative behaviour, we will investigate
relationship [4–6]. For example, results of a number of the first research question by reviewing observational studies
survey studies indicate that there is more misunderstanding, on intercultural medical communication as this method
less compliance and less satisfaction in intercultural medical yields the most reliable data with regard to assessing
consultations compared to intra-cultural medical consulta- behaviour. However, with respect to the second research
tions,2 even after adjusting for socio-economic variables question, the broader literature on intercultural health
such as education and income [7–10]. Moreover, health care communication (including other research methods as
providers find consultations with ethnic minority patients surveys, focus groups and so on) will be included as this
often emotionally demanding and patients’ reasons for may add relevant findings to the results of our observational
visiting unclear [11,12]. Furthermore, numerous studies review. Combined with our review results, in the second part
of this article these findings will be used to design a research
1
Culture has been defined in many ways. It is beyond the scope of this model that can be used in future research on this topic.
article to go into this topic in much detail. Important to note here is that we
assume that culture guides communicative behaviour. Furthermore,
although much empirical work seems to equate culture with ethnicity,
we do not equate these two concepts. On the contrary, one of the main
2. Methods
points of this article is that future research should cease to equate culture
with ethnicity, as within one ethnic group various cultures may exist, and We performed a literature review using online databases
ethnicity is but one element of culture. (Pubmed, Psychlit), and searched for further eligible literature
2
In the current article, intercultural medical consultations are defined as through references in scientific papers and books. The
consultations between health care practitioners and patients having different
ethnic and/or cultural backgrounds, intra-cultural medical consultations as following key words were used, in different combinations:
consultations between health care practitioners and patients sharing the doctor–patient communication, physician–patient commu-
same ethnic and/or cultural background. nication, culture, ethnicity, race, diversity, non-English
B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34 23

speaking, intercultural communication, cross-cultural com- ranged from five to nine, and for the quantitative studies
munication, medicine, language barriers, medical consu- from 51 to 458. With regard to the observational strategy,
ltation, medical communication and medical interview. eight studies were based on audiotapes, four on videotapes,
Publications were included if they met the following criteria: and two study directly assessed doctor–patient communica-
tion. Four studies applied the Roter Interaction Analysis
1. The study was directed at the verbal and/or non-verbal System (RIAS) [30] to code the verbal behaviour of
communication between doctors and patients, with participants. This category system distinguishes between
patients and doctors having dissimilar cultural/ethnic instrumental utterances, such as information-giving and
backgrounds. question-asking, and affective utterances, such as showing
2. The study involved either audio or video recordings of empathy and giving reassurance. Henbest and Stewart’s
the consultation or direct scoring of doctor–patient patient-centeredness measure was used in the study by
communication during consultations. Rivadeneyra et al. [20]. This system assesses the extent to
3. The study was published in English from 1974 to 2004. which a physician responds to patients’ offers, that is, ‘any
topic or question introduced by the patient during the
The literature search produced 14 articles meeting the medical encounter that was not a direct answer to a
inclusion criteria. These articles examined the impact of physician’s question’ [20]. The other six quantitative studies
patients’ culture/ethnicity on doctor–patient communication made use of self-developed analysis systems; all of these
and formed the basis of the current review study. The studies were category systems coding (frequencies of) various
were evaluated in terms of their methods, research questions aspects of participants’ verbal behaviour. Just one study
and findings, and whether variations in doctor–patient assessed non-verbal behaviour [17]. Erzinger [18] made use
communication were related to antecedent factors and of conversation-analytical micro-analysis, Cass et al. [22]
outcome measures. did not mention their observational instrument.
Nine of the quantitative studies mentioned inter-rater
reliabilities, which in general were satisfactory (except for
3. Results one study). Finally, about half of the studies mentioned
sampling method and response rates.
3.1. Study design and methods
3.2. Research questions and findings
Table 1 contains an overview of the study design and
methods used; 12 studies were quantitative and two The aim of all the studies was to assess the effect of
studies qualitative in nature. Most studies were carried out patients’ ethnic background on the medical communication
in the United States during the last decade within the process (Table 2). To realise this objective, most studies
setting of a family practice or general practice. Parti- compared the communication process between White
cipating physicians were mostly residents in their second doctors and White patients with the communication
or third year of medical training (in the US) or general process between White doctors and ethnic minority
practitioners. patients. Both doctors’ and patients’ communicative
Patients’ age and sex was reported in the quantitative behaviour was investigated, and in most cases encounter
studies and mostly their educational level too, whereas length was assessed too. We will first discuss findings
patients’ language proficiency was only reported twice related to doctors’ communicative behaviour, then turn to
[19,25]. The reason of visit, patients’ perceived health, the findings on patients’ communicative behaviour. Finally, we
extent to which doctor and patient knew each other and will present results on possible factors related to
patients’ religious background were infrequently mentioned. differences in and consequences of (gaps in) intercultural
Age and sex of the physician were reported in most communication.
quantitative studies.
The studies most frequently involved a comparison of 3.2.1. Doctors’ communicative behaviour
doctor–patient communication between visits of patients In general, all studies found significant differences in
belonging to one of the major ethnic minority groups, White doctors’ behaviour when comparing consultations with
physicians on the one hand, and visits of White patients to White patients and ethnic minority patients. These
White doctors on the other. In the American studies ethnic differences concerned doctors’ affective and instrumental
minority patients were African, Americans and Hispanics; in verbal behaviour as well as consultation length. Most
the Dutch studies they were mainly of Surinamese, studies found that physicians showed less affective
Antillean, Turkish or Moroccan background, and in the behaviour when communicating with ethnic minority
Australian study they were Aboriginals. patients [16,17,23,24,28,29], but other results indicated no
Except for the study by Cooper et al. [28], none of the differences or, on the contrary, found that doctors were
studies explicitly reported matching patients’ and physi- more affective towards ethnic minorities [25]. Three studies
cians’ ethnicity. The sample size of the qualitative studies [16,17,28] showed significantly decreased empathy with
24
Table 1
Overview of studies on intercultural doctor–patient communication: setting, sample and method
Study Country Practice Patient sample a Doctor sample Observational strategy Observational instrument Inter-rater reliability
Shapiro and Saltzer [16] USA Resident clinic 39 WA, 15 LES-Hispanics, 10 WA family practice Audiotapes Self-developed Range: 0.50–0.61
quantitative 7 ES-Hispanics (n = 61) resident physicians interaction analysis (correlation)
(n = 10) instrument
Hooper et al. [17] USA Outpatient clinic of 67 WA, 74 Hispanics 15 WA resident Direct ratings of Self-developed 94% agreement
quantitative teaching hospital (n = 150) physicians (n = 15) interaction interaction
through one-way analysis instrument
mirror
NR b

B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34


Erzinger [18] USA Family practice 9 Hispanics (n = 9) Not reported Audiotapes, Conversation
qualitative residency at transcripts analysis
public hospital
Seijo et al. [19] USA Internal medicine clinic 51 Hispanics (n = 51) 5 WA internal Direct ratings of Self-developed NR
quantitative medicine physicians, interaction interaction
4 Latin American analysis instrument
physicians
Rivadeneyra et al. [20] USA Primary care clinic 15 WA, 19 LES-Latinos, 3 WA, 4 Asian, Videotapes Henbest & Stewart’s 0.96 (correlation)
quantitative affiliated 4 ES-Latinos (n = 38) 1 Middle Eastern Patient-Centeredness
with university primary care (resident) Measure
physicians (n = 8)
Sleath et al. [21] USA Family practices and 153 WA, 254 Hispanics 19 WA, 6 Hispanic, Audiotapes, Self-developed Range: 0.80–0.93
quantitative general internal (n = 407) 2 Asian family transcripts interaction (Cronbach’s alpha)
medicine clinics practice analysis instrument
at university or general internal
medicine resident
physicians (n = 27)
Cass et al. [22] Australia Satellite dialysis unit 5 Aboriginal (n = 5) 5 White Australian Videotapes NR NR
qualitative physicians (n = 5)
van Wieringen The Netherlands General practices 48 Ethnic minority 7 Dutch, 1 Aruban Videotapes Roter interaction NR
et al. [23] (mostly Surinamese/ general practitioners analysis system
quantitative Turkish/Moroccan), (n = 8)
38 Dutch (n = 87)
Sleath and Rubin [24] USA Family practices and 141 WA, 242 Hispanic 19 WA, 6 Hispanic, Audiotapes, Self-developed Range: 0.61–0.87
quantitative general internal (n = 383) 2 Asian family transcripts interaction (correlation)
medicine clinics at practice or general analysis instrument
university internal medicine
resident physicians
(n = 27)
Harmsen [25] Netherlands Family practices 34 Ethnic minority 7 Dutch general Videotapes Roter Interaction Range: 0.55–0.79
quantitative (mostly Surinamese/ practitioners (n = 7) Analysis System (correlation)
Turkish/Moroccan),
32 Dutch (n = 66); children
B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34 25

average doctor: 0.90,

average doctor: 0.88,


average patient: 0.86

average patient: 0.79


ethnic minority patients compared to White patients.

Range: 0.45–1.00

Range: 0.06–1.00
Range: 0.70-0.98

Harmsen [25] found that doctors expressed more empathy

(correlation);

(correlation);
(correlation)

with ethnic minority patients during pediatric consulta-


tions, and Sleath et al. [21] could not detect significant
differences in physicians’ expressions of empathy when
NR

comparing Hispanic with White American patients. Other


affective behavioural variables assessed in the studies were
the extent of rapport-building [16], physicians’ respon-
analysis instrument

analysis instrument

siveness towards patients’ offers [20], physicians’ expres-


Roter interaction

Roter interaction
analysis system

analysis system
Self-developed

Self-developed

siveness of positiveness [21], and affective verbal


interaction

interaction

behaviour as categorised by RIAS, such as physicians’


social talk and partnership-building [23,28,29]. All studies
reported significantly lower scores on these variables in
consultations with ethnic minority patients compared to
White patients: there was less social talk and rapport-
building, doctors were rated as less friendly and concerned,
and patients’ comments were ignored by their doctors
Audiotapes,

Audiotapes,

Audiotapes

Audiotapes
transcripts

transcripts

more often.
With regard to instrumental verbal behaviour, physi-
cians were rated higher on giving medical explanations
[16] and on interviewing skills (using open-ended
30 WA, 21 AA, 9 Asian/

questions and allowing patients to ask questions) when


care physicians (n = 31)
13 WA, 18 AA primary
2 Asian family practice

1 other primary care

communicating with Anglo-American patients compared


19 WA, 6 Hispanic,

19 WA, 6 Hispanic
physicians (n = 27)

physicians (n = 61)
resident physicians
practice or general
or general internal

Indian Americans,
medicine resident

internal medicine

to Hispanic patients [17]. Sleath et al. [26] found that


doctors communicated more about antidepressants use
with White patients than with Hispanics. At the same
(n = 25)
family

time, they asked more questions about anxiety in


WA: White American; LES: limited English-speaking; ES: English-speaking; AA: African-American.

consultations with Hispanics than with White patients


[24]. Four studies reported no significant differences with
regard to doctors’ instrumental verbal behaviour
177 WA, 226 Hispanics

[20,23,25,28].
43 WA, 44 Hispanics

110 WA, 142 AA

202 WA, 256 AA

Results on consultation length were inconsistent. Johnson


et al. [29] and Hooper et al. [17] did not find any difference
in consultation length between visits of Anglo-American
(n = 403)

(n = 252)

(n = 458)
(n = 98)

and Spanish-American patients, while Harmsen [25]


reported that consultations with ethnic minority patients
were on average 3 min longer than consultations with White
patients. In contrast, Cooper et al. [28] found that race-
Primary care practices
Family practices and

Family practices and

concordant visits were significantly longer than race-


Family practices
medicine clinics

medicine clinics
general internal

general internal

discordant visits (about 2 min), and also had lower speech


health centres
at university

at university

and federal

speed.
Erzinger [18] identified various ‘communicative tasks’
for both doctors and patients, which should be completed
for a medical encounter to be successful. Communication
styles were either described as supportive or conflictual,
depending on whether participants assisted each other in
completing the communicative tasks. Both communica-
USA

USA

USA

USA

tion styles occurred in these medical encounters,


suggesting that some consultations with ethnic minority
patients were going smoothly while others were not. Cass
NR: not reported.

et al. [22] found that a shared understanding between


Johnson et al. [29]

doctors and Aboriginal patients was rarely achieved.


Cooper et al. [28]
Sleath et al. [26]

Sleath et al. [27]

Patients had few opportunities to initiate a topic; the


quantitative

quantitative

quantitative

quantitative

doctor determined the conversational direction and often


patients acted politely, which health care practitioners
b
a

were seldom aware of.


26 B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34

Table 2
Overview of predictors, research questions and findings in intercultural studies
Study Research questions Main findings
Shapiro and Comparison of relationship-building and quality Less rapport-building and empathy with Hispanics than with
Saltzer [16] of doctors’ explanations between Hispanic and non-Hispanics, even in the case of English-speaking Hispanics
non-Hispanic patients
Effect of doctor–patient communication on Relationship between language and ethnicity & patient understanding
patient understanding and compliance
No differences between ethnic groups on compliance
Hooper et al. Effect of patient characteristics on physicians’ Better interviewing skills and more empathy with Anglo-American
[17] information conveyance, interviewing skills, than with Spanish-American patients
empathy, nonverbal attention, courtesy and
consultation length
No other differences observed
Erzinger [18] Exploration of communication problems of Four patient tasks: describing concerns, clarifying doctor’s information,
Spanish-speaking patients in the USA obtaining an adequate explanation and developing a personal relationship
Four doctor tasks: exploring symptoms, interpreting data, explaining and
advising, understanding patient’s personal situation
Overall success of medical encounter is determined by how doctor and
patient assist each other in completing the communicative tasks
Two dominant communication styles occurred: supportive and conflictual
Seijo et al. [19] Comparison of doctor–patient communication No difference between language-concordant and language-discordant
(i.e. patients’ question-asking and doctors’ consultations on amount of doctors’ information-giving behaviour
information-giving) between language-concordant
and language discordant visits
Differences in patient recall between language- More patient question-asking in language-concordant than in
concordant and language discordant visits language-discordant consultations
More patient recall in language-concordant than in language-
discordant consultations
Rivadeneyra Comparison of frequency of physicians’ English-speaking patients received more responses than
et al. [20] responses to patients’ ‘offers’ between Spanish-speaking patients
English-speaking and Spanish-speaking patients
No difference in frequency of physicians’ responses between
English-speaking Latino patients and White English-speaking patients
Sleath Comparison of physicians’ expression of No differences in expression of empathy between
et al. [21] empathy and positiveness between Hispanic Hispanic and White patients
and White patients
More expression of positiveness to White patients than to Hispanic patients
Cass Exploration of the extent of miscommunication Shared understanding between doctors and patients is rarely achieved;
et al. [22] between Australian doctors and serious miscommunication regarding diagnosis,
Aboriginal patients treatment and prevention
van Wieringen Effect of communication and patient beliefs on Less mutual understanding in consultations with ethnic minority patients
et al. [23] understanding and compliance of White and than in consultations with White patients
ethnic minority patients
Differences in patient beliefs between ethnic minority
patients and White patients
Consultations with ethnic minority patients show less social talk,
less doctors’ concern and less doctors’ friendliness than
consultations with White patients
No relation between communication and mutual understanding/compliance
Sleath and Effect of patients’ ethnicity on physician–patient No effect of patients’ ethnicity on physician–patient
Rubin [24] communication about depression and anxiety communication about depression
Physicians were more likely to ask Hispanic patients questions
about anxiety than White patients
Harmsen [25] Comparing doctor–patient communication More empathy with ethnic minority than with White patients
between ethnic minority and White patients
Effect of education and language proficiency Ethnic minority patients expressed less interest and concern than
on doctor–patient communication White patients
Differences in consultation length and Education and language proficiency unrelated to communication differences
prescription behaviour
Consultation length is longer with ethnic minority patients
B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34 27

Table 2 (Continued )
Study Research questions Main findings
Consultation length is unrelated to communication differences
Sleath Effect of patients’ ethnicity on physician–patient More physician–patient communication about antidepressants
et al. [26] communication about antidepressants and with White patients than with Hispanic patients
antidepressant adherence
Hispanic patients were less adherent to their therapy
than White patients
Only one communication variable (patient question-asking)
related to antidepressant adherence
Sleath Effect of patients’ ethnicity on patients’ White patients were more likely to complain
et al. [27] expression of complaints about and problems about their medication than Hispanic patients
of adherence to antidepressant
medication and physicians’ reactions to these
complaints and problems
No differences in reported adherence problems between
the two patient groups
Physicians reacted similarly to Hispanic and to White patients
Cooper Comparison of patient-physician communication Race-concordant visits were longer and had slower
et al. [28] in race-concordant and race-discordant visits speech speed than race-discordant visits
Effect of communication behaviours on Higher ratings of positive patient affect in race-concordant than
patient ratings of satisfaction and in race-discordant visits
participatory decision-making
Patients in race-concordant visits rated their physician
as more participatory than patients in race-discordant visits
Patients in race-concordant visits were more satisfied than patients
in race-discordant visits
Race concordance has an independent effect on patients’
judgments about the visit regardless of doctor–patient communication
Johnson Comparison of doctor–patient communication No difference in duration or speech speed of medical visits between
et al. [29] between ethnic minority patients African-American and White patients
and White patients
More physicians’ verbal dominance with African-American patients
than with White patients
Physicians are less patient-centred with African-American patients
than with White patients
Less positive affect in medical visits with African-American
patients than with White patients

3.2.2. Patients’ communicative behaviour expectations and thoughts than English-speaking patients. It
A majority of the quantitative studies investigated should be noted however that no differences emerged
differences in patients’ verbal behaviour as a function of between Latino and non-Latino English-speaking patients,
ethnic background. Findings of these studies indicated that indicating that differences might be attributable to language
ethnic minority patients express less affective and instru- rather than to ethnicity. More evidence for the role of
mental verbal behaviour than White patients and that they language on patients’ communicative behaviour comes from
seem to be less assertive. For instance, in the Dutch studies it a study by Seijo et al. [19], in which significant differences
was found that ethnic minority patients expressed less were found on the amount of patients’ question-asking
concern as well as less friendliness and social talk [23,25]. between language-concordant and language-discordant
These findings are supported by the results of the American consultations. Patients who communicated in their own
studies, which also yielded evidence of less expression of language with their physician (i.e. Spanish) asked more
verbal behaviour by ethnic minority patients [29]. Sleath questions than Hispanic patients who had to speak English.
et al. [26,27] found that White Americans were almost twice
more likely to complain and to state information about their 3.2.3. Culture’s influence on doctor–patient
antidepressants than Hispanics. In another study, signifi- communication: antecedents and outcomes
cantly higher ratings of positive patient affect (i.e. The variable most often studied (seven times) as a
friendliness, interest, responsiveness and engagement) were culture-related explanatory factor of gaps in intercultural
found in race-concordant as opposed to race-discordant medical communication has to do with linguistic factors.
visits [28]. Rivadeneyra et al. [20] found that Spanish- Most studies did not find any relation between linguistic
speaking patients mentioned fewer symptoms, feelings, factors and variations in communication though. Results of
28 B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34

the study by Rivadeneyra et al. and Seijo et al. [19,20] show despite the fact that there are considerable differences
that language may have had an impact on physicians’ between the two groups on a number of factors relevant for
communicative behaviour. Also, Cass et al. [22] suggest that the topic of doctor–patient communication, such as their
employing differing modes of discourse leads to inter- average educational level (Turkish people in The Nether-
cultural communication problems. Other predictor variables lands are higher educated and more literate than Moroccan
were seldom incorporated in the studies, although Cass et al. people in The Netherlands). In short, clarification of
[22] suggest that differences in beliefs about health and categorization criteria and the reporting of results per ethnic
illness as well as differences in cultural values between minority group would greatly enhance the possibility to
members of different ethnic groups are associated with gaps compare across studies and to reach more definite
in doctor–patient communication. As far as educational conclusions, which may well differ between the various
level is concerned, results are mixed. Harmsen [25] could ethnic minority groups.
not establish a significant relation between education and Taking into account the above, it seems safe to conclude
communication, whereas Sleath et al. [24,26,27] found that that findings do suggest considerable differences and
higher educated ethnic minority patients did communicate difficulties in communication between doctors and patients
more about depression issues and also received more from different cultural and ethnic backgrounds. There is
positive responses from their doctors than lower educated evidence suggesting that doctors behave less affectively
ethnic minority patients. when interacting with ethnic minority patients. More or less
Three studies assessed the impact of intercultural doctor– the same can be said of doctors’ instrumental verbal
patient communication on a number of health-related behaviour, although these results were less pronounced.
outcomes, such as patient understanding, compliance and Ethnic minority patients themselves were also less verbally
satisfaction [23,26,28]. All in all, no relationship could be expressive and seem to be less assertive and affective during
demonstrated between the communication process and these the medical encounter than White patients. These findings
outcome variables. Difficulties in communicating with are mostly in concordance with results of another review
ethnic minority patients did not result in worse compliance, study on the topic of culture and the doctor–patient
satisfaction and understanding as compared to commu- relationship, which also indicated less affective and assertive
nicating with White patients. What was found is that ethnic behaviour in medical consultations with minority patients
minority patients were less satisfied with GPs’ commu- [31].
nicative behaviour and were less compliant, and that less Although the current review clearly shows that there is
mutual understanding was achieved between ethnic minority considerable variation in doctor–patient communication as a
patients and their GPs [16,23,26,28]. Harmsen [25] found consequence of patients’ ethnic background, explanations
that ethnic minority patients were prescribed medication for and consequences of these communication differences
more often than White patients, irrespective of differences in were only seldom incorporated in the studies. For instance,
communication. It therefore seems that patients’ ethnicity why White doctors and ethnic minority patients express less
has an independent and negative effect on outcomes, affective verbal behaviour towards each other is hard to tell
regardless of the communication process as measured by the because most of the studies did not relate communication
observational instruments used in these studies. behaviour to possible predictor variables, except for
patients’ ethnic background. The only other variable
researched in the studies under review concerns linguistic
4. Discussion and conclusion barriers, and in general no relationship could be established
between this variable and variations in doctor–patient
In reviewing the literature on intercultural medical communication [16,25]. None of the intercultural medical
communication we only found a handful of observational communication studies has incorporated any other predictor
studies addressing this topic, yielding partly inconsistent variables, leaving us in the dark about possible explanatory
results. To some extent, these contradictory findings may mechanisms beyond culture and ethnicity for gaps in
have emerged as a consequence of the wide variety of intercultural doctor–patient communication.
research questions and designs used in the studies. This With regard to the consequences of gaps in intercultural
makes it hard to reach definite conclusions about the cultural medical communication, studies could not demonstrate a
variability of doctor–patient communication. Besides, none relationship with difficulties in doctor–patient communica-
of the studies under review explained on what definition the tion. Findings clearly showed that ethnic minority patients
categorization of ethnic minority patients was based. It suffer worse health-related outcomes, such as satisfaction
would be worthwhile in future research to clearly state how and compliance, than White patients, but patients’ ethnicity
the various patients are categorised into different ethnic seemed to have an independent effect. This could be due to
groups, and to take into consideration the considerable the fact that other culture-related variables not assessed in
variation that may exist between and within ethnic minority these studies influence patient satisfaction, understanding
groups. For instance, in the Dutch studies Turkish and and compliance. For instance, in two studies by Brown and
Moroccan patients are regarded as one (Muslim) group, Segal [32,33] it was found that African-Americans were
B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34 29

more present-oriented than Whites in the daily management fact that one knows that one scores behaviour of ethnic
of their hypertension, which is possibly related to higher minorities may in itself lead to bias, because the label ‘ethnic
non-compliance rates and greater use of home remedies. minority’ is associated with other connotations than the label
However, as it is well established that good doctor–patient ‘White’ or ‘native-born’. Second, the risk of interpreting the
communication is associated with improved health out- communicative behaviour of someone from another ethnic
comes [15], it is difficult to assume that no relationship group incorrectly may be higher than interpreting the
exists between intercultural problems in medical commu- communicative behaviour of someone belonging to one’s
nication and patient outcomes. This points to the necessity to own group, because of lack of knowledge about cultural
make use of other observational instruments than the ones differences in communication styles [34]. Consequently,
most frequently employed at present (i.e. RIAS, other self- future research should not only incorporate other doctor–
developed frequency systems) to assess doctor–patient patient dyads but also add ethnic minority coders. Thus, to
communication. Specifically, almost all studies under be able to disentangle possible ethnic effects on doctor–
review have made use solely of frequency systems that patient communication, the most effective research design
focus on the relational aspects of communication (instru- would be a two (White versus non-White doctor) by two
mental versus affective), thereby neglecting other, more (White versus non-White patient) by two (White versus non-
content-related, aspects of communication. Frequency White coder) design.
countings of behaviour however do not shed light on the
dynamics of the communication process, thereby running 4.1. Proposal of research model
the risk of leaving essential aspects of difficulties in
intercultural medical communication undetected. In addi- This review clearly shows that there is a profound lack
tion to the traditional quantitative measures of communica- of knowledge about a wide range of issues related to the
tion behaviour more qualitative methods of observational complex research field of intercultural medical commu-
analyses should be employed, as we want to gain more nication. There are hardly any empirical studies in which
insight into gaps in intercultural medical communication. doctor–patient communication has been assessed directly,
From a methodological perspective, it is not only and most of the study findings are hard to compare
necessary to use more diverse observational means and because of differences in definitions, aims and research
analysis systems (e.g. none of the studies systematically designs. Moreover, the majority of the studies did not
investigated cultural differences in non-verbal behaviour) relate communication difficulties to outcome measures,
when investigating the doctor–patient communication nor did they measure possible explanatory mechanisms for
process, other research designs should also be employed. these communication problems. This has resulted in an
For instance, except for the study by Cooper et al. [28], none atheoretical, purely descriptive research field, without a
of the studies matched patients and doctors on their ethnic coordinating principle in which the different research
background. Hence, most studies only compared White parts can be brought together. Without such a guiding
doctor-ethnic minority patient dyads with White doctor– principle we can hardly expect to find effective solutions
White patient dyads. However, Cooper et al. [28] found that for the communication problems encountered within
race-concordant pairs of physicians and patients had higher intercultural medical visits. It is thus first and foremost
ratings of positive affect than race-discordant pairs, necessary to design a model on which future research in
suggesting that it is perhaps not patients’ ethnic background this area can be based. For this reason we have examined
per se, but differences in ethnic and cultural background that the broader literature on intercultural (medical) commu-
lead to difficulties in the medical communication process. nication and identified five variables likely to play a
Perhaps resemblance in communication styles among significant role in explaining culture’s influence on
members of the same ethnic groups enhances verbal medical communication. We will discuss these factors
expressions, while differences may get in the way of a in more detail now and propose a research model that can
smooth flow of conversation. To test for this possibility, be used to advance the research field of intercultural
future research should also incorporate other dyads, such as medical communication.
ethnic minority patient and ethnic minority doctor dyads,
which up to now have been absent from research on this 4.1.1. Cognitive processes: Kleinman’s explanatory
topic. model
Finally, a closely related methodological issue is the need To understand how patients’ cultural background may
to make use of coders who are of the same ethnic influence doctor–patient communication, Kleinman has
background as the patients whose behaviour they score. proposed the idea of the Explanatory Model (EM) [3,35].
Although none of the studies stated the ethnic background of EM is defined as ‘the notions about an episode of sickness
the people who scored patients’ and doctors’ communicative and its treatment that are employed by all those engaged in
behaviour, it is likely that predominantly White people were the clinical process’. Both patients and doctors hold EMs as
used. This may lead to bias in several ways. First, as it is they offer explanations of episodes of sickness and
nearly impossible in this kind of research to code blindly, the appropriate treatments. According to Kleinman, there will
30 B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34

often be discrepancies between patients’ and doctors’ EMs, ness was not), so the hypothesised explanatory mechanisms
resulting in gaps in doctor–patient communication. These for culture’s influence on patient participation were not
discrepancies result from patients’ EMs being heavily substantiated. In a study by Kim et al. [46], cultural
influenced by personality and cultural factors, while doctors’ differences in willingness to be assertive and beliefs about
EMs are mostly based on a more narrow biomedical participation did emerge. They found that Asians had fewer
perspective. With regard to the question of how to positive beliefs about patient participation than Western
empirically study culture’s influence on patients’ EMs, participants and were less willing to be assertive. Perhaps the
the model proposed by Angel and Thoits [36] is particularly perceived appropriateness of assertiveness rather than the
promising. It describes how culture impacts on certain perceived ability to be assertive or the perceived effective-
cognitive structures that mediate people’s interpretation of ness of assertiveness mediates the influence of patients’
physical and emotional states, such as the likelihood of cultural background on patient participation, as is also
certain states being labelled as symptoms, the evaluation of suggested by Young and Klingle [45].
these symptoms and so on, and is a worthwhile starting point While it is clear that the individualism–collectivism
for more extensive research on this topic. Maclachlan [37] construct provides a useful framework for understanding
offers another relevant method; he designed a technique for cultural differences in communication behaviour, it does not
assessing the interplay of culture and health in such a way clarify the underlying psychological processes to which
that it acknowledges the influence of culture on health by these differences may be attributable. Besides, it does not
embracing the pluralism of conceptual perspectives. take into consideration the heterogeneity in behaviour and
underlying values within single cultures [47]. One of the
4.1.2. Cultural values: individualism–collectivism and factors that have been identified as possible mediator of the
self-construals influence of cultural individualism–collectivism on com-
One of the dimensions most often used in explaining munication behaviour is the individuals’ self-construal, in
variations in communication between cultures is the terms of independent versus interdependent [48]. The
individualism–collectivism construct [38,39]. In short, independent construal of the self ‘. . .requires construing
people belonging to predominantly individualistic cultures oneself as an individual whose behaviour is organized and
see the self as independent of groups, whereas in made meaningful primarily by reference to one’s own
collectivistic cultures the self is seen as part of the group. internal repertoire of thoughts, feelings, and action, rather
Personal goals are more important in individualistic than by reference to the thoughts, feelings, and actions of
cultures, while goals and norms of the group prevail in others’ [48, p. 226]. The interdependent construal of the self
more collectivistic-oriented societies. The distinction ‘. . .entails seeing oneself as part of an encompassing social
between individualistic and collectivistic cultures provides relationship and recognizing that one’s behaviour is
a useful framework for understanding cultural variations in determined, contingent on, and, to a large extent organized
communication, and there is indeed much research showing by what the actor perceives to be the thoughts, feelings, and
that rules for communication and conversation as well as actions of others in the relationship’ [48, p.227]. It is
actual communication behaviour differ between these two assumed that in collectivistic cultures individuals tend to
types of cultures. For example, Asians, who are collecti- rely more on their interdependent self, whereas people in
vistically oriented, are less assertive and less direct in their individualistic cultures rely mainly on their independent
conversations than Americans, who are individualistically self. It has been shown however that these two self-
oriented [40,41]. Moreover, collectivists pay more attention construals coexist in individuals, and that it is partly the
to the situation and to paralinguistic cues, whereas situation that determines which of the two self-construals is
individualists pay more attention to the exact content of activated [49]. Gudykunst et al. [50] demonstrated that self-
the message but tend to neglect the paralinguistic cues construals are better predictors of and explain more variance
[39,42]. Hall [43] explained this variation in communication in communication style than the individualism–collectivism
style by differentiating between low- and high-context construct.
communication. The kind of communication style people
use and find most appropriate also differs between 4.1.3. Motivational processes: patient preferences
individuals within a single country who belong to different An indisputable shift in the doctor–patient relationship
ethnic groups [44]. has been noticeable in the last couple of decades. Because of
Within medicine, Young and Klingle [45] hypothesised several societal and technological changes during that time
that cultural norms influence patients’ assertiveness, their period in the Western world [51], the traditional paterna-
perceived ability to perform certain responses (self-efficacy) listic way of treating patients is no longer considered
and their conviction that the recommended response is acceptable; instead, a model of mutual participation in
effective (response-efficacy), which together influence the which patient education, patient involvement and shared
amount of patient participation. Although there were no decision-making are the new conventions and patient
cultural differences between response- and self-efficacy, autonomy the new ethos is advocated [52,53]. One of the
they were indeed related to patient participation (assertive- key dimensions underlying this paradigm shift is the
B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34 31

concept of patient-centeredness, that is, the ideal of an literature on this topic [66]). In reviewing a number of US
egalitarian doctor–patient relationship in which doctors and studies on language barriers among Latino and Asian
patients share responsibility for the interaction and its patients, Ramirez [4] found that linguistic barriers can lead
outcomes [54,55]. to a number of negative consequences, such as an increased
One of the main assumption underlying ideas about chance of non-compliance, feelings of fear and despair, and
shared decision-making and mutual participation is that problems in achieving rapport. Moreover, the inability of
patients prefer to be involved in their own care. This patients to understand and speak the language of their
assumption may not be valid for every patient and in every physician may be an important obstacle in seeking care. His
situation though. It has been shown that depending on one’s findings are supported by results of some European studies,
personality (e.g. locus of control, assertiveness), beliefs which show that poor language proficiency is negatively
about the extent to which powerful others such as doctors related to patients’ perceptions of the quality of doctor–
should be responsible for making decisions about patients’ patient communication as well as to compliance [24,67].
health differ [56]. Patients’ ethnic background is also related Although it is beyond doubt that poor language
to differences in preferences for different types of doctor– proficiency will hinder adequate doctor–patient commu-
patient relationships. For instance, Korean-Americans and nication, the extent to which these linguistic barriers are the
Mexican-Americans have less need for information dis- predominant factor in determining the quality of commu-
closure and participation in decision-making than White nication and care is not that clear-cut [68]. There is some
American patients [57]. Furthermore, patients from different empirical evidence indicating that other barriers play a more
European countries attach different importance to various prominent role in intercultural communication problems.
communication aspects, such as the amount of biomedical For example, informing ethnic minority patients in their own
and psychosocial communication they consider important language does not improve compliance rates with preventive
[58]. Although it is likely that some elements of patient screening activities [69]. Besides, patients’ satisfaction with
preferences are universal, such as the need to be heard and communication in health care is only partly related to their
understood, other aspects are likely to be culture-bound. For ability to speak the same language as the physician. Morales
instance, for Chilean patients being touched by their doctor et al. [70] found that Latinos who spoke English were still
is an important attribute, while British patients do not less satisfied than White patients with doctor–patient
mention this as a valuable quality [59]. This finding may communication, and Elsass et al. [71] found that, in spite
reflect a culture-specific translation of a universal need to of language difficulties and insufficient communication
feel understood. between 50 Greenlandic patients and their Danish therapists,
patients were satisfied with received care. Patient satisfac-
4.1.4. Perceptions: racism/bias tion was related to perceptions of emotional factors such as
Another barrier to adequate doctor–patient communica- doctors’ kindness and smiling faces, and not to linguistic
tion in interethnic encounters is the issue of racism and problems.
perceptual bias [60,61]. For instance, American physicians The way the five aspects described above are related to
perceive their Black patients as being less intelligent, more intercultural doctor–patient communication is summarised
likely to perform high-risk behaviours and more likely to be in Fig. 1.
non-compliant than White patients. They also have less
affiliate feelings towards Black patients compared to White 4.2. Study limitations
patients [62]. When ethnic minority patients are asked about
racist experiences, they are more likely to perceive racism, Due to the nature of our research questions, our review of
report mistrust and, in general, perceive more unfairness in the literature has some limitations. While we define
getting the treatment they need than Whites [63,64]. intercultural health communication broadly, we limited our
Apart from the study by van Ryn and Burke [62], whether review to observational studies, because our primary aim was
the racist experiences ethnic minority patients perceive to research communicative behaviour. As observational
correspond with how they are actually treated by physicians methods yield the most reliable assessment of behaviour
and the health care system in general has not been numerous studies on the topic of intercultural health
empirically studied yet. However, their perceptions are communication were excluded, based on their methods.
likely to have a negative impact on the quality of care and Thus, studies based on self-reports, surveys, in-depth inter-
should therefore be topic of research in the field of views and the like were not included in our review. However,
intercultural doctor–patient communication [63,65]. we did make use of the broader literature on intercultural
health communication in the second part of this article. In
4.1.5. Language: linguistic barriers doing so, our aim was not to give an exhaustive account of all
One obvious hindrance to intercultural communication is available studies, but to identify key predictors of difficulties
the often-encountered lack of language understanding in intercultural health communication, because our aim was to
between doctors and patients belonging to different be able to design a research model that may be used in future
ethnic/cultural groups (see for an extensive review of the research on this topic. This approach did not make it feasible
32 B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34

Fig. 1. Research model.

to discuss each variable in detail. Detailed reviews on in cultural values may play a role of importance in
language variables do exist [e.g. 4, 66] but with regard to the intercultural medical communication. Providers’ knowledge
other variables, much needed review studies still have to be about patients level of acculturation and a good exchange of
carried out. explanatory models are essential in achieving an adequate
doctor–patient relationship. Furthermore, effective commu-
4.3. Conclusion nication skills that expand language capabilities need to be
enhanced. In this respect, more use could be made of
As is clear from our review study, the above-identified migrant health educators employed as mediators to support
variables were not investigated at all in the studies under the consultations of ethnic minority patients. Previous
review, except for linguistic barriers. To be able to pin down research has indicated that experiences in The Netherlands
the explanatory mechanisms beyond gaps in intercultural of GPs, patients and these link workers have been positive
doctor–patient communication, future studies should start to [73]. In sum, using a cultural sensitive approach by paying
incorporate these variables into their research designs and attention to the cultural variables as outlined in our
begin to integrate available knowledge and theories in the field theoretical model is recommended. Only by employing
of cross-cultural communication with those in the field of such a combined approach we will make every effort to
doctor–patient communication. Hopefully this will be a first decrease the extent of problematic communication in
step towards advancing our knowledge about the origins and intercultural medical encounters.
solutions of gaps in intercultural medical communication.
In sum, the main conclusion of this review is that the
extent of gaps in intercultural medical communication, its References
origins and the relationship between cultural variations in
medical communication and health outcomes are still near- [1] Statistics Netherlands. Allochtonen in Nederland [Migrants in The
unexplored topics for research. Far more empirical research Netherlands]. The Hague: Statistics Netherlands; 2004.
[2] Gudykunst WB. Cultural variability in communication. Commun Res
on the topic is needed, research that not only takes into 1997;24:327–48.
account other observational strategies and communication [3] Kleinman A. Patients and healers in the context of culture. London:
variables such as doctors’ and patients’ non-verbal University of California Press; 1980.
behaviour, but also tries to explain communication problems [4] Ramirez AG. Consumer-provider communication research with spe-
cial populations. Patient Educ Couns 2003;50:51–4.
by incorporating theory-based variables such as the ones
[5] Penn NE, Kar A, Kramer J, Skinner J, Zambrana RE. Panel VI: ethnic
outlined in our research model. Closely related is the need to minorities, health care systems, and behavior. Health Psychol 1995;
reach some consensus about what we mean by concepts as 14:641–6.
race, culture and ethnicity, why and how we think these [6] Kleinman A. Clinical relevance of anthropological and cross-cultural
concepts will influence medical communication, and how research: concepts and strategies. Am J Psychiat 1978;135:427–31.
we should measure these relationships. Furthermore, a [7] Harmsen JAM, Meeuwesen L, van Wieringen J, Bernsen R, Bruijn-
zeels M. When cultures meet in general practice: intercultural differ-
clearer sense of which problems are culture-specific and ences between GPs and parents of child patients. Patient Educ Couns
which are related to possible confounders of culture (i.e. 2003;51:99–106.
social class, inequality in power, etc.) is needed as well [72]. [8] Laveist TA, Nuru-Jeter A. Is doctor–patient race concordance asso-
In other words, we need to ask ourselves which aspects of ciated with greater satisfaction with care? J Health Soc Behav 2002;
4:296–306.
doctor–patient communication are universal and which
[9] Murray-Garcı́a JL, Selby J, Schmittdiel J, Grumbach K, Quesenberry
aspects are culture-specific, as culture adds just one more C. Racial and ethnic differences in a patient survey: patients’ values,
dimension to an already difficult communication situation. ratings, and reports regarding physician primary care performance in a
large health maintenance organization. Med Care 2000;38:300–10.
4.4. Practice implications [10] Saha S, Komaromy M, Koepsell TD, Bindman AB. Patient-physician
racial concordance and the perceived quality and use of health care.
Arch Intern Med 1999;159:997–1004.
In line with our conclusions, health care providers should [11] Nierkens V, Krumeich A, de Ridder R, Dongen M. The future of
keep an open mind regarding consultations with ethnic intercultural mediation in Belgium. Patient Educ Couns 2002;46:
minority patients and need to reflect on the way differences 253–9.
B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34 33

[12] Gerits YC, Uitenbroek DG, Dijkshoorn H, Verhoeff AP. De commu- [35] Kleinman A. The cultural meanings and social uses of illness. J Fam
nicatie tussen huisarts en Turkse en Marokkaanse patiënten nader Pract 1983;16:539–45.
bekeken [A study about communication between GP and Turkish and [36] Angel R, Thoits P. The impact of culture on the cognitive structure of
Moroccan patients]. Tijdschr voor Gezondheidswetenschappen 2001; illness. Cult Med Psychiat 1987;11:465–94.
79:15–20. [37] Maclachlan M. Cultivating pluralism in health psychology. J Health
[13] Ibrahim SA, Whittle J, Bean-Mayberry B, Kelley ME, Good C, Psychol 2000;5:373–82.
Conigliaro J. Racial/ethnic variations in physician recommendations [38] Hofstede G. Culture’s consequences. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 2001.
for cardiac revascularization. Am J Public Health 2003;93:1689–93. [39] Triandis HC, Trafimow D. Culture and its implications for intergroup
[14] Rucker-Whitaker C, Feinglass J, Pearce WH. Explaining racial varia- behavior. In: Brown R, Gaertner SL, editors. Blackwell handbook of
tion in lower extremity amputation. Arch Surg 2003;138:1347–51. social psychology: intergroup processes. Oxford: Blackwell; 2001.
[15] Roter D, Hall JA. Doctors talking with patients/patients talking with [40] Holtgraves T. Styles of language use: Individual and cultural varia-
doctors. Westport: Auburn House; 1992. bility in conversational indirectness. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;73:
[16] Shapiro J, Saltzer E. Cross-cultural aspects of physician–patient 624–37.
communication patterns. Urban Health 1981;10:10–5. [41] Zane NWS, Sue S, Hu L, Kwon J. Asian-American assertion: a social
[17] Hooper EM, Comstock LM, Goodwin JM, Goodwin JS. Patient learning analysis of cultural differences. J Couns Psychol 1991;38:
characteristics that influence physician behavior. Med Care 1982; 63–70.
20:630–8. [42] Nisbett RE. The geography of thought: how Asians and Westerners
[18] Erzinger H. Communication between Spanish-speaking patients and think differently— and why. London: Nicholas Brealey; 2003.
their doctors in medical encounters. Cult Med Psychiat 1991;15:91– [43] Hall ET. Beyond culture. New York: Doubleday; 1976.
110. [44] Hecht ML, Ribeau S, Sedano MV. A Mexican-American perspective
[19] Seijo R, Gomez H, Freidenberg J. Language as a communication on interethnic communication. Int J Intercult Rel 1990;14:31–55.
barrier in medical care for Hispanic patients. Hispanic J Behav Sci [45] Young M, Klingle RS. Silent partners in medical care: a cross-cultural
1991;13:363–73. study of patient participation. Health Commun 1996;8:29–53.
[20] Rivadeneyra R, Elderkin-Thompson V, Cohen-Silver R, Waitzkin H. [46] Kim M, Klingle RS, Sharkey WF, Park HS, Smith DH, Cai D. A test of
Patient-centeredness in medical encounters requiring an interpreter. a cultural model of patients’ motivation for verbal communication in
Am J Med 2000;108:470–4. patient-doctor interactions. Commun Monogr 2000;67:262–83.
[21] Sleath B, Rubin RH, Arrey-Wastavino A. Physician expression of [47] Betancourt H, López SR. The study of culture, ethnicity, and race in
empathy and positiveness to Hispanic and non-Hispanic White American psychology. Am Psychol 1993;48:629–37.
patients during medical encounters. Fam Med 2000;32:91–6. [48] Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: implications for
[22] Cass A, Lowell A, Christie M, Snelling PL, Flack M, Marrnganyin B, cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol Rev 1991;98:224–53.
Brown I. Sharing the true stories: improving communication between [49] Singelis TM. The measurement of independent and interdependent
Aboriginal patients and healthcare workers. Med J Australia 2002; self-construals. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 1994;20:580–91.
176:466–70. [50] Gudykunst WB, Matsumoto Y, Ting-Toomey S, Nishida T, Kim K,
[23] van Wieringen JCM, Harmsen JAM, Bruijnzeels MA. Intercultural Heyman S. The influence of cultural individualism–collectivism, self-
communication in general practice. Eur J Public Health 2002;12:63–8. construals, and individual values on communication styles across
[24] Sleath B, Rubin RH. Gender, ethnicity, and physician–patient com- cultures. Hum Commun Res 1996;22:510–43.
munication about depression and anxiety in primary care. Patient Educ [51] Dupuis HM, de Beaufort ID. Informed consent. In: Dupuis HM, De
Couns 2002;48:243–52. Beaufort ID, editors. Handboek gezondheidsethiek [handbook of
[25] Harmsen JAM. When cultures meet in medical practice (Ph.D. thesis). health ethics]. Assen: van Gorcum; 1988.
Rotterdam: Erasmus University; 2003. [52] de Haes H, Koedoot N. Patient centered decision-making in palliative
[26] Sleath B, Rubin RH, Huston SA. Hispanic ethnicity, physician–patient cancer treatment: a world of paradoxes. Patient Educ Couns
communication, and antidepressant adherence. Compr Psychiat 2003; 2003;50:43–9.
44:198–204. [53] Wear S. Informed consent: patient autonomy and physician benefi-
[27] Sleath B, Rubin RH, Wurst K. The influence of Hispanic ethnicity on cence within clinical medicine. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publish-
patients’ expression of complaints about and problems with adherence ers; 1993.
to antidepressant therapy. Clin Ther 2003;25:1739–49. [54] Roter D. The enduring and evolving nature of the patient-physician
[28] Cooper LA, Roter DL, Johnson RL, Ford DE, Steinwachs DM, Powe relationship. Patient Educ Couns 2000;39:5–15.
NR. Patient-centered communication, ratings of care, and concor- [55] Stewart M, Brown JB, Weston WW, McWinney IR, McWilliam CL,
dance of patient and physician race. Ann Intern Med 2003;139: Freeman JR. Patient-centered medicine: transforming the clinical
907–15. method. London: Sage; 1995.
[29] Johnson RL, Roter D, Powe NR, Cooper LA. Patient race/ethnicity and [56] Braman AC, Gomez RG. Patient personality predicts preference for
quality of patient-physician communication during medical visits. Am relationships with doctors. Pers Indiv Differ 2004;37:815–26.
J Public Health 2004;94:2084–90. [57] Blackhall LJ, Murphy S, Frank G, Michel V, Azen S. Ethnicity and
[30] Roter DL. The Roter method of interaction process analysis: RIAS attitudes toward patient autonomy. J Am Med Assoc 1995;274:820–5.
manual. Baltimore: John Hopkins University; 1993. [58] van den Brink-Muinen A, Verhaak PFM, Bensing JM, Bahrs O,
[31] Ferguson WJ, Candib LM. Culture, language, and the doctor–patient Deveugele M, Gask L, Leiva F, Mead N, Messerli V, Oppizzi L,
relationship. Fam Med 2002;34:353–61. Peltenburg M, Perez A. Doctor–patient communication in different
[32] Brown CM, Segal R. Ethnic differences in temporal orientation and its European health care systems: relevance and performance from the
implications for hypertension management. J Health Soc Behav 1996; patients’ perspective. Patient Educ Couns 2000;39:115–27.
3:350–61. [59] Scarpaci JL. Help-seeking behavior, use, and satisfaction among
[33] Brown CM, Segal R. The effects of health and treatment perceptions frequent primary care users in Santiago de Chile. J Health Soc Behav
on the use of prescribed medication and home remedies among 1988;29:199–213.
African-American and White American hypertensives. Soc Sci Med [60] Ashton CM, Haidet P, Paterniti DA, Collins TC, Gordon HS, O’Malley
1996;43:903–17. K, Petersen LA, Sharf BF, Suarez-Almazor ME, Wray NP, Sharf BF,
[34] Banks SP, Ge G, Baker J. Intercultural encounters and Street RL. Racial and ethnic disparities in the use of health services:
miscommunication. In: Coupland N, Giles H, Wiemann JM, editors. bias, preferences, or poor communication? J Gen Intern Med 2003;18:
Miscommunication and problematic talk. London: Sage; 1991. 146–52.
34 B.C. Schouten, L. Meeuwesen / Patient Education and Counseling 64 (2006) 21–34

[61] Robinson M, Gilmartin J. Barriers to communication between health their general practitioner for cervical screening. Brit Med J 1994;309:
practitioners and service users who are not fluent in English. Nurs 1126–8.
Educ Today 2002;22:457–65. [68] Patel S. Intercultural consultations: language is not the only barrier.
[62] van Ryn M, Burke J. The effects of patient race and socio-economic Brit Med J 1995;310:194.
status on physician’s perceptions of patients. Soc Sci Med 2000;50: [69] Stone MA, Patel H, Panja KK, Barnett DB, Mayberry JF. Reasons for
813–28. non-compliance with screening for infection with Helicobacter pylori,
[63] LaVeist TA, Nickerson KJ, Bowie JV. Attitudes about racism, medical in a multi-ethnic community in Leicester, UK. Public Health 1998;
mistrust, and satisfaction with care among African-American and 112:153–6.
White cardiac patients. Med Care Res Rev 2000;57:146–61. [70] Morales LS, Cuningham WE, Brown JA, Liu H, Hays RD. Are Latinos
[64] Lillie-Blanton M, Brodie M, Rowland D, Altman D, McIntosh M. less satisfied with communication by health care providers? J Gen
Race, ethnicity, and the health care system: public perceptions and Intern Med 1999;14:409–17.
experiences. Med Care Res Rev 2000;57:218–35. [71] Elsass P, Christensen HP, Falhof J, Hvolby A. Greenlanders in
[65] Levy DR. White doctors and black patients: influence of race in the hospital: lack of language understanding is not always a hindrance
doctor–patient relationship. Pediatrics 1985;75:639–43. to intercultural communication. Arctic Med Res 1994;53:97–104.
[66] Jacobs EA, Agger-Gupta N, Chen AH, Piotrowski A, Hardt EJ. [72] Lillie-Blanton M, LaVeist T. Race/ethnicity, the social environment,
Language barriers in health care settings: an annotated bibliography and health. Soc Sci Med 1996;43:83–91.
of the research literature. Woodland Hills, CA: The California Endow- [73] Bruijnzeels MA, Voorham AJJ, de Hoop T. Migrantenvoorlichting in
ment; 2003. de huisartspraktijk: een procesevaluatie [migrant education in general
[67] Naish J, Brown J, Denton B. Intercultural consultations: investigation practice: evaluating the process]. Huisarts en Wetensc 1999;42:
of factors that deter non-English speaking women from attending 307–10.

You might also like