You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/41138738

Childhood experience and the development of reproductive strategies

Article  in  Psicothema · February 2010


Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

47 205

1 author:

Jay Belsky
University of California, Davis
508 PUBLICATIONS   39,325 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The Trondheim Early Secure Study (TESS) View project

National Evaluation of Sure Start View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jay Belsky on 30 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


心 理 学 报 2007,39(3):454~468
Acta Psychologica Sinica

Experience in Childhood and the Development of Reproductive Strategies

Jay Belsky
Birkbeck University of London

Even though a great deal of mainstream developmental psychology is devoted to understanding whether and how
experiences in childhood shape psychological and behavioural development later in life, little theoretical attention
has been paid to why such cross-time influences should characterize human development. This is especially true
with respect to the well-studied determinants of mating, pair bonding and parenting. Theoretically, Draper and
Harpending (1982), Belsky et al. (1991), Ellis (2004) and Chisholm (1996) have all addressed this lacuna,
stimulating research on linkages between childhood experience and reproductive strategy, which is summarised in
this paper. Concern for experiential effects on pubertal timing distinguishes this line of inquiry from more
traditional developmental studies because an evolutionary perspective suggests that experiences in the family
might affect somatic development. Fifteen years since BSD advanced their “uncanny” prediction, it seems clear
that this pubertal timing, at least in females, is related to selected aspects of early family experience (Ellis, 2004).
Keywords: developmental psychology, pubertal timing, evolutionary perspective.

早期经验对繁衍策略形成与发展的影响

很多发展心理学家探讨了早期经验与心理行为发展的关系,但这些发展心理学家较少以进化理论为指导
来探讨不同发展阶段的心理行为特点。比如,配偶选择、性交往与父母教养等成果颇丰的研究领域均缺
少以进化理论为基础的理论解释。与主流的发展心理学研究不同,Draper 和 Harpending (1982),Ellis
(2004),Chisholm (1996) 和 Belsky 等(1991)以进化理论为基础探讨了早期经验与繁衍策略对儿童心理行为
发展的影响。进化观点认为儿童在家庭中获得的早期经验会影响身体发育与未来的繁衍策略。Ellis
(2004)已经证实女性青春期确实与早期家庭经验有关。
关键词:发展心理学,青春期时间表,进化观点。
分类号:B84-069

Students of human development, especially those childhood was an inadvertent consequence of the
working from traditional psychological perspectives, development of a post-reproductive lifespan which
generally take for granted the biological structure of enabled grandmothers to increase their reproductive
the life course. Introductory textbooks in success by assisting daughters in childrearing, and
developmental psychology routinely point out that thus that learning represents a mere secondary effect
humans are born relatively helpless at birth, have an of a long juvenile period, itself determined by the
extended juvenile period before reaching reproductive long lifespan (Hawkes 2003; Hawkes et al. 2000).
maturity and, in the case of females, experience a However one views this contest of ideas, it makes
rather long post-reproductive life (i.e., menopause). clear that from the standpoint of evolutionary
The fact that the very nature of the human life course anthropology, the main question about human life
is something in need of explanation, at least from an history concerns selection pressures which led to the
evolutionary-biological perspective, is rarely emergence of our species, with the central features of
considered. Indeed, it is taken as a given that our that life history more or less taken for granted. Given
species’ lengthy period of juvenile dependency the field’s focus upon cross-species comparisons (e.g.,
evolved in order to facilitate brain development and chimpanzees’ faster development and shorter
learning and thereby afford successful functioning in lifespans), it is not surprising that little attention has
the highly varied physical environments and social been paid to within-species variation in life history.
contexts in which our ancestors resided (see Until perhaps two decades ago, the same was
Bjorklund & Pellegrini, 2002; Flinn, 2005; Geary, generally true of mainstream developmental-
2002; Kaplan et al. 2000). Not yet appreciated, psychological thinking as well.
however, is the competing claim that an extended Even though a variety of theoretical perspectives
(e.g., social learning theory, life-course theory,
Received 2006-06-30 attachment theory) and a huge research literature
Correspondence should be addressed to Jay Belsky, Institute for the Study address the determinants and sequelae of variation in
of Children, Families and Social Issues, Birkbeck University of London, 7 multiple elements of human life history (e.g., onset of
Bedford Square, London, WC1E 7HX, UK; e-mail: j.belsky@bbk.ac.uk.
454
3期 Jay Belsky. Experience in Childhood and the Development of Reproductive Strategies 455

sexual behaviour, parenting, marital/partner relations), childhood experiences more generally arise only
evolutionary thinking informs little of this work. when evolutionary “why” questions about ultimate
Indisputably, it is to a history of rewards and causation are posed.
punishments or to the child’s psychological Even though mainstream developmental
attachment to a parent or to the quality of important psychology and human development have not, for the
relationships in the child’s life (e.g., parent-child, most part, applied an evolutionary perspective when
marital, friendship) that students of human asking and answering questions about how
development routinely turn when seeking insight into experiences in childhood might shape development
developmental “outcomes” that evolutionary thinkers later in the life course, it is not the case that no such
would characterize as features of life history and efforts have been made in that direction. In this paper,
elements of reproductive strategy (e.g., age of first sex, one such programme of theory building dealing with
sexual “promiscuity”, quality of parenting). In many childhood experience and reproductive strategy, along
respects this is because students of child development, with some relevant evidence, will be reviewed. Before
whether trained in psychological, sociological or doing so, however, two foundational topics must be
cultural-anthropological traditions, have been--and addressed. The first is the proposition that experiences
remain—concerned principally with proximate early in life might shape developments later in life.
questions of how--How does development operate?-- The second is life-history theory, as this serves as the
rather than with ultimate questions of why: Why does higher-level framework on which this paper builds
development operate the way it seems to? (Ellis 2004; Ketelaar & Ellis 2000).
An excellent example of such an almost exclusive
focus upon proximate rather than ultimate influences I. The legacy of childhood experience
on development can be found, ironically, in four For most students of child development, it is
decades of research on the infant’s attachment to its presumed that experiences in the early years of life
(principal) caregiver, usually the mother. Even though influence individual differences in later development.
Bowlby (1969), perhaps the first modern practitioner This is not to say, however, that there is no debate
of evolutionary psychology and the theoretician who about the power of earlier experiences to shape later
coined the now well-worn phrase “environment of development (e.g., determining vs. contributing;
evolutionary adaptation (EEA), drew heavily upon contingent vs. inevitable), the timing of putatively
Darwin’s insights when formulating attachment influential experiences (e.g., first year of life, first 5
theory, his concern with ultimate causation, while years, prepubertal years), the nature of these
mentioned in passing in textbook discussions of experiences (e.g., prenatal exposure to alcohol,
infant-parent attachment, has been more or less quality of parenting, sib and/or peer relationships), the
abandoned entirely in developmental studies of means by which any such influence is exerted (e.g.,
individual differences in attachment, especially the direct vs. indirect) or the aspects of development
determinants and consequence of this first which are affected (e.g., intelligence, antisocial
relationship (but see Belsky 1997, 1999; Chisholm behaviour). Nevertheless, it remains the case that only
1996). rarely is the specific question already posed with
Although an abundance of evidence supports respect to attachment entertained more generally:
Bowlby’s (1969) thesis that secure attachment lays “Why should experiences early in life contribute to
the foundation for general well being (Thompson and forecast developments later in life?” In fact, in
1999), as well as Ainsworth’s (1973) postulate that recent social-policy-oriented discussions of early-
sensitive (vs. insensitive) mothering fosters secure (vs. experience effects on brain development (e.g., Schore
insecure) attachment (de Wolff & van Ijzendoorn 2001; Shonkoff & Phillips 2000), virtually no attempt
1997), developmentalists fascinated with the has been made to explain why natural selection would
attachment bond virtually never wonder why have crafted a developmental system whereby what
development operates this way rather than some other. transpires early in life, well before reproductive
Given the risk in the EEA of mother being physically maturity, would affect functioning later in the lifespan.
or emotionally compromised and/or even dying Yet almost three decades ago, Kagan et al. (1978)
during the infant’s first years of life, to say nothing of argued that the notion that early experiences affect
the inherent unpredictability of the future, would it long-term psychological/behavioural development
not have made more sense for nature to design a was more a (western) cultural myth than biological or
relationship system that was less—if at all—shaped psychological reality. This theme was taken up more
by the quality of care that mother provided or that recently by Bruer (1997) in challenging claims that
would exert less of a developmental impact on the human research illuminates early-experience effects
child than attachment theory presupposes? Clearly, on brain development and, thereby, provides a basis
such fundamental theoretical issues pertaining to the of crafting social policies. Relatedly, Lewis (1997)
developmental legacy of early attachment security or argued that concurrent experience is of far greater
456 心 理 学 报 39 卷

consequence to human functioning than experiences maternal care that shapes reproductive
and developments earlier in life. Finally, a seemingly potential (Clutton-Brock, 1991; Mousseau &
logical case could be made that it would make much Fox, 1988). In sum, although it should be
more sense for nature to shape human development so regarded as an empirical question whether any
as to be consistent with Lewis’s (1999) thesis kind of experience early in life influences some
emphasizing responsiveness to contemporary particular aspect of development later in life,
environmental input rather than to earlier life there seems to be good theoretical and
experiences. What should be clear by this point, then, empirical reason for entertaining the prospect
is that grounds most certainly exist for questioning that they could. This, of course, is the working
much of what will be the primary focus of this paper. assumption on which this paper is based.
But even when plausible arguments against early-
experience effects sound convincing, there remains II. Life-history theory
the matter of evidence. Perhaps most noteworthy is The beginning of this paper made clear that the
recent research showing that antenatal experiences, biological organisation of the human life course is
especially but not exclusively involving nutrition, something in need of explanation. Life-history theory
affect foetal growth and forecast physical health (LHT) is the metatheoretical framework within more
across the lifespan, including risk for developing (a) general evolutionary theory that seeks to account for
impaired glucose tolerance and reduced insulin the timing of reproductive and lifespan developments
sensitivity (Phillips 1998), (b) impaired immune in terms of evolved strategies for distributing
function (McDade et al. 2001), (c) coronary heart metabolic resources between the competing demands
disease (Robinson & Barker 2002) and (d) even of growth, maintenance and reproduction (Charnov
senescence (Sayer et al. 1998). If the very early 1993; Ellis 2004; Kuzawa 2005; McArthur 1967;
environment of the womb and foetal growth Stearns 1992; Wilson 1975). LHT describes an
systematically affect physical health over the ensuing individual’s total bioenergetic and material resources
decades of life—and in a manner likened to a as allocated between somatic effort (i.e., resources
“weather forecast” signalling the unborn child of a devoted to the continued survival) and reproductive
poorly nourished woman that it is about to enter a effort (i.e., resources devoted to producing-supporting
harsh world (Bateson et al. 2004)--why should offspring). Reproductive effort can itself be further
postnatal experience operate any differently on divided into mating effort (i.e., resources devoted to
psychological and behavioural phenotypes? After all, obtaining-retaining sexual partners) and parental
it is now well appreciated that brain development, to effort (i.e., resources devoted to enhancing offspring
say nothing of psychological and behavioural survival and quality). Life-history traits are the basic
development, continues well into adolescence and units of analysis in LHT and include, for example, age
even beyond (Giedd et al. 1999). at weaning, age at sexual maturity, adult body size,
Moreover, it would seem just as time to first reproduction, and litter size.
commonsensical to assume that rather than LHT provides a useful means of organising life-
leaving human functioning to the whim of history traits in a manner that highlights costs,
contemporary circumstances, nature would benefits and trade offs of different patterns of
have crafted an organism capable of modifying development. Such trade-offs are inevitable because
its phenotype in response to early environmental time and energy used for one purpose cannot be used
inputs provided those inputs afforded a for another. In a determinant growing species like
reasonable degree of prediction—in the EEA-- humans (i.e., cease growing at reproductive maturity),
about the future that the developing organism the decision of when to switch from investing energy
would encounter as it grew and developed. (For in growth to investing in reproduction is a classic
detailed consideration of this latter point, see example of such an adaptive trade off. So, too, is that
Belsky 2005a, b; Ellis 2004.) This argument between number and fitness of offspring. Even though
seems even more plausible the moment the there are benefits, reproductively speaking, of large
possibility is entertained that developmental adult size, risks are associated with delaying maturity
constraints imposed on the organism as it in order to grow large; this is because there is always
matured could restrict its future responsiveness the chance of dying before the reproductive benefits
to the environment (Boyce & Ellis 2005). In of large size are realised (Williams 1966). At the age
point of fact, many organisms other than when mortality risk associated with delaying maturity
humans are strikingly capable of altering their outweighs the reproductive benefits of growing larger,
development in response to their environment, LHT predicts that the organism will cease growth and
including environments encountered early in direct energy otherwise devoted to growth into
development (Kaplan & Lancaster 2003; West- supporting reproduction. These assumptions about the
Eberhard 2003) and in many cases it is costs and benefits of delaying maturity have been
3期 Jay Belsky. Experience in Childhood and the Development of Reproductive Strategies 457

used to predict the timing of reproductive maturity reproductive strategy. Thus, some individuals are
across species (Charnov 1993). observed to mature sooner than others, mate earlier
Natural selection should favour earlier reproduction, than others, establish less enduring pair bonds than
ceterus paribus, for several reasons (Ellis 2004). others, bear more offspring than others and invest less
Because the risk of death always exceeds zero, no intensively in them than others. Rather than speaking
matter what the time scale, producing offspring earlier in terms of r/K life histories (but see Rushton 1985),
in the lifespan lowers the chance of leaving no such variation is typically discussed in terms of
descendants. Derivatively, the benefits to fitness of quantity/quality reproductive strategies (Figueredo et
early reproduction should be greater the higher the al. 2006) or, when applied to men, in terms of
mortality risk (Chisholm 1999). In addition, earlier involved fathers who provide for their children (i.e.,
reproduction affords a longer period of reproduction, dads) or those who do not and spend their time
as onset and termination of reproduction (e.g., seeking additional mates and matings (i.e., cads)
menopause) are unrelated (Borgerhoff Mulder 1989; (Draper & Harpending 1982).
Peccei 2000). Finally, early reproduction increases 1. The role of heritability
total lineage reproduction, which of course is The reality of variation in human reproductive
important from the standpoint of inclusive fitness, due strategy raises the question of determinants. This will
to abbreviated generation times. be the principle focus of the remainder of this paper,
All this is not to say that delayed reproduction is with emphasis placed on childhood experience.
without benefit. In fact, competing selection pressures Because individual differences in virtually every
favour delayed reproduction: Slower developing measurable psychological and behavioural trait has
organisms usually attain larger adult body size which been shown to be at least partly heritable (Bouchard
itself is generally related to lower risk of death, 2004), including core life-history traits like pubertal
greater energy production and stores to channel to timing (e.g., Rowe 2002, Treloar & Martin 1990), the
reproduction across the lifespan and, perhaps most role of the genotype merits consideration as well. But
importantly, increased chances of succeeding in before a simple genetic explanation of variation in
intrasexual competition for mates (Charnov 1993; reproductive strategy is embraced, three things must
Clutton-Brock, 1991; Hill & Kaplan 1999). It should be acknowledged.
be clear, then, that no one means of organising life- First, even though variation in the timing of
history traits is likely to maximize inclusive fitness menarche has been linked to an androgen-receptor
across all species and ecological niches. gene (Comings et al. 2002), an oestrogen receptor
gene (Stavrou et al. 2002) and still other genes
A. Variation in human reproductive strategy (Kadlubar et al. 2003), a first effort to replicate
Two general life histories—r and K-- reflecting the Comings et al.’s molecular-genetic results in two
co-occurrence of a suite of life-history traits are often epidemiological studies using samples drawn from the
discussed when cross-species variation is considered general population failed to do so (Jorm et al. 2004).
(Pianka 1970), even if there is some sense that these This is not an uncommon result in studies
may mask as much variation as they presume to endeavouring to link specific genes to development
illuminate and thus are somewhat dated. K-selected (Caspi et al. 2002; Kim-Cohen, Caspi, Taylor,
species are relatively long lived; spend a considerable Williams, Newcombe, Craig & Moffitt, 2006).
portion of that lifespan as juveniles; and tend to bear Second, when Rowe (2000) specifically sought to
and rear relatively few young while investing heavily disconfirm Belsky, Steinberg and Draper’s (1991)
in them (e.g., elephants). In contrast, r-selected developmental theory of reproductive strategies
species generally follow the opposite pattern—short discussed below (and hereafter referred to as BSD
lifespan, brief juvenile period, heightened fecundity, theory) in a behaviour-genetic study, he proved
and limited parental investment (e.g., rabbits). unable to administer a “knock-out punch”, as some
Although humans obviously qualify more as a K- than evidence of environmental influence emerged (see
an r-selected species, r/K thinking that has typically also Ellis, 2004, p. 922). Moreover, as Belsky et al.
been used to characterise and explain cross-species (1991) speculated, the secular trend in pubertal timing
variation in life histories has been applied to within- that has reduced the age of reproductive maturation
species variation (Stearns & Koella 1986), including over the past 150 years in the western world and is
aboriginal human populations (Hill & Hurtado 1996; largely attributed to improvements in nutrition and
see also Rushton 1985). Indeed, it was within this hygiene, may well have attenuated a great deal of
context, though limited to considerations of variation which had, in ancestral times and beyond,
biogeography, that MacAthur and Wilson (1968) been more susceptible to environmental influence
originally discussed r/K. than may appear to be the case in the modern world
When humans become the focus of concern, today (see also, Ellis 2004, p. 922).
terminology often shifts to discussions of
458 心 理 学 报 39 卷

Third, Belsky (2000; 2005a,b) argued that rather III. Childhood experience and reproductive
than thinking in terms of genetic or environmental strategy
effects, it might make the most sense to think in terms In a seminal paper that would eventually stimulate
of nature and nurture in the case of reproductive a (somewhat delayed) cascade of theoretical
strategies, because for some individuals such suites of developments and empirical studies over the past
life-history traits may be “born” (i.e., fixed strategists) decade and a half pertaining not just to variation in
whereas for others they may be “made” (i.e., plastic human reproductive strategies but, more importantly,
strategists). Within the context of behaviour-genetic to the role of childhood experiences in shaping it,
research designs, any such “differential susceptibility” Draper and Harpending (1982) argued that girls
to developmental experiences would attenuate shared growing up in father-present and father-absent homes
environmental effects, because these require different pursue distinctive reproductive strategies. Whereas
children in the same family to be affected in exactly father-absent girls develop behaviour profiles
the same manner and to the very same degree by consistent with an expectation that paternal
family experiences. Drawing on evolutionary bet- investment in childrearing will not be forthcoming
hedging logic, Belsky (2000, 2005a,b) further argued and that pair bonds will not be enduring, those from
that such differential susceptibility would make father-present households develop as if anticipating
biological sense—to parents, children and siblings— the opposite, deferring sexual activity once they reach
because efforts by parents to shape their children’s biological maturity while seeking to establish and
development could prove misguided, especially from maintain enduring, close, heterosexual relationships.
the standpoint of enhancing reproductive fitness, What was unique to the Draper and Harpending (1982)
given the inherent unpredictability of the future (see argument, especially from the standpoint of traditional
also Ellis, Jackson & Boyce, 2006; Flinn, 2006). For theories of child development, was the casting of
this reason, it would make evolutionary-biological early-experience influences within the family in
sense for children to vary in the extent to which evolutionary terms emphasizing reproductive fitness,
parental efforts to guide their development proved parental investment, pair bonds, and reproductive
successful (see also Boyce & Ellis 2005). strategy. Gone was any moral approbation about
Certainly not inconsistent with the notion of “problem” behaviour and in its place were the
differential susceptibility to rearing influence is the potential reproductive-fitness benefits of varying
ubiquity of nonshared, as opposed to shared, mating and parenting behaviour (i.e., reproductive
environmental effects in behaviour-genetic studies of strategy) to fit the ecological context.
development (Bouchard 2004; Turkheimer & Two things were lacking in this most provocative
Waldron 2000); these are effects indicating that and original paper, at least from the perspective of
shared family experiences (e.g., divorce) affect students of child development. First, no
different children growing up in the same household developmental process was offered to explain how the
differently. Also pointing in the same direction (i.e., particular childhood experience in question (i.e.,
toward differential susceptibility) is recent molecular- father absence) would shape later functioning in
genetic evidence showing that the anticipated effect of adolescence and adulthood. And second, although the
child maltreatment in promoting antisocial behaviour Draper and Harpending (1982) argument cast old data
in adulthood varies as a function of an individual’s linking father-absence in childhood with sexual,
genotype (Caspi et al. 2002). mating and parenting behaviour in adulthood in new
In sum, although it would be foolish to argue that theoretical terms, it failed to generate any new
heritability plays no (or even just a marginal) role in predictions. Was it more, then, than just old wine in a
human reproductive strategy, there seems no grounds new bottle? Why, in fact, embrace evolutionary
for concluding at the current time that developmental theorising about “reproductive strategy” when a
programmes are not at least somewhat malleable with myriad of widely-acknowledged theoretical
respect to life-history traits, though this may be more perspectives dating back to Freud himself already
true for some individuals than for others and less so in offered accounts of why the later-life developments
the present than in the past. Indeed, as Ellis (2004, addressed by Draper and Harpending (1982) would
p.925) recently observed, any number of scholars result from father absence in childhood?
have argued that “selection can be expected to favour
adaptive developmental plasticity of mechanisms A. Belsky, Steinberg & Draper’s (1991) (BSD)
(within genetic capacities and constraints) in response critical prediction: Pubertal timing
to particular ecological conditions….” Attention is Considered reflection on these limitations led
now turned to theory development and some evidence Belsky et al. (1991) to advance “an evolutionary
pertaining to humans on just this score. theory of socialization” (i.e., BSD theory) linking
childhood experience, interpersonal orientation and
reproductive strategy, building directly on the insights
3期 Jay Belsky. Experience in Childhood and the Development of Reproductive Strategies 459

of Draper and Harpending (1982). Central to BSD probabilistically, to sensitive, supportive, responsive
theory was the thesis that stressful and supportive and positively affectionate styles of mothering and
extra-familial environments influenced family fathering and, thereby, secure attachments, a trusting
dynamics, most especially parent-child and internal working model and a reciprocally-rewarding
marital/pair-bond relations, thereby shaping children’s interpersonal orientation. Collectively, these
early emotional and behavioural development and, developments would delay pubertal maturation
through it, subsequent social development, including (within the range of reaction) and defer the onset of
sexual/mating behaviour, pair bonding and parenting. sexual activity while fostering enduring pair bonds
Moreover, BSD argued, this complex and and greater parental investment.
environmentally sensitive developmental system As BSD made clear at the time, a great deal of
evolved as a means of fitting the organism to its traditional developmental research provided evidence
environment in the service of promoting reproductive that stressful rearing milieus, whether conceptualized
fitness (i.e., not psychological well being). in demographic terms (e.g., low income, lone
Of central importance to the BSD theory was the parenthood), relationships terms (e.g.,
view that parenting, the parent-child relationship and, harsh/neglecting parenting; marital conflict, divorce)
in particular, the attachment relationship mediated the or psychological terms (e.g., depressed mother,
influence of stressors and supports external to the insecure attachment), predict developmental
parent-child relationship on the child’s general “outcomes” that are regarded by mainstream child
trustful-mistrustful outlook on the world and developmentalists—and many others—as
opportunistic vs. mutually-beneficial orientation “unfavourable” and certainly not “optimal.” These
toward others, as well as his/her behaviour. But what included, among other things, precocious and
fundamentally distinguished BSD from all other promiscuous sexual behaviour; aggressive/antisocial
theories of, or perspectives on, early experience and behaviour, depression, relationship instability and
human development was the explicitly-labelled unsupportive, if not harsh parenting. The opposite
“uncanny prediction” that these developmental tends to be true of rearing environments that are well
experiences and psychological orientations would resourced and emotionally and relationally supportive
influence somatic development by affecting the (e.g., Bradley & Caldwell 1988; Cicchetti & Carlon
timing of puberty; and that this cascade of 1989; Emery 1988; McLoyd 1990; Patterson 1986;
developments shaped, in adolescence and adulthood, Pettit & Bates 1989). In the decade and a half since
sexual behaviour, pair-bond orientation and parenting. BSD advanced their theory, the publication of
More specifically, while noting that it remained evidence highlighting such environmental effects has
unclear whether environmental processes and the continued unabated (e.g., Amato 2001; Belsky &
development of reproductive strategies should be Fearon 2002; Buehler & Gerard 2002; Parke et al.
conceptualized dimensionally or typologically (i.e., 2004; Seccombe 2000).
continuous vs. discrete phenotypic plasticity), BSD As already noted, what made BSD distinctive—and
posited two distinctive developmental trajectories for purposefully so—was the hypothesis that social-
purposes of presentation. A quantity-oriented developmental experiences within the family would
reproductive strategy was most likely to arise, BSD influence the timing of sexual maturation (i.e.,
argued, in the context of a variety of stressors which puberty). Because this is a core life-history variable
would undermine parental well being and family and because it is a feature of development that no
relationships, including general stress, marital discord other theory of, or perspective on, human
and/or inadequate financial resources. These forces development suggests would be affected by social-
would, probabilistically, give rise to harsh, rejecting, developmental experiences in the family, it
insensitive and/or inconsistent parenting, which highlighted the potential “added value” of an
would foster insecure attachment, a mistrustful evolutionary approach to human development.
internal working model and an opportunistic, As it turns out, a good deal of evidence has
advantage-taking interpersonal orientation. These emerged since BSD promulgated their “uncanny”
developments would stimulate an earlier timing of hypothesis that, at the least, is not inconsistent with it.
puberty than otherwise would be the case (i.e., within First, greater parent-child warmth, cohesion and
the organism’s range of reaction) and an earlier onset positivity predict later pubertal development--in both
of sexual activity, short-term and unstable pair bonds, prospective longitudinal studies (Ellis et al. 1999;
and limited parental investment. Graber et al. 1995; Steinberg 1988) and retrospective
The alternative, quality-oriented developmental or concurrent ones (Kim & Smith 1988a; Kim et al.
trajectory was fostered by exposure to a supportive 1997; Miller & Pasta 2000; Romans et al. 2003; Rowe
rearing environment, characterized by spousal 2000). Second, greater parent-child conflict and
harmony and adequate financial resources. These coercion predict earlier timing of puberty, again in
ecological foundations would give rise, again both prospective longitudinal work (Moffitt et al.
460 心 理 学 报 39 卷

1992) and in research adopting retrospective or B. The distinctive influence of the father
concurrent-assessment designs (Jorm et al. 2004; Kim From an empirical perspective, Bruce Ellis has
& Smith 1988a,b; Kim et al. 1997; Mezzich et al. undertaken the most systemic research endeavouring
1997; Weirson et al. 1993). Finally, and with respect to test propositions derived from BSD theory,
to marital/partner relations, the happier and/or less especially those dealing with its theory-defining and
conflicted the relationship between mother and father, distinctive pubertal-timing prediction. Notably, Ellis
the more delayed pubertal maturation—in both and associates have tracked girls from early childhood
prospective-longitudinal studies (Ellis et al. 1999; into adolescence in a series of investigations (Ellis et
Ellis & Garber 2000) and investigations adopting al. 2003; Ellis & Garber 2000; Ellis et al. 1999). One
weaker research designs in which predictor and important result of this work and Ellis’ (2004) review
outcome data are gathered at the same time and/or of pubertal-timing research has been to highlight the
retrospectively (Kim & Smith 1998b; Kim et al. 1997; potentially unique influence of the father-child
Romans et al. 2003). relationship and stepfather presence (Ellis 2004).
It is important to note that virtually all the relevant Whereas Draper and Harpending (1982) exclusively
findings come from studies of girls, basically due to addressed the role of father absence during childhood
measurement difficulties in demarcating pubertal in shaping reproductive strategy, BSD expanded upon
development in boys. Of importance as well is that their model, arguing that father absence was an
while there is a good deal of data linking quality of indicator of a stressful family environment and that
parent-child and marital relations with pubertal timing, Draper and Harpending’s (1982) narrow
not all tests derived from BSD theory have supported conceptualisation of the influential rearing milieu
it. Ellis et al. (1999), Miller and Pasta (2000) and could be expanded to consider a larger set of stressors
Steinberg (1988), for example, failed to document the and supports which contribute to the development of
hypothesized linkage between family conflict and reproductive strategy. Thus, BSD called attention to
coercion and accelerated pubertal timing. attachment security/insecurity, parental sensitivity/
Nevertheless, when considered in their entirety, the insensitivity, harsh vs. warm parenting and
results of the work cited above led Ellis (2004, pp. harmonious vs. conflicted pair bonds, drawing no
935-936) to conclude in a recent and comprehensive particular distinction between contributions of
review of research on the determinants of pubertal mothers and fathers in shaping offspring reproductive
timing that “empirical research has provided strategy.
reasonable, though incomplete” support for BSD Upon repeatedly detecting effects of father
theory. presence vs. absence and even unique effects of the
That said, it must be acknowledged that with rare quality of fathering, father-daughter relationships and
exception (e.g., Figueredo et al., 2006; Rowe 2000), the presence of a stepfather on pubertal timing (see
relevant work has not been positioned to discount the below), Ellis et al. (1999, 2003; Ellis & Garber 2000)
possibility that common genes account both for why concluded that fathers may have a special role to play
some families function the way they do and why in the development of girl’s reproductive strategies, or
children in the family mature at the rate they do. In at least their pubertal development. In fact, rather than
fact, one previously cited molecular-genetic inquiry being a marker of stress, father absence and stepfather
found that associations like those under consideration presence operate as a paternal-investment cues
might reflect little more than genetic effects, as an X- indicative of low-quality paternal investment: “girls
linked androgen receptor GGC-repeat polymorphism detect and internally encode information specifically
being associated with parental divorce, father absence about the quality of paternal investment during
and earlier age of menarche (Comings et al. 2002). approximately the first 5 years of life as a basis of
The fact, however, that Jorm et al.’s (2004) calibrating …the timing of pubertal maturation and
aforementioned effort to replicate such results failed certain types of sexual behaviour” (Ellis 2004, p. 938).
to do so cautions against prematurely embracing the Such processes should be understood in the broader
conclusion that genetic effects are masquerading in context of human evolution, argued Ellis (2004); not
all-too-much of the cited work as environmental only are humans the only great ape in which males
effects of rearing experiences on pubertal timing. engage in provisioning or care of offspring, but such
Moreover, even if it ultimately proves to be the case paternal investment is highly variable, with the
that common genes play a role both in shaping both contribution of fathers to the family determined by the
putative causes (e.g., father absence, parental divorce) trade off between paternal and mating investment in
and consequences (e.g., age of menarche), this does the service of fitness goals (Geary, 2005).
not mean, when considered from the standpoint of Rather consistently, father absence has been related
developmental process, that experience plays no role to accelerated pubertal development in girls,
whatsoever in the causal process. demarcated either in terms of age of menarche or the
development of secondary sexual characteristics; and
3期 Jay Belsky. Experience in Childhood and the Development of Reproductive Strategies 461

this is so across rather diverse studies, including mother and father as interchangeable agents of
prospective inquiries following girls from childhood influence when it comes to understanding how
into adolescence (Campbell & Udry 1995; Ellis & childhood experiences may shape reproductive
Garber 2000; Ellis et al. 1999; Hetherington & Kelly strategy and for paying especial attention to the
2002; Moffitt et al. 1992; Rowe 2000; Wierson et al. presence of a biologically unrelated male figure in the
1993) and retrospective research using adult samples home of a prepubescent girl.
(Doughty & Rodgers 2000; Hoier 2003; Jones et al.
1972; Jorm et al. 2004; Kiernan & Hobcraft 1997; C. Mortality rate, time preference and attachment
Quinlan 2003; Romans et al. 2003; Surbey 1990). Not long after BSD extended Draper and
Although effects of father absence have emerged in Harpending’s (1982) thinking about the role of
work conducted in a variety of western countries, it is developmental experience in shaping human
noteworthy that they have not been detected in reproductive strategies, Chisholm (1993, 1996, 1999)
African American samples (Campbell & Udry 1995; further developed this line of theorising about the
Rowe 2000); Ellis (2004, p. 940) suggests this could human life course; and he did so in three specific
be the result of the “extraordinary secular trend” in ways, each of which is considered in turn.
this population. (The “secular trend” refers to the 1. Local mortality rates.
dramatic acceleration in the timing of pubertal First, whereas BSD highlighted economic and
maturation that has occurred over the past 150 years marital resources, or the absence thereof, as forces
throughout the western world and which is typically shaping parenting, attachment and thereby nascent
attributed to improved nutrition.) reproductive strategies, Chisholm (1999) called
There is evidence to suggest that both timing of attention, following Stearn (1992), to the importance
father absence and stepfather presence are important of local mortality rates. Such information, he argued,
to understanding effects of an absent father. The afforded organisms unconscious if not conscious
earlier in the child’s life that father absence occurs, insight into the relative risk and uncertainty of the
perhaps especially in the first 5 years of life, the more developing child surviving until maturity to bear its
potent an impact it appears to have on female pubertal own offspring. Furthermore, as already noted,
development (Jones et al. 1972; Ellis 2004; Ellis & initiating reproduction earlier rather than later in life
Garber 2000; Quinlan 2003; Surbey 1990). The makes especially good biological sense when the risk
conditions under which stepfather presence exerts its of dying before reproducing is high or, probably more
influence may also be important, perhaps even importantly, perceived to be high.
accounting for effects of father absence (Ellis 2004). Several notable findings appear consistent with
Given what has already been stated about relationship Chisholm’s (1999) theorising, though the research in
quality more generally and father absence in question was not carried out in direct response to it.
particular, it should not be surprising that conflicted First, Wilson and Daly (1997) found that as life
relations between mother and stepfather and early expectancy declined across Chicago neighbourhoods,
onset of stepfather presence have been found to be the probability of a woman reproducing by age 30
particularly influential in accelerating pubertal increased. Similarly, Johns (2003) found that teen
development in girls (Ellis & Garber 2000). mothers in Gloucestershire (in the United Kingdom)
Discussion of father absence and stepfather expected to die at younger ages than did women who
presence should not distract from the fact that became mothers after their teenage years. Such results
biological fathers seem to matter, too. Consistent with not only accord with Geronimus’ (1996) qualitative
BSD’s original emphasis on the quality of parent- interviews with poor, African-American teen mothers
child relationships, Ellis et al. (1999) found that the which reveal their awareness of their risks for an early
more time such men spent caring for their daughters death, but also with her “weathering hypothesis”
across the child’s first five years of life and the more suggesting that early birth is a strategic response to
they engaged in positive interaction with their the rapid decline in health of these women in their
daughters at age five, the more delayed were girls’ third and fourth decade of life. Finally, extending the
pubertal development when followed up around 12 line of inquiry to parenting and the child’s
years of age. Given all the evidence considered development, meta-analytic research on attachment
through this point regarding relationship influences on shows that when a mother experiences the loss of a
(female) pubertal development, the apparently loved one through death sometime in her life and this
important role which males in the family play in loss remains emotionally “unresolved”, the likelihood
influencing girls’ pubertal maturation should not be of her own offspring developing a type of insecure
read to imply that mothers and marriages are attachment increases, namely, disorganised
unimportant. What the work of Ellis and others attachment which is defined by the absence of an
clearly indicates, however, is that there are strong organised strategy for co-regulating emotion with the
empirical and theoretical grounds for not treating parent and often involves tendencies to
462 心 理 学 报 39 卷

simultaneously or sequentially avoid and approach the resistant attachment, which reflects a strategy of
attachment figure (vanIJzendoorn 1995). Considered exaggerating emotional neediness to evoke care and
together, these illustrative results highlight the value support, he speculated, arose in reaction to a parent’s
in considering local mortality rates as an important inability to invest, whereas insecure-avoidant
feature of the broader ecology shaping reproductive attachment, which reflects a strategy of dampening
strategy. communications of emotional need, derived from a
2. Time preference. parent’s unwillingness to invest. In a not unrelated
Whereas BSD highlighted the role of interpersonal vein, Belsky (1997, 1999) further refined thinking
orientation, behavioural development and pubertal about the opportunistic-advantage-taking
timing in mediating the effect of rearing environment, interpersonal orientation central to the BSD typology,
parenting and attachment security on future mating suggesting that while it was most likely promoted by
and parenting, a second contribution of Chisholm insecure-avoidant attachment, insecure-resistance
(1999, p. 135) was to call attention to an additional may have evolved to promote helper-at-the-nest type
psychological mediator linking childhood experience behaviour and thus foster in the child emotional and
and reproductive strategy, time preference: “Time behavioural dependency on the mother well beyond
preference is the degree to which people prefer to or the infancy years.
believe they will achieve their desires (i.e., benefits or To date, a rather large body of evidence
consequences of action) now, more-or-less highlighting the potential role of attachment security
immediately, or later, at some point in the future.” in predicting features of reproductive strategy has
Theoretically, individuals living in highly risky and emerged, though rarely has it been cast in such life-
uncertain environments in which waiting for a reward history terms or has it been stimulated by
might prove to be a “fool’s errand” should opt for evolutionary thinking. Two different sets of findings
immediate payoffs even when delayed ones would be merit consideration, one linking adult attachment with
greater (Wilson & Daly, 2005). In such circumstances, sexual behaviour, mating and pair bonding and the
they are presumed to be hedging their bets against the other linking adult attachment with parenting. Before
risk that they may not be around to collect the larger summarising relevant results of this work, it must be
reward. Here, of course, payoff and reward refer to noted that very little of the research actually tracks
the likelihood of reproducing. children prospectively from childhood into adulthood.
According to Chisholm (1999), then, time With this important caveat in mind, it remains of
preference should be regarded as an evolutionary theoretical significance that self assessments of
important psychological construct that is sensitive to attachment security in the context of romantic
rearing experience and influences future reproductive relationships, presumed to be shaped at least in part
functioning, broadly conceived (i.e., mating, by rearing history, systematically relate to a variety of
parenting). Evidence that children growing up in more aspects of sexual behaviour, mating and pair bonding
economically-, socially- and psychologically- in a manner consistent with BSD theorising. With
disadvantaged families have a more difficult time respect to sexual attitudes and behaviour, individuals
waiting to secure a more attractive reward and are self-classified as secure are less likely to endorse
more inclined to settle for a lesser reward sooner (i.e., promiscuous sexual behaviour (Brennan et al. 1998)
difficulty delaying gratification) would seem or to engage in one-night stands or extra-pair sexual
consistent with Chisholm’s argument about influences liaisons (Brennan & Shaver 1995; Hazan et al. 1994,
on time preference (e.g., Evans & English 2002; as cited in Kirkpatrick 1998). One study, in fact,
Lengua 2002). showed that over a hypothetical 30-year period, males
3. Attachment styles. and females with secure attachment orientation
A third notable contribution that Chisholm (1996) ideally desired only one romantic partner (Miller &
made to thinking about developmental influences on Fishkin 1997), less than those with insecure
human reproductive strategies involved his orientations. Related work further indicates that in the
elaboration of the role that BSD attributed to case of females, attachment security is associated with
attachment security in entraining the development of an older age of first sexual intercourse (Bogaert &
the most appropriate alternative reproductive Sadava 2002).
strategies. In addition to reiterating the influential role Turning to consideration of pair bonding and
BSD and attachment theory more generally accord relationship processes, one consistent finding
warm-sensitive-responsive parenting in shaping pertinent to this analysis of attachment and
attachment security and, thereby, the child’s reproductive strategy is that self-reported relationship
orientation toward others and relationships, Chisholm satisfaction is greater when individuals describe
(1996) distinguished two different manifestations of themselves as secure rather than dismissing (i.e., the
insensitive parenting while theorising that each had adult form of insecure-avoidant) or preoccupied (i.e.,
distinctive developmental consequences. Insecure- the adult from of insecure-resistant) (e.g., Davilia et al.
3期 Jay Belsky. Experience in Childhood and the Development of Reproductive Strategies 463

1999; Feeney 2000; Milkulincer et al. 2002). early childhood (Fraley 2002; Hamilton 2000;Waters
Moreover, higher scores on dismissing-avoidant et al. 2000).
attachment predict lower levels of marital quality, With respect to the parental investment component
both reported and observed (Shaver & Mikulincer of reproductive strategy, evidence indicates that
2002). Observational research also indicates that parents classified as autonomous-secure and thus
secure partners manifest less negative affect, less presumed (but not demonstrated) in the relevant
avoidant nonverbal behaviour, and more constructive studies to have experienced supportive rearing
conversation patterns in response to their partners’ environments while growing up, parent their offspring
distancing behaviour (e.g., Feeney 1998; Rholes et al. in a more supportive, sensitive manner. Such a
1998). These findings are consistent with related parenting style is indicative of higher parental
results showing that attachment security predicts investment than that rendered by parents who
greater communication levels within close manifest insecure internal working models of
relationships in adulthood (Collins & Read 1990), attachment in adulthood. More specifically, security
including greater self-disclosure to the romantic has been linked to more warmth and appropriate
partner and responsiveness to the partner’s self structuring of the child’s learning environment for
disclosure (Collins & Read 1990; Kobak & Hazan both mothers and fathers (Adam, Gunnar & Tanaka,
1991; Mikulincer & Nachson 1991). Such findings 2004; Cohn et al. 1992) and to greater provision by
can be meaningfully interpreted as reflecting the BSD mothers of emotional support in a variety of contexts
view that security promotes a mutually-beneficial (Crowell & Feldman 1988, 1991), less negativity
interpersonal orientation (as opposed to an (Slade et al. 1999; Adam et al., 2004), along with
opportunistic-advantage-taking one). greater sensitivity to the child’s needs and states (Das
In light of the findings just summarised and the Eiden et al. 1995). In sum, then, not only is adult
interpretation offered, it seems almost attachment security related to mating and pair
commonsensical that secure individuals prove less bonding processes in social-psychological research, it
likely to get divorced or separated from their romantic is also related to presumptive indices of parental
partners (e.g., Hazan & Shaver 1987; Kirkpatrick & investment in developmental research.
Hazan 1994); have longer lasting relationships (Hazan Work on the intergenerational transmission of
& Shaver 1987; Kirkpatrick & Davis 1994; parenting that is not carried out within an attachment
Kirkpatrick & Hazan 1994); and manifest greater tradition is also of relevance to this discussion, as a
levels of commitment to and trust of their partners, central idea underpinning BSD thinking is that
irrespective of whether they are dating (Brennan & childhood rearing experiences shape psychological,
Shaver 1995; Levy & Davis 1988; Pistole 1989; behavioural and even somatic development, including
Pistole & Clark 1995; Simpson 1990) or married attachment security. Moreover, these effects are
(Feeney 1994; Feeney et al. 1994; Fuller & Fincham themselves considered to influence parental
1995; Kobak & Hazan 1991). In sum, the data suggest, investment when children become parents themselves.
consistent with evolutionary theorising (Belsky et al. Studies pertaining to the intergenerational
1991; Belsky 1997; Chisholm 1996; 1999) and transmission of parenting typically focus upon child
attachment theory more generally, that attachment abuse and neglect by means of retrospective designs
orientation in adulthood is systematically related to or involve prospective follow ups in young adulthood
sexual behaviour, mating and pair bonding processes. of teenagers at risk for criminal careers (for reviews,
What about parental investment? When it comes to see Belsky & Jaffee, in press; Serbin & Karp 2003).
linking attachment in adulthood with this aspect of Consistent with the results of such research, there is
reproductive strategy, investigators stand on evidence from prospective studies beginning in
somewhat firmer ground, basically because the childhood that harsh, inconsistent and unsupportive
methodology used for measuring adult attachment in parenting (Caspi & Elder 1988), as well as sensitive-
this body of work, the Adult Attachment Interview warm-stimulating parenting (Belsky et al. 2005), is
(George et al. 1985), has been found to capture transmitted across generations. Importantly, as in the
variation that is itself predicted by attachment work on the transmission of child maltreatment across
measured in the opening years of life in prospective generations, continuity is by no means inevitable,
longitudinal research (Hamilton 2000; Waters et al. underscoring the important point that developments
2000). It would be mistaken to conclude, however, later in life are a function of both early and
that continuity inevitably characterises the subsequent experience. Perhaps as important as that
developmental process with respect to attachment observation is that the intergenerational transmission
security, as such continuity has not always been of harsh parenting seems to be mediated by antisocial
detected (Lewis et al. 2000; Weinfield et al. 2000) or and problematic child behaviour which can certainly
has been found to be contingent on events and be conceptualised, within the BSD framework, as a
experiences transpiring—or not transpiring—after form of opportunistic-advantage taking (e.g., Capaldi
464 心 理 学 报 39 卷

et al. 2003; Conger et al. 2003; Thornberry et al. does and does not show? The answer to this question
2003). would have to be “no” were the research carried out
in the same manner as the six studies underpinning
IV. Conclusion Ellis’ (2004) conclusion. This is because neither these
Even though a great deal of mainstream studies nor Ellis (2004) takes account of a critically
developmental psychology is devoted to important qualification of BSD’s evolutionary theory
understanding whether and how experiences in of socialisation.
childhood shape psychological and behavioural Even though Belsky et al. (1991) presented their
development later in life, little theoretical attention developmental-trajectories’ model of reproductive
has been paid to why such cross-time influences strategies as a linearly sequenced one with one event
should characterize human development. This is or process (e.g., attachment security) contributing to
especially true with respect to the well-studied the one immediately subsequent to it (e.g.,
determinants of mating, pair bonding and parenting. interpersonal orientation) in a simple and linear chain
Theoretically, Draper and Harpending (1982), Belsky of causation (i.e., A B C D…), they explicitly
et al. (1991), Ellis (2004) and Chisholm (1996) have raised the prospect that an alternative model might
all addressed this lacunae, stimulating research on more accurately capture the complex dynamics of
linkages between childhood experience and human development. None of the six studies cited by
reproductive strategy. Concern for experiential effects Ellis (2004) considered this alternative model, nor did
on pubertal timing distinguishes this line of inquiry Ellis (2004) himself.
from more traditional developmental studies because, The alternative model offered by Belsky et al.
as already noted, an evolutionary perspective suggests (1991) was a conditional probability one. Instead of
that experiences in the family might affect somatic any link in the causal developmental chain being
development. Fifteen years since BSD advanced their solely or even principally a function of the
“uncanny” prediction, it seems clear that this pubertal immediately preceding link (i.e., A B, B C, C D),
timing, at least in females, is related to selected or even an earlier link (i.e., A C, B D), each
aspects of early family experience (Ellis, 2004). development in the sequence could be a function of
BSD theorised that this would be the case because the co-occurrence of multiple previous links in the
pubertal timing more or less mediates linkages chain of causation (i.e., A × B C; B × C D).
between experiences in childhood and reproductive Thus, even though pubertal timing might not, in and
functioning later in development. Ellis (2004, p. 947) of itself, directly predict the mating, pair bonding
recently questioned this proposition, arguing that (a) and/or parenting outcomes that Ellis (2004) regards as
while family experiences do predict pubertal timing critical for substantiating BSD thinking, it might
(in girls) and, independently, other reproductive- nevertheless do so in interaction with developments
strategy-relevant outcomes, (b) pubertal timing does prior to puberty (e.g., attachment insecurity,
not itself predict important features of reproductive opportunistic interpersonal orientation) or thereafter
strategy that BSD stipulates it should. Most critically, (e.g., early onset of sexual activity).
he commented, “although earlier timing of puberty In fact, the latter and highly likely possibility would
clearly predicts earlier onset of major forms of sexual be perfectly consistent with Sroufe’s (1988) argument
experience and reproduction”-- meaning age at first that development is a function of both early and more
dating, kissing, petting, and engaging in sexual contemporaneous experience. Were it to be the case,
intercourse, as well as increased rates of teenage then, that pubertal timing influences mating, pair
pregnancy and even first birth in natural fertility bonding and parenting principally in interaction with
populations—“there is currently no empirical basis other factors and forces, assessments of only direct
for the hypothesis that earlier timing of puberty leads effects of pubertal timing on these features of
to a more unrestricted sociosexual orientation, reproductive strategy--such as those examined in the
unstable pairbonds, greater number of sexual partners, six available studies-- would simply not be in position
or lower parental investment.” With only six studies to detect its impact on reproductively strategic
addressing the latter issues, none of which behaviour.
longitudinally follow individuals from childhood, All this is not to say that Ellis’ (2004) alternative
Ellis (2004) appropriately acknowledged that “more model of evolutionarily adaptive developmental
research is needed.” pathways does not merit serious consideration.
But would there still be grounds for casting aside According to his “child development” theory of
BSD thinking in exchange for an alternative model of pubertal timing, even though family experiences
adaptive human development offered by Ellis (2004) shape age of menarche and, independently, adult
(see below) even if the data base were larger and reproductive strategy (e.g., pair bonding, parental
remained consistent with Ellis’s (2004) analysis of investment), pubertal timing does not mediate the
what the currently available evidence (i.e., six studies) linkage between childhood experience and adult
3期 Jay Belsky. Experience in Childhood and the Development of Reproductive Strategies 465

reproductive strategy, as postulated by BSD. And this influences on human fertility and sexuality: Theoretical and
empirical contributions from the biological and behavioural
is because, as already noted, pubertal timing has not sciences (pp. 127-145). Boston: Kluwer Academic.
(yet) been found to predict “qualitative aspects of Belsky, J. (2005a). The developmental and evolutionary psychology of
mating and parenting strategies, independent of age at intergenerational transmission of attachment. In C. S. Carter, L.
Ahnert, K. Grossman, S. Hrdy, M. Lamb, S. Porges, & N. Sascher
onset of sexual and reproductive events” (Ellis 2004, (Eds). Attachment and bonding: A new synthesis (pp. 169-198).
p. 48, emphasis added). Although it remains unclear Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
why pubertal timing must operate in such a way (i.e., Belsky, J. (2005b). Differential susceptibility to rearing influence. In
B.J. Ellis & D.F. Bjorklund (Eds), Origins of the social mind:
independent of age of first sex) to be legitimately Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 139-163).
conceived as part and parcel of the development of Guilford Press, New York.
reproductive strategy, Ellis (2004) provocatively Belsky, J. & Fearon, R. M. P (2002). Infant-mother attachment security,
argues that childhood experiences influence the contextual risk and early development. Development and
Psychopathology, 14, 293-310.
duration of childhood, serving to abbreviate it (via Belsky, J. & Jaffee, S. (in press). The multiple determinants of
accelerated pubertal timing) when family social parenting. In D. Cicchetti & .D Cohen (Eds), Developmental
resources are limited and extend it (via delayed Psychopathology (2nd edition). New York: Wiley.
Belsky, J., Jaffee, S., Sligo, J., Woodward, L., & Silva, P. (2005).
maturation) when family resources are relatively Intergenerational transmission of warm-sensitive-stimulating
abundant. Under the former conditions, he theorises, parenting. Child Development, 76, 384-396.
nature has designed development to move the child Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience,
interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy. Child
out of the family and into the world of peers when the Development, 62, 647-670.
family’s capacity to enhance the child’s competitive Bjorklund, D., & Pellegrini, A. (2002). The origins of human nature:
advantage is limited. In contrast, under higher- Evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
resource conditions, an extended childhood serves to Bogaert, A. F., & Sadava, S. (2002). Adult attachment and sexual
increase the time that children can benefit from behaviour. Personal Relationships, 9, 191-204.
parental investment. Thus, children “should be Borgerhoff-Mulder, M. (1989). Menarche, menopause and
reproduction in the Kipsigis of Kenya. Journal of Biological Science,
selected to capitalize on the benefits of high-quality 21, 179-192.
parental investment, and to reduce the costs of low- Bourchard, T. (2004). Genetic influence on human psychological traits.
quality parental investment, by contingently altering Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 148-151.
the period of growth and development prior to Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. New York:
Basic Books.
reproductive maturity” (p. 947). Important to note, in Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context.
closing, is that irrespective of whether one embraces Development and Psychopathology, 17, 271 - 301.
Ellis’ (2004) reconceptualisation of BSD’s uncanny Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B.M., & Rock, S. L. (1998). Home
environment and school performance. Child Development, 59, 852 –
prediction linking social-developmental experiences 867.
in the family to pubertal timing (i.e., as just Brennan, K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1995). Dimensions of adult
terminating childhood rather than facilitating mating, attachment, affect regulation, and romantic relationship functioning.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 267-283.
pair bonding and parenting), the fact remains that he, Brennan, K. A., Clark, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report
just like Draper and Harpending (1982), Belsky et al. measures of adult attachment In J. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds),
(1991) and Chisholm (1996), regards early Attachment Theory and Close Relationships (pp. 46–76). New York:
Guilford Press.
experiences in the family as playing an influential role Bruer, J. T. (1997). The myth of the first three years. New York: Taylor
in shaping human reproductive strategy. This remains & Francis.
an all-too-uncommon way of thinking about Buehler, C., & Gerard, J. (2002). Marital conflict, ineffective parenting,
and children’s and adolescents adjustment. Journal of Marriage and
development within the field of developmental the Family, 64, 78-92.
psychology. Campbell, B. C., & Udry, J. R. (1995). Stress and age at menarche of
References mothers and daughters. Journal of Biosocial Science, 27, 127-134.
Adam, E., Gunnar, M., & Tanaka, A. (2004). Adult attachment, parent Capaldi, D., Pears, K., Patterson, G., & Owen, L. (2003). Continuity of
emotion, and observed parenting behaviour. Child Development, 75, parenting practices across generations in an at-risk sample. Journal
110-122. of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 127-142.
Ainsworth, M. D. (1973). The development of infant-mother Caspi, A., & Elder, G. H. (1988). Emergent family patterns. In R.
attachment. In B. M. Caldwell & H.N. Ricciuti (Eds), Review of Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within families
Child Development Research (Vol. 3, pp. 1-94). University of (pp.218-240). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chicago Press, Chicago. Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W, et
Amato, P. (2001). Children of divorce in the 1990s. Journal of Family al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated
Psychology, 15, 355-370. children. Science, 297, 851-854.
Bateson, P., Barker, D., Clutton-Brock, T., Deb, D., D’Udine, B., Charnov, E. L. (1993). Life history invariants. Oxford: Oxford
Foley, R.A. et al. (2004). Developmental plasticity and human University Press.
health. Nature, 430, 419 – 421. Chisholm, J. S. (1993). Death, hope, and sex. Current Anthropology,
Belsky, J. (1997). Patterns of attachment mating and parenting: An 34, 1–24.
evolutionary interpretation. Human Nature, 8, 361-381. Chisholm, J. S. (1996). The evolutionary ecology of attachment
Belsky, J. (1999). Modern evolutionary theory and patterns of organization. Human Nature, 7, 1–38.
attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds), Handbook of Chisholm, J. S. (1999). Death, hope and sex. New York: Cambridge
attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 141- University Press.
161). New York: Wiley. Cicchetti, D., & Carlson, V. (Eds.) (1989). Child maltreatment: theory
Belsky, J. (2000). Conditional and alternative reproductive strategies. and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and
In J. L. Rodgers, D.C. Rowe, & W.B. Miller (Eds), Genetic neglect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
466 心 理 学 报 39 卷

Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1991). The evolution of parental care. Princeton, Feeney, J. A. (2000). Implications of attachment style for patterns of
NJ: Princeton University Press. health and illness. Child: Care, Health and Human Development, 26,
Cohn, D. A., Cowan, P. A., Cowan, C.P., & Pearson, J. (1992). 277-288.
Mothers’ and fathers’ working models of childhood attachment Feeney, J. A. (1994). Attachment style, communication patterns and
relationships, parenting styles, and child behavior. Development and satisfaction across the life cycle of marriage. Personal Relationships,
Psychopathology, 4, 417-431. 1, 333-348.
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Callan, V. J. (1994). Attachment style,
and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality communication and satisfaction in the early years of marriage. In K.
and Social Psychology, 58, 644-663. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.) Advances in personal
Comings, D. E., Muhleman, D., Johnson, J. P., & MacMurray, J. P. relationships (Vol. 5, pp. 269-308). London: Jessica Kingsley.
(2002). Parent-daughter transmission of the androgen receptor gene Figueredo, J. A., Vasquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., Schneider, S. M. R.,
as an explanation of the effect of father absence on age of menarche. Sefcek, J. A., Tal, I. R., Hill, D., Wenner, C. J., & Jacobs, W. J.
Child Development, 73, 1046-1051. (2006). Consilience and life history theory: From genes to brain to
Conger, R., Nell, T., Kim, K., & Scaramella, L. (2003). Angry and reproductive strategy. Developmental Review, 26, 243-275.
aggressive behaviour across three generations. Journal of Abnormal Fuller, T. L., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Attachment style in married
Child Psychology, 31, 143-160. couples: Relation to current marital functioning, stability over time,
Crowell, J. A. & Feldman, S. S. (1988). Mothers’ internal working and method of assessment. Personal Relationships, 2, 17-34.
models of relationships and children’s behavioral and developmental Geary, D. C. (2002). Sexual selection and human life history. In R.
status. Child Development, 59, 1273-1285. Kail (Eds.), Advances in child development and behaviour (Vol. 30,
Crowell, J. A., & Feldman, S.S. (1991). Mothers’ internal working pp. 41-101). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
models of attachment relationships and mother and child behavior Geary, D. C. (2005). Evolution of paternal investment. In D. M. Buss
during separation and reunion. Developmental Psychology, 27, 597- (Ed), The evolutionary psychology handbook (pp. 483-505). New
605. York: Wiley.
Das Eiden, R., Teti, D. M. & Corns, K. M. (1995). Maternal working George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). Adult attachment
models of attachment, marital adjustment, and the parent-child interview. Unpublished manuscript, University of California,
relationship. Child Development, 66, 1504-1518. Berkeley.
Davila, J., Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1999). Attachment Geronimus, A. T. (1996). What teen mothers know. Human Nature, 7,
change processes in the early years of marriage. Journal of 323-352.
Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 783–802. Giedd, J., Blumentahal, J., Jeffries, N., Castellanos, F., Liu, H.,
DeWolff, M. S. & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and Zigdenbos, A., Paus, T., Evans, A.C., & Rapport, J. L. (1999). Brain
attachment. Child Development, 68, 571-591. development during childhood and adolescence. Nature
Doughty, D. & Rodger, J. L. (2000). Behavior genetic modelling of Neuroscience, 2, 861-863.
menarche in US females. In J. L. Rodges, D. C. Rowe, & W. B. Graber, J. A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. (1995). The
Miller (Eds), Genetic influences on human fertility and sexuality (pp. antecedents of menarcheal age. Child Development, 66, 346-359.
169–181). Boston: Kluwer Academic. Hamilton, C. (2000). Continuity and discontinuity of attachment from
Draper, P. & Harpending, H. (1982). Father absence and reproductive infancy through adolescence. Child Development, 71, 690–694.
strategy. Journal of Anthropological Research, 38, 255–272. Hawkes, K. (2003). Grandmothers and the evolution of human
Ellis, B. J. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls. longevity. American Journal of Human Biology, 15, 380-400.
Psychological Bulletin, 130, 920-958. Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., Blurton Jones, N. G., Alvarez, H. &
Ellis, B. J., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. Charnov, E. L. (2000). The grandmother hypothesis and human
J., Pettit, G. S., & Woodward, L. (2003). Does father absence place evolution. In L. Cronk, N. Chagnon & W. Irons (Eds). Adaptation
daughters at special risk for early sexual activity and teenage and Human Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective (pp. 231-
pregnancy? Child Development, 74, 801-821. 252). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Ellis, B. J., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). The origins of the social mind: Hazan, C. & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an
Evolutionary psychology and child development. London: Guilford. attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Ellis, B. J., & Garber, J. (2000). Psychosocial antecedents of variation 52, 511-524.
in girls’ pubertal timing. Child Development, 71, 485-501. Hetherington, E. M. & Kelly, J. (2002). For better or for worse. New
Ellis, B. J., Jackson, J. J., Boyce, W. T. (2006). The stress response York: Norton.
systems: Universality and adaptive individual differences. Hill, K. & Hurtado, A. (1996). Ache Life history. New York: Aldine
Developmental Review, 26, 175-212. De Gruyter.
Ellis, B. J., McFadyen-Ketchum, S., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G.S., & Hill, K. & Kaplan, H. (1999). Life history traits in humans. Annual
Bates, J. E. (1999). Quality of early family relationships and Review of Anthropology, 28, 397-430.
individual differences in the timing of pubertal maturation in girls. Hoier, S. (2003). Father absence and age at menarche. Human Nature,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 387 – 401. 14, 209-233.
Ellison, P. T. (2001). On fertile ground: A natural history of human Jones, B., Leeton, J., McLeod, I., & Wood, C. (1972). Factors
reproduction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. influencing the age of menarche in a lower socio-economic group in
Emery, R. (1988). Marriage, divorce and children’s adjustment. Melbourne. Medical Journal of Australia, 2, 533-535.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Johns, S. E. (2003). Environmental Risk and the Evolutionary
Evans, G. & English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty. Child Psychology of Teenage Motherhood. Unpublished PhD thesis,
Development, 73, 1238-1248. University of Bristol.
Flinn, M. V. (2006). Evolution and ontogeny of stress response to Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Rodgers, B., Jacomb, P. A., & Easteal, S.
social challenges in the human child. Developmental Review, 26, (2004). Association of adverse childhood experiences, age of
138-174. menarche and adult reproductive behaviour: Does the androgen
Flinn, M. V., & Ward, C. V. (2005). Ontogeny and evolution of the receptor gene play a role? American Journal of Medical Genetics
social child. In B. J. Ellis & D. F. Bjorklund (Eds),Origins of the Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 125, 105-111.
social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. Kadlubar, F. F., Berkowitz, G. S., Delongchamp, R. R., Wang, C.,
45-75). New York: Guilford Press. Green, B. L., Tang, G., et al. (2003). The CYP3A4*1B variant is
Fraley, R. C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: related to the onset of puberty, a known risk factor for the
A meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental development of breast cancer. Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers
mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 123-151. and Prevention, 12, 327-331.
Feeney, J. A. (1998). Adult attachment and relationship-centered Kagan, J., Kearsley, R., & Zelazo, P. (1978) Infancy. Cambridge, MA:
anxiety. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds), Attachment theory Harvard University Press.
and close relationships (pp. 189-218). New York: Guilford Press. Kaplan, H. S., & Lancaster, J. B. (2003). An evolutionary and
ecological analysis of human fertility, mating patterns and parental
investment. In K. W. Wachter & R. A. Bulatao, (Eds), National
3期 Jay Belsky. Experience in Childhood and the Development of Reproductive Strategies 467

Research Council (2003) fertility behaviour in biodemographic child adjustment in Mexican American and European families. Child
perspective (pp. 170-223). Washington, DC: National Academics Development, 75, 1613-1909.
Press. Patterson, G. R. (1986). Performance models for antisocial boys.
Kaplan, H. S., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of American Psychologist, 41, 432 – 444.
human life history evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156- Peccei, J. S. (2000). Genetic correlation between the ages of menarche
185. and menopause. Human Nature, 11, 43–63.
Kiernan, K. E., & Hobcraft, J. (1997). Parental divorce during Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. W. (1989). Family interaction patterns and
childhood. Population Studies, 51, 41–55. children’s behaviour problems from infancy to four years.
Kim, K., & Smith, P. K. (1998a). Childhood stress, behavioural Developmental Psychology, 25, 413 – 420.
symptoms and mother-daughter pubertal development. Journal of Phillips, D. I. W. (1998). Birth weight and the future development of
Adolescence, 21, 231–240. diabetes. Diabetes Care, 21, 150–155.
Kim, K. & Smith, P. K. (1998b). Retrospective survey of parental Pianka, E. R. (1970). On r and k selection. American Naturalist, 104,
marital relations and child reproductive development. International 592-597.
Journal of Behavioral Development, 22, 729–751. Pistole, M. C. (1989). Attachment in adult romantic relationships.
Kim, K., Smith, P. K., & Palermiti, A. L. (1997). Conflict in childhood Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6, 505–510.
and reproductive development. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, Pistole, M. C., Clark, E. M. (1995). Love relationships. Journal of
109-142. Mental Health Counselling, 17, 199–210.
Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Williams, B., Newcombe, R., Quinlan, R. J. (2003). Father absence, parental care, and female
Craig, I..W., & Moffitt, T.E. (2006). MAOA, maltreatment, and reproductive development. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24,
gene-environment interaction predicting children’s mental health: 376–390.
new evidence and a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 1-11. Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J., & Stevens, J. G. (1998). Attachment
Kirkpatrick, L.A. (1998). Evolution, pair-bonding, and reproductive orientations, social support, and conflict resolution in close
strategies. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds). Attachment relationships. In J. Simpson, & W. S. Rholes (Eds.) Attachment
theory and close relationships (pp. 353–393). New York: Guilford theory and close relationships (pp. 166–188). New York: Guilford
Press. Press.
Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Davis, K. E. (1994). Attachment style, gender Robinson, S. M., & Barker, D. J. (2002). Coronary heart disease.
and relationship stability. Journal of Personality and Social Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 61, 537–542.
Psychology, 66, 502-512. Romans, S. E., Martin, M., Gendall, K., & Herbison, G. P. (2003). Age
Kirkpatrick, L. A. & Hazan, C. (1994). Attachment styles and close of menarche: The role of some psychosocial factors. Psychological
relationships. Personal Relationships, 1, 123–142. Medicine, 33, 933–939.
Kobak, R. R., & Hazan, C. (1991). Attachment in marriage. Journal of Rowe, D. C. (2000). Environmental and genetic influences on pubertal
Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 861–869. development. In J. L. Rodgers, D. C. Rowe, & W. B. Miller (Eds),
Kuzawa, C. (2005). Fetal origins of developmental plasticity. Genetic influences on human fertility and sexuality (pp. 147–168).
American Journal of Human Biology, 17, 5-21. Boston: Kluwer Academic.
Lengua, L. (2002). The contribution of emotionality and self- Rowe, D. C. (2002). On genetic variation in menarche and age at first
regulation to the understanding of children’s response to multiple sexual intercourse. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 365-372.
risk. Child Development, 72, 144-161. Rushton, P. (1985). Differential K theory. Personality & Individual
Levy, M. B., & Davis, K. E. (1998). Love styles and attachment styles Differences, 6, 441-452.
compared. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 5, 439 – Sayer, A.A., Cooper, C., Evans, J. R., Rauf, A., Wormald, R. P.,
471. Osmond, C., Barker, D. J. (1998). Are rates of ageing determined in
Lewis, M. (1997). Altering fate. New York: Guilford Press. utero? Age and Ageing, 27, 579–583.
Lewis, M., Feiring, C., & Rosenthal, S. (2000). Attachment over time. Schore, A. (2001). Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right
Child Development, 71, 707-720. brain development, affect regulation and infant mental health. Infant
McArthur, R. H. & Wilson, E. O. (1967). The theory of island Mental Health Journal, 22, 7-66.
biogeography. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Seccombe, K. (2000). Families in poverty in the 1990s. Journal of
McDade, T. W., Beck, M. A., Kuzawa, C.W., & Adair, L. S. (2001). Marriage and the Family, 62, 1094-1113.
Prenatal undernutrition, postnatal environments, and antibody Serbin, L., & Karp, J. (2003). Intergenerational studies of parenting
response to vaccination in adolescence. American Journal of and the transfer of risk from parent to child. Current Directions in
Clinical Nutrition, 74, 543–548. Psychological Science, 12, 138-142.
McLoyd, V. C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on black Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2002). Attachment-related
families and children. Child Development, 61, 311-346. psychodynamics. Attachment & Human Development, 4, 133–161.
Mezzich, A. C., Tarter, R. E., Giancola, P. R., Lu, S., Kirisci, L., & Shonkoff, J., & Phillips, D. (2000). From neurons to neighbourhoods:
Parks, S. (1997). Substance use and risky sexual behaviour in female The science of early child development. Washington, DC: National
adolescents. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 44, 157–166. Academies Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Nachshon, O. (1991). Attachment styles and Simpson, J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on romantic
patterns of self – disclosure. Journal of Personality and Social relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59,
Psychology, 61, 321–331. 971–980.
Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2002). Slade, A., Belsky, J., Aber, J. L., & Phelps, J. L. (1999). Maternal
Attachment Security in Couple Relationships. Family Process, 41, representations of their relationship with their toddlers.
405–434. Developmental Psychology, 35, 611-619.
Miller, C., & Fishkin, S. A. (1997). On the dynamics of human Sroufe, L. A. (1988). The role of infant-caregiver attachment in
bonding and reproductive success. In J. A. Simpson, D. T. Kenrick development. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds). Clinical
(Eds), Evolutionary Social Psychology (pp. 197-235). New Jersey: implications of attachment (pp. 18-40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lawrence Erlbaum. Stavrou, I., Zois, C., Ioannidis, J. P., & Tsatsoulis, A. (2002).
Miller, W. B., & Pasta, D. J. (2000). Early family environment, Association of polymorphisms of the oestrogen receptor alpha gene
reproductive strategy and contraceptive behaviour. In J. L. Rodger, with the age of menarche. Human Reproduction, 17, 1101–1105.
D. C. Rowe, & W. B. Miller (Eds.), Genetic influences on human Stearns, S. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford: Oxford
fertility and sexuality (pp. 183–230). Boston: Kluwer Academic. University Press.
Moffitt, T. W., Caspi, A., Belsky, J., & Silva, P. A. (1992). Childhood Stearns, S., & Koella, J. C. (1986). The evolution of phenotypic
experience and the onset of menarche. Child Development, 63, 47– plasticity in life-history traits. Evolution, 40, 893-913.
58. Steinberg, L. (1988). Reciprocal relation between parent-child distance
Mousseau, T. A., & Fox, C. W. (1998). Maternal effects as and pubertal maturation. Developmental Psychology, 24, 122-128.
adaptations. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Surbey, M. K. (1990). Family composition, stress, and the timing of
Parke, R., Coltrane, S., Duffy, S., Buriel, R., Dennis, J., Powers, J., human menarche. In T. E. Ziegler, & F. B. Bercovitch (Eds).
French, S., & Widaman, K. (2004). Economic stress, parenting, and
468 心 理 学 报 39 卷

Monographs in primatology: Vol. 13. Socioendocrinology of Weinfield, N., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment from
primate reproduction (pp. 11–32). New York: Wiley-Liss. infancy to early adulthood in a high-risk sample. Child Development,
Thompson, R. (1999). Early attachment and later development. In J. 71, 695–702.
Cassidy, & P. R. Shaver (Eds). Handbook of Attachment (pp. 265– West-Eberhard, M. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution.
286). New York: Guilford. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Thornberry, T., Feeeman-Gallant, A., Lizotte, A., Krohn, M., & Smith, Wierson, M., Long, P. J., & Forehand, R. L. (1993). Toward a new
C. (2003). Linked lives. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, understanding of early menarche. Adolescence, 28, 913–924.
171-184. Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton,
Treloar, S. A., & Martin, N. G. (1990). Age at menarche as a fitness NJ: Princeton University Press.
trait. American Journal of Human Genetics, 47, 89–107. Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Turkheimer, E., & Waldron, M. (2000). Non-shared environment. University.
Psychological Bulletin, 126, 78-108. Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1997). Life expectancy, economic inequality,
van Ijzendoorn, M. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental homicide and reproductive timing in Chicago neighbourhoods.
responsiveness, and infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 604- British Medical Journal, 314, 1271-1274.
609. Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2005). Carpe diem: Adaptation and devaluing
Waters, E,, Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J., & Albersheim, L. the future. Quarterly Review of Biology, 80, 55–60.
(2000). Attachment security in infancy and early adulthood. Child
Development, 71, 684–689.

View publication stats

You might also like