You are on page 1of 5

Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 146–150

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Item selection and validation of a brief, 20-item version of the


Differentiation of Self Inventory—Revised
David Sloan a,⁎, Dirk van Dierendonck b
a
Whitworth University, USA
b
Erasmus University, Netherlands

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The 46-item Differentiation of Self Inventory—Revised (DSI-R) (Skowron & Schmitt, 2003) is perhaps the most
Received 30 December 2015 widely used and psychometrically validated instrument measuring the family systems theory (Bowen, 1978)
Received in revised form 7 March 2016 concept of differentiation of self. Given the substantial length of the DSI-R, Drake et al. (2015) offered the
Accepted 16 March 2016
Differentiation of Self Inventory—Short Form (DSI-SF). However, based on limitations of the DSI-SF, we
Available online 28 March 2016
empirically validated a brief version of the 4-factor DSI-R using data from two adult samples (Sample 1: n =
Keywords:
541; Sample 2: n = 203). First, we conducted an EFA on the full 46-item DSI-R scale on Sample 1 to help us
Differentiation of self identify items most strongly representing each factor. Then we conducted CFAs comparing the model fit between
Item selection our brief DSI, the DSI-SF, and the full DSI-R on both samples. Results indicated that the brief DSI had the strongest
Short model fit in both samples. Finally, a comparison of associations among the brief DSI, the DSI-SF, and full DSI-R and
Brief various relevant variables showed similar correlations. The brief DSI provides counselors and researchers in the
Abridged areas of psychology, organizational behavior, and business a useful measure of differentiation of self when
Validation circumstances prohibit the use of the full DSI-R.
Family systems theory
© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction emotional reactivity, and become emotionally overinvolved with or


“fused” with others in relationships (p. 239). The most widely used
Because of an increasing interest in understanding how relational and psychometrically validated instrument measuring DOS is the
systems affect psychological and behavioral functioning, scholars have 46-item Differentiation of Self Inventory—Revised (DSI-R) (Skowron &
looked to Bowen's (1978) Family Systems Theory (FST). Articulated as Schmitt, 2003).
one of the most developed theories on human functioning (Skowron Practical reasons warrant the use of a brief version of the DSI-R. The
& Friedlander, 1998), FST claims that differentiation of self (DOS) is length of the full 46-item scale takes time that could be used for other
of key importance (Bowen, 1978). The concept has already been purposes in a clinical setting, or that could be used for measuring
widely examined in the area of counseling psychology; more recently, additional variables in a research setting (Roets & Van Hiel, 2011). The
researchers have begun to explore the application of DOS in the areas large number of items may also introduce respondent fatigue or bore-
of business and organizational behavior (Beebe & Frisch, 2009; dom, which has potential to diminish the quality of the response set
Cochran, 2011). (Credé, Harms, Niehorster, & Gaye-Valentine, 2012). The purpose of
DOS refers to the degree to which an individual can balance emo- this paper is to put forth a brief version of the four-factor DSI-R while
tional with intellectual functioning and closeness with independence maintaining the validity of the full instrument.
in relationships with others (Bowen, 1978). Individuals high in DOS
are able to distinguish their thoughts from their feelings and are capable 1.1. Background to DSI-R
of balancing degrees of autonomy and intimacy (Bowen, 1978; Skowron
& Friedlander, 1998). More specifically, individuals with high DOS have Researchers have found that DOS influences the psychological
the ability to maintain the I-positon, in which they keep a well-defined health and functioning of emotional systems across various contexts
sense of self and possess the capacity to follow convictions despite (e.g., Gushue & Constantine, 2003; Peleg, 2005; Tuason & Friedlander,
experiencing pressure to do otherwise (Skowron & Friedlander, 1998). 2000). Empirically, differentiation has been found to lower stress and
Individuals low in DOS tend to be more emotionally cut off, engage in anxiety (Murdock & Gore, 2004), increase psychological well-being
(Skowron, Holmes, & Sabatelli, 2003), and improve relational function-
⁎ Corresponding author at: Whitworth University, 300 W. Hawthorne Road, Spokane,
ing (Chung & Gale, 2009). Evidence also supports differentiation as
WA, USA. negatively related to attachment avoidance and anxiety while being
E-mail address: DSloan@Whitworth.edu (D. Sloan). positively related to effortful control (Skowron & Dendy, 2004).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.037
0191-8869/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
D. Sloan, D. van Dierendonck / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 146–150 147

Although multiple instruments measuring DOS exist (i.e., Bartle & 1.3. The need for a brief version of the DSI-R
Sabatelli, 1995; Haber, 1993; Jankowski & Hooper, 2012; Licht &
Chabot, 2006; Skowron & Friedlander, 1998; Skowron & Schmitt, Clinical psychologists and researchers in the disciplines of
2003), the 46-item DSI-R (Skowron & Schmitt, 2003) is one of the psychology, organizational behavior, and business would benefit
most used and psychometrically validated instruments. It is the de from having an efficient method to evaluate DOS that is generaliz-
facto instrument for the majority of researchers and practitioners able to populations outside college students. In this study we present
interested in DOS.1 an instrument for this purpose: a brief version of the previously val-
Skowron and Friedlander (1998) developed the Differentiation of idated DSI-R, which is psychometrically sound and captures each of
Self Inventory as a multidimensional construct measuring differentia- the four dimensions assessed in the full 46-item scale. In construct-
tion with four subcategories: emotional cutoff (EC), emotional reactivity ing our brief version, we followed the recommendations by Smith,
(ER), I-position (IP), and fusion with others (FO). Although three of the McCarthy, and Anderson (2000), offering a clear intended use of
four subscales were psychometrically sound, the FO subscale lacked the short form and refraining from assuming that the reliability
reliability and validity. Skowron and Schmitt (2003) recognized this and validity of the original full-length scale automatically applied
weakness and revised the FO subscale to strengthen reliability and va- to shortened versions.
lidity. Their efforts resulted in the construction of the Differentiation To develop the brief DSI, we combined insights gained from reliabil-
of Self Inventory—Revised (DSI-R) with acceptable internal consisten- ity analysis in terms of internal consistency of the subscales with an
cies measured using Cronbach's alpha ranging from .81 to .89 for sub- exploratory factor analysis (EFA) on the full 46-item DSI-R scale using
scales and to .92 for the full scale. Skowron and Dendy (2004) a single sample of adults. To reduce the scale, we kept those items
confirmed the four-factor structure and showed similar reliabilities. with the highest item-total analysis and removed those items with the
However, Chung and Gale (2009) reported weaker internal consisten- lowest loadings on their own dimension as well as loading on multiple
cies among college students in a Korean sample (ranging between .63 factors. In this way, we developed a brief version of the scale that
and .69 for the subscales; full scale = .85) and a European American included the essential items from each subscale while removing those
sample (ranging between .65 and .84; full scale = .87). items that were not clearly representative of one subdimension only.
To confirm our final solution, we conducted a CFA comparing the
1.2. DSI-SF model fit between our brief DSI, the DSI-SF, and the full DSI-R. We
repeated the same CFA on a second independent sample. Finally,
The need for a shorter scale was recognized by Drake, Murdock, to test the construct validity of our short version, we compared the
Marszalek, and Barber (2015), who performed further analysis to associations among the brief DSI, the DSI-SF, and the full DSI-R with
construct a short-form version of the DSI-R. They introduced the other relevant variables.
Differentiation of Self Inventory—Short Form (DSI-SF) as a 20-item Variables chosen for comparison were based on theoretical
instrument constructed using item response theory. However, whereas and empirical associations. These variables included attachment
the original DSI-R focused on adults (Skowron & Schmitt, 2003), the anxiety and avoidance (Bowlby, 1982) and effortful control
DSI-SF was developed exclusively with a college-aged sample. (Rothbart, Ahadi, & Evans, 2000). According to attachment theory
Certain considerations are important when relying solely on college- (Bowlby, 1982), anxiously attached individuals are hyperactive in
age participants. First, several studies have reported differences response to stressful systems. Individuals with avoidant characteristics
between a college-age sample and the society at large regarding tend to deny the importance of intimacy and attachment in relation-
elements crucial for DOS. For example, college students in the ships. The capacity for effortful control depends on a person having
United States differed from their adult, nonstudent counterparts in been securely attached with caregivers. This capacity predicts social
susceptibility to attitude change (Krosnick & Alwin, 1989), degree functioning and psychological adjustment in adults (Skowron &
of self-monitoring (Reifman, Klein, & Murphy, 1989), and social Dendy, 2004). According to Bowen (1978), DOS either constrains or en-
influence (Pasupathi, 1999). In addition, Lamont's (2000) findings ables that individual's capacity for self-regulation, or effortful control, as
suggest that adults without college educations differ from college well as his or her tendency to exhibit emotionally reactive (anxious) or
students in terms of the makeup of their social networks. These emotionally cutoff (avoidant) behaviors.
findings bring into question the generalizability of the outcomes of Skowron and Dendy (2004) empirically tested the theoretical
a validity study based exclusively on a college-age sample to the associations among self-differentiation and attachment anxiety, attach-
general population (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). ment avoidance, and effortful control. Their results confirmed that DOS
Second, the selection method for the DSI-SF items was item response was significantly related to effortful control and to both attachment
theory (IRT), and no factor analysis was performed. IRT is an excellent dimensions. These variables were chosen to compare the magnitudes
method for testing for equivalence of item parameters within one factor of the brief DSI, the DSI-SF, and the full DSI-R scales.
(Meade & Lautenschlager, 2004). However, what is missing is taking
into account in the overlap in item content between constructs. For
testing multidimensional models such as the four-factor construct of 2. Method
differentiation (Skowron & Schmitt, 2003), factor analysis provides
that information (Wang & Russell, 2005). Regretfully, Drake et al. 2.1. Participants
(2015) conducted only a principal component analysis (PCA) to confirm
the unidimensionality of the separate subscales. They also did not report 2.1.1. Sample 1
any confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) results. DSI-R data were collected from 614 fully employed adults living
Third, the number of items kept in the DSI-SF subscales was not in the United States through recruitment services provided by
equivalent. With three items for EC, five in FO, and six in ER and IP, SurveyMonkey Audience. 541 (85%) participants provided usable
the overall scores for ER and IP are better represented than EC data. Population demographics were as follows: 52.1% were male,
and slightly better represented than FO. With this study, we aim to 89.9% white/Caucasian, 2.9% Asian, 2.9% black/African American,
introduce a shorter scale that takes into account all three criticisms. 2.0% Latin/Hispanic, and 1.6% multiracial. Nearly twenty-three
percent (22.7%) of participants were between 25 and 39 years of
1
Skowron, Friedlander, and Mallinckrodt's (2009) 43-item instrument is the original
age, 23.4% were between 40 and 49, 35.5% were between 50 and 59,
DSI developed in 1998. The 2009 article simply reports an error correction found in and 18.5% were 60 or older. The average job tenure was 12.00 years
Skowron and Friedlander's (1998) study. (SD = 9.94), ranging from less than 1 year to 60 years.
148 D. Sloan, D. van Dierendonck / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 146–150

2.1.2. Sample 2 CFA results comparing the brief DSI, the DSI-SR, and the full DSI-R in
DSI-R data were collected from 252 fully employed adults living in two samples.
the United States through recruitment services provided by Amazon's In step one, using Sample 1, we calculated the item/total correlation
Mechanical Turk. Slightly more than 200(203, or 81%) participants with internal consistency. We also conducted an EFA on the full 46-item
provided usable data. Population demographics were as follows: 39.1% scale, eliminating items that loaded relevantly low (less than .3) on their
male, 82.8% white/Caucasian, 2.0% Asian, 9.4% black/African American, own factors and relatively high (greater than .6) on one or more of the
4.4% Latin/Hispanic, and 1.5% from other races. The average age of other factors. This analysis allowed us to reduce the 46-item instrument
participants was 37.47 years (SD = 12.52), ranging from 19 to to a 20-item brief DSI scale. The extraction method was Principle
77 years, and the average job tenure was 6.22 years (SD = 5.85), Component Analysis and the rotation method was Varimax with Kaiser
ranging from less than 1 year to 35 years. Normalization. The items selected for the 20-item brief DSI scale includ-
ed emotional cutoff (items 3, 20, 24, 28, & 39); emotional reactivity
2.2. Measures (items 14, 18, 21, 30, & 34); fusion with others (items 13, 17, 25, 33, &
44); and I-positon (items 7, 15, 19, 31, & 43). The FO subscale is
2.2.1. Differentiation of Self Inventory—Revised the only subscale with items loading on more than one factor. We
Differentiation was measured with the full 46-item Differentiation expected these results, given that in the literature the FO subscale has
of Self Inventory—Revised (DSI-R) (Skowron & Schmitt, 2003). consistently shown weaker validity and less reliability than the other
Respondents rated items on a 6-point Likert-type scale on which 1 scales (e.g., Drake et al., 2015; Jankowski & Hooper, 2012; Johnson,
represents not at all true of me and 6 represents very true of me. The Thorngren, & Smith, 2001; Skowron, 2004).
DSI-R is a self-report measure focusing on adults and their significant We subsequently calculated the internal consistency of the full
relationships that measures four factors: emotional cutoff (e.g., “I scale and the four subscales. Our brief DSI had acceptable internal
have difficulty expressing my feelings to people I care for”), emotional consistencies measured using Cronbach's alpha: brief DSI (Full
reactivity (e.g., “People have remarked that I'm overly emotional”), Scale = .90; EC = .76; ER = .85; FO = .76; IP = .70).
fusion with others (e.g., “I usually need a lot of encouragement from Next, on the dataset from Sample 1, we conducted a CFA using MPlus
others when starting a big job or task”), and the ability to speak from on the 20-item brief DSI scale (MLM X2[164] = 413.364; SRMR = .046,
the I-position (e.g., “I tend to remain pretty calm even under stress”). RMSEA = .053, CFI = .918). We then compared the results with a CFA
The DSI-R was included in both samples. Internal consistencies as on the DSI-SF (Drake et al., 2015) (MLM X2[164] = 520.717; SRMR =
reported by Skowron and Schmitt (2003) reflected the full scale and .061, RMSEA = .064, CFI = .888) and on the full 46-item DSI-R
its subscales (Cronbach's α: DSI-R full scale = .92, ER = .89, IP = .81, (Skowron & Schmitt, 2003) (MLM X2[983] = 3479.846; SRMR = .084,
and FO = .85). RMSEA = .069, CFI = .701). Of the three versions, the factor structure
of our brief DSI clearly showed the strongest fit with the data.
2.2.2. Experiences in close relationships scale To further verify our results, we repeated the CFA with the second
Attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance were measured sample. Although the data from this sample indicated a lesser fitting
in Sample 2 using the 36-item self-report experiences in close relation- model compared with the first sample, our brief DSI scale, again, consis-
ships scale (ECR) (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). Participants rated tently showed a better fit (MLM X2[164] = 325.014; SRMR = .068,
items on a 7-point Likert-type scale on which 1 represents disagree RMSEA = .070, CFI = .879) than the DSI-SF (MLM X2[164] = 374.562;
strongly and 7 represents agree strongly. An example of an item measur- SRMR = .080, RMSEA = .080, CFI = .870) and the full DSI-R (MLM
ing attachment anxiety is “I worry a lot about my relationships.” An item X 2[983] = 3084.433; SRMR = .114, RMSEA = .103, CFI = .524).
example measuring attachment avoidance is “I am nervous when Table 1 illustrates the comparative CFA results for the three scales.
partners get too close to me.” The internal consistency of the experi- Additional analysis showed that it would be possible to improve the
ences in close relationships scale in the current sample was good overall fit by removing FO items 21 and 33, which loaded on more than
(Cronbach's α: ECR Full Scale = .96, Anxiety = .95, Avoidance = .93). one factor. The CFA results of the further reduced instrument showed a
better fit in both Sample one (MLM X2[129] = 271.776; SRMR = .039,
2.2.3. Effortful control scale RMSEA = .045, CFI = .945) and Sample two (MLM X2[129] = 249.964;
Effortful control was measured in Sample 2 using the 19-item SRMR = .066, RMSEA = .068, CFI = .898). However, based on our
ATO-S-EC scale from the Adult Temperament Questionnaire—Short rationale of wanting to keep the number of items representing
Form (ATO-S) (Rothbart et al., 2000). The ATO-S-EC is a self-report each construct consistent, and because of the results showing that the
measure where participants rate items on a 7-point Likert-type 20-item brief DSI consistently produced a stronger model fit than
scale on which 1 represents extremely untrue of you and 7 represents the DSI-SF and the DSI-R, we prefer, for now, to keep the proposed
extremely true of you. Effortful control measures three factors, including 20-item brief DSI.
activation control (e.g., “I can keep performing a task even when I would To test for construct validity, using Sample 2 we calculated correla-
rather not do it”), attention control (e.g., “When interrupted or tions between the brief DSI, the DSI-SF, and full DSI-R scales and
distracted, I usually can easily shift my attention back to whatever I variables previously connected to DOS, such as attachment anxiety
was doing before”), and inhibitory control (e.g., “It is easy for me to
inhibit fun behavior that would be inappropriate”). The internal Table 1
consistency of the effortful control scale in the current sample was Goodness-of-Fit Indicators for the brief DSI, DSI-SF, and the full DSI-R on Sample 1 and
good (Cronbach's α = .86). Sample 2.

Model χ2 df RMSEA CFI SRMR


3. Results
Sample 1 (n = 541)
Brief DSI-R 413.36⁎⁎⁎ 164 .053 .918 .046
In the selection of items from the DSI-R for the brief DSI, (a) we DSI-SF 753.42⁎⁎⁎ 165 .082 .816 .104
focused on strong internal consistency by checking for high item-total Full DSI-R 3479.85⁎⁎⁎ 983 .069 .701 .084
correlations and a strong factorial validity based on the results of an Sample 2 (n = 203)
EFA; (b) we checked the content validity of individual items, such that Brief DSI-R 325.01⁎⁎⁎ 164 .070 .879 .068
items chosen represented the breadth and scope of differentiation DSI-SF 599.05⁎⁎⁎ 165 .114 .637 .118
constructs; and (c) we tried to maintain the reliabilities of the 20-item Full DSI-R 3084.43⁎⁎⁎ 983 .103 .524 .114

brief DSI and its subscales. We confirmed the proposed brief DSI with ⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.
D. Sloan, D. van Dierendonck / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 146–150 149

Table 2
Correlations among and descriptive statistics for the brief DSI, DSI-SF, full DSI-R, attachment anxiety, attachment avoidance, and effortful control.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15

1. ER brief DSI –
2. ER DSI-SF .99⁎⁎ –
3. ER DSI-R .96⁎⁎ .97⁎⁎ –
4. EC brief DSI .57⁎⁎ .57⁎⁎ .58⁎⁎ –
5. EC DSI-SF .45⁎⁎ .44⁎⁎ .45⁎⁎ .78⁎⁎ –
6. EC DSI-R .51⁎⁎ .52⁎⁎ .53⁎⁎ .95⁎⁎ .86⁎⁎ –
7. FO brief DSI .79⁎⁎ .81⁎⁎ .80⁎⁎ .62⁎⁎ .43⁎⁎ .59⁎⁎ –
8. FO DSI-SF .80⁎⁎ .82⁎⁎ .80⁎⁎ .62⁎⁎ .44⁎⁎ .58⁎⁎ .97⁎⁎ –
9. FO DSI-R .73⁎⁎ .74⁎⁎ .73⁎⁎ .40⁎⁎ .30⁎⁎ .33⁎⁎ .77⁎⁎ .75⁎⁎ –
10. IP brief DSI .43⁎⁎ .41⁎⁎ .41⁎⁎ .25⁎⁎ .18⁎ .21⁎⁎ .41⁎⁎ .41⁎⁎ .40⁎⁎ –
11. IP DSI-SF .49⁎⁎ .46⁎⁎ .46⁎⁎ .24⁎⁎ .14⁎ .19⁎⁎ .44⁎⁎ .44⁎⁎ .40⁎⁎ .84⁎⁎ –
12. IP DSI-R .52⁎⁎ .49⁎⁎ .49⁎⁎ .30⁎⁎ .18⁎⁎ .25⁎⁎ .54⁎⁎ .54⁎⁎ .48⁎⁎ .92⁎⁎ .93⁎⁎ –
13. ECR anxiety −.54⁎⁎ −.56⁎⁎ −.58⁎⁎ −.66⁎⁎ −.48⁎⁎ −.65⁎⁎ −.64⁎⁎ −.63⁎⁎ −.44⁎⁎ −.27⁎⁎ −.28⁎⁎ −.37⁎⁎ –
14. ECR avoid −.37⁎⁎ −.38⁎⁎ −.38⁎⁎ −.70⁎⁎ −.66⁎⁎ −.75⁎⁎ −.41⁎⁎ −.41⁎⁎ −.20⁎⁎ −.18⁎⁎ −.19⁎⁎ −.24⁎⁎ .60⁎⁎ –
15. ATO-S
Effortful control .53⁎⁎ .51⁎⁎ .52⁎⁎ .46⁎⁎ .42⁎⁎ .43⁎⁎ .53⁎⁎ .53⁎⁎ .43⁎⁎ .49⁎⁎ .54⁎⁎ .57⁎⁎ −.50⁎⁎ −.35⁎⁎ –

n = 203.
⁎ p b .05.
⁎⁎ p b .01.

and avoidance (ECR) (Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000) and effortful Both versions used virtually identical items for the emotional reactivity
control (ATO-S-EC) (Rothbart et al., 2000). As shown in Table 2, results subscale. Based on these differences, the brief DSI is a good option for
for all three versions were consistent with the findings reported by researchers interested in a shortened version of the instrument that
Skowron and Dendy (2004). also captures the complexity of each subscale.
Next, we compared how our brief DSI and the DSI-SF subscales Our brief DSI shows good construct validity by combining a strongly
related to the full DSI-R. The correlations between the brief DSI and improved factorial validity with good internal consistencies for sub-
the full DSI-R and between the DSI-SF and the full DSI-R were almost scales and a content validity comparable to that of the full DSI-R. Most
identical under each subscale except emotional cutoff. On that subscale, notable is the improvement of the factorial validity compared with
the correlation between the brief DSI subscale and the full instrument both the full DSI-R scale and the DSI-SF. The brief DSI measures four
was slightly stronger than that between the DSI-SF and the full factors: emotional cutoff (e.g., “When things go wrong, talking about
instrument (r = .95, r = .86, respectively). them usually makes it worse”), emotional reactivity (e.g., “I'm overly
sensitive to criticism”), fusion with others (e.g., “I feel a need for
4. Discussion approval from virtually everyone in my life”), and the ability to
speak from the I-position (e.g., “When I am having an argument with
The purpose of this study was to provide clinical psychologists someone, I can separate my thoughts about the issue”).
and researchers in the areas of psychology, organizational behavior,
and business with an empirically validated brief version of the Differen-
tiation of Self Inventory—Revised (Skowron & Schmitt, 2003). A total of 4.1. Limitations
20 items were selected based on their component loadings, including
five items from each DSI-R subscale. CFAs on two samples revealed Despite the advantages of having a validated brief scale, we rec-
that our brief DSI showed a better model fit than the short form version ognize limitations of this study. First, the FO subscale contained
by Drake et al. (2015) and the full DSI-R scale while keeping the internal items loading on more than one factor; the weakness and lack of
consistency of the subscales intact. Finally, variables associated with the clarity of this subscale, as noted by other studies (Drake et al.,
DSI-R in the literature showed similar relationships with both the 2015; Jankowski & Hooper, 2012; Johnson, Thorngren, & Smith,
full DSI-R and our brief DSI. The results of this study suggest that our 2001; Skowron, 2004) warrants further research strengthening the
20-item brief DSI may be used as a valid alternative to the DOS FO subscale, perhaps through the addition of new items. Despite hav-
construct. We would like to emphasize that the brief DSI does not ing two items with double loadings, the final result remains the
attempt to replace the full 46-item scale under all circumstances. The strongest fitting model compared with the DSI-SF and full DSI-R.
brief DSI is designed to measure DOS only when practical circumstances A second limitation is that the ethnic/racial make-up of our partici-
prohibit clinical psychologists and researchers from using the full DSI-R. pant pool was disproportionately Caucasian (89.9% in Sample one
The brief DSI version differed from the DSI-SF (Drake et al., 2015) in and 82.8% in Sample two). However, we propose that our sample of
8 out of 20 items. In our selection of items, we attempted to capture the working-age adults, regardless of ethnic/racial makeup is a more
complexity of all four factors. For example, whereas the items measur- appropriate for developing a brief version of the DSI-R than is a stu-
ing emotional cutoff in the DSI-SF emphasized a sense of discomfort in dent sample. A third limitation is that the first sample consisted of
close, intense relationships, the brief DSI emotional cutoff subscale in- working adults. We attempt to address this limitation by including
cludes items related to concern about loss of personal independence, unemployed and retired adults in Sample 2. Regardless, results of this
confidence regarding the outcomes of talking about stressful issues, study should be generalized with caution to the greater population of
and discomfort in overly intense relationships. In the fusion with others adults, as it is questionable whether those of differing ethnic/racial
subscale, although both versions captured the need for acceptance and backgrounds and those without full-time work would have pro-
personal discomfort with criticism, only the brief DSI included items re- duced results different from those of the participants in our study.
garding a lack of confidence when making decisions alone. When mea- Nevertheless, the use of two adult samples is a strong point of our
suring the I-position, both instruments addressed individual sense of study and gives us confidence that our results can be replicated.
self, personal stability, and concern for approval of others. Only the Conducting research using the brief DSI with diverse people groups
brief DSI measured the ability to accept unchangeable circumstances is encouraged to expand the generalizability of the instrument
and the tendency to separate thoughts about a person from the issue. (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1991).
150 D. Sloan, D. van Dierendonck / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 146–150

References Murdock, N. L., & Gore, P. A., Jr. (2004). Stress, coping, and differentiation of self: A test of
Bowen theory. Contemporary Family Therapy: An International Journal, 26(3),
Bartle, S. E., & Sabatelli, R. M. (1995). The behavioral and emotional reactivity index: 319–335.
Preliminary evidence for construct validity from three studies. Family Relations, 44, Pasupathi, M. (1999). Age differences in response to conformity pressure for emotional
267–277. and nonemotional material. Psychology and Aging, 14, 170–174.
Beebe, R., & Frisch, N. (2009). Development of the differentiation of self and role invento- Peleg, O. (2005). The relation between differentiation and social anxiety: What can be
ry for nurses (DSRI-RN): A tool to measure internal dimensions of workplace stress. learned from students and their parents? American Journal of Family Therapy, 33(2),
Nursing Outlook, 57(5), 240–245. 167–183.
Bowen, M. (1978). Family therapy in clinical practice. New York, NY: J. Aronson. Reifman, A., Klein, J., & Murphy, S. (1989). Self-monitoring and age. Psychology and Aging,
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Volume 1. Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books. 4, 245–246.
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult Roets, A., & Van Hiel, A. (2011). Item selection and validation of a brief, 15-item version of
attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson, & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), the need for closure scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(1), 90–94.
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. (1991). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data
Chung, H., & Gale, J. (2009). Family functioning and self-differentiation: A cross-cultural analysis. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
examination. Contemporary Family Therapy, 31(1), 19–33. Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000). Temperament and personality: Origins
Cochran, N. B. (2011). Bowen family systems theory and its relationship to teachers: Does and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 122–135.
differentiation of self predict teacher job satisfaction? (Doctoral disssertation) Skowron, E. A., & Dendy, A. K. (2004). Differentiation of self and attachment in adulthood:
Hattiesburg, MS: The University of Southern Mississippi (ProQuest Dissertations Relational correlates of effortful control. Contemporary Family Therapy, 26(3),
and Theses). 337–357.
Credé, Harms, Niehorster, & Gaye-Valentine (2012). An evaluation of the consequences of Skowron, E. A., & Friedlander, M. L. (1998). The differentiation of self inventory:
using short measures of the big five personality traits. Journal of Personality and Social Development and initial validation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 235–246.
Psychology, 102(4), 874–888. Skowron, E. A., & Schmitt, T. A. (2003). Assessing interpersonal fusion: Reliability and
Drake, J. R., Murdock, N. L., Marszalek, J. M., & Barber, C. E. (2015). Differentiation of self validity of a new DSI fusion with others subscale. Journal of Marital and Family
inventory—Short form: Development and preliminary validation. Contemporary Therapy, 29(2), 209–222.
Family Therapy, 1–12. Skowron, E. A., Holmes, S. E., & Sabatelli, R. M. (2003). Deconstructing differentiation: Self
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis regulation, interdependent relating, and well-being in adulthood. Contemporary
of self-report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Family Therapy, 25(1), 111–129.
Psychology, 78(2), 350–365. Skowron, E. R. (2004). Differentiation of self mediates college stress and adjustment.
Gushue, G. V., & Constantine, M. G. (2003). Examining individualism, collectivism, and Journal of Counseling and Development, 82(1), 69–78.
self-differentiation in African American college women. Journal of Mental Health Skowron, E., Friedlander, M., & Mallinckrodt, B. (2009). Errata. Journal of Counseling
Counseling, 25(1), 1–16. Psychology, 56(4), 597–598.
Haber, J. (1993). A construct validity study of a differentiation of self scale. Scholarly Smith, G. T., McCarthy, D. M., & Anderson, K. G. (2000). On the sins of short-form
Inquiry for Nursing Practice, 7(3), 165–178. development. Psychological Assessment, 12(1), 102–111.
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Tuason, M. T., & Friedlander, M. L. (2000). Do parents' differentiation levels predict those
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–135. of their adult children? And other tests of Bowen theory in a Philippine sample.
Jankowski, P. J., & Hooper, L. M. (2012). Differentiation of self: A validation study of the Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47(1), 27–35.
Bowen theory construct. Couple and Family Psychology: Research and Practice, 1, Wang, M., & Russell, S. S. (2005). Measurement equivalence of the job descriptive index
226–243. across Chinese and American workers: Results from confirmatory factor analysis
Johnson, P., Thorngren, W. C., & Smith, A. J. (2001). Parental divorce and family functioning: and item response theory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 65, 709–732.
Effects on differential levels of young adults. The Family Journal, 9, 265–272.
Krosnick, J. A., & Alwin, D. F. (1989). Aging and susceptibility to attitude change. Journal of Dr. Sloan is a visiting assistant professor of Management in the School of Business
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 416–425. at Whitworth University. His areas of emphasis are leadership, business psychology,
Lamont, M. (2000). The dignity of working men. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. organizational behavior, and human relations.
Licht, C., & Chabot, D. (2006). The Chabot emotional differentiation scale: A theoretically
and psychometrically sound instrument for measuring Bowen's intrapsychic aspect Dr. van Dierendonck is professor of Human Resource Management at Rotterdam
of differentiation. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 32(2), 167–180. School of Management, Erasmus University (RSM). His areas of expertise include human
Meade, A. W., & Lautenschlager, G. J. (2004). A comparison of item response theory resource management, leadership and leadership development, positive organizational
and confirmatory factor analytic methodologies for establishing measurement scholarship, and measurement development.
equivalence/invariance. Organizational Research Methods, 7(4), 361–388.

You might also like