You are on page 1of 9

J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:938–945

DOI 10.1007/s10803-009-0700-0

ORIGINAL PAPER

Emotion Recognition in Children and Adolescents


with Autism Spectrum Disorders
Sanna Kuusikko Æ Helena Haapsamo Æ Eira Jansson-Verkasalo Æ
Tuula Hurtig Æ Marja-Leena Mattila Æ Hanna Ebeling Æ
Katja Jussila Æ Sven Bölte Æ Irma Moilanen

Published online: 10 February 2009


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Abstract We examined upper facial basic emotion rec- Keywords Asperger syndrome  Development 
ognition in 57 subjects with autism spectrum disorders Eye region  High-functioning autism  Interpretation 
(ASD) (M = 13.5 years) and 33 typically developing Self-esteem
controls (M = 14.3 years) by using a standardized com-
puter-aided measure (The Frankfurt Test and Training of
Facial Affect Recognition, FEFA). The ASD group scored
lower than controls on the total scores of FEFA and per- Understanding emotions usually requires multi-sensory
ceived ambiguous stimuli more often as a negative processing (e.g., Klucharev and Sams 2004). Emotions and
emotion. The older ASD group (C12 years) performed the feelings of others are interpreted from speech prosody
better than the younger ASD group (\12 years) on the as well as from facial and body gestures. The fetus can
blended emotions of FEFA. The results support the find- already recognize the prosody of speech before birth (e.g.,
ings that individuals with ASD have difficulties in emotion Smith et al. 2007) and for infants, facial expressions
recognition. However, older subjects with ASD seem to become another main source to identify emotional states in
have better skills than younger subjects with ASD. others (Meltzoff and Moore 1977; Woodhead et al. 1995).
The ability to interpret others’ emotions (more exactly)
improves with age, achieving adult level (of interpretation)
around the age of 10–11 (e.g., Custrini and Feldman 1989;
S. Kuusikko (&)  H. Haapsamo  T. Hurtig  M.-L. Mattila  Tonks et al. 2007). The ability to recognize the emotional
H. Ebeling  K. Jussila  I. Moilanen state of others requires the ability to divide attention and
Institute of Clinical Medicine, Department of Child Psychiatry, focus gaze on relevant information. Most of this processing
University and University Hospital of Oulu, P.O. Box 26, is subconscious. From speech prosody, a commanding
90029 OYS Oulu, Finland
e-mail: kuusisan@paju.oulu.fi; sanna.kuusikko@gmail.com expression has been reported to be the easiest to recognize,
while a sad expression is the most difficult (e.g., Kujala
E. Jansson-Verkasalo et al. 2005). There is no full consensus as to which basic
Faculty of Humanities, Logopedics, University of Oulu, Oulu, emotion (i.e., happiness, sadness, fear, anger, surprise, or
Finland
disgust) is the easiest to decode from facial expressions. In
E. Jansson-Verkasalo some studies, disgust or happiness has been reported to be
Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, University Hospital the easiest, whereas fear has been reported to be the most
of Oulu, Oulu, Finland
difficult emotion to recognize (e.g., Ekman and Friesen
T. Hurtig 1976; Kohler et al. 2004; Montagne et al. 2007).
Institute of Health Sciences, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland Autism spectrum disorders (ASD), such as Asperger
syndrome (AS) and High-functioning autism (HFA) are
S. Bölte
characterized by impairment in social interaction, such as
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, J.W. Goethe
University Frankfurt/M, Deutschorden Str. 50, 60528 Frankfurt, difficulty in forming friendships and a lack of under-
Germany standing of emotions and minds of others (Theory of mind

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:938–945 939

[ToM]; e.g., Baron-Cohen et al. 1985; Kaland et al. 2007). reactions towards outside stimuli and not dependent on
Caregivers of children with ASD have reported oddness in cognitive processing or learned in the cultural environment
their child’s social development as early as at 30 months of as more complex mental states are. Furthermore, regardless
age (Howlin and Asgharian 1999) as shown by impairment of many previous studies of emotion recognition skills of
of social orienting and joint attention (Dawson et al. individuals with ASD, the research field is lacking studies
2004a). Social oddness may be due to abnormal eye-to-eye which are focused on that what emotion individuals with
contact, since children with ASD tend to avoid eye-to-eye ASD are decoding when they do not get the correct inter-
gaze. Furthermore, impaired social interaction may also be pretation from the shown facial expression.
linked to deficient skills in recognizing social cues from The objectives of this study were (a) to evaluate basic
other people’s facial expressions, body postures, and facial emotion recognition in partial faces (i.e., the eye
speech prosody (e.g., Attwood et al. 1988; Korpilahti et al. region) in typically developing children and children and
2007; Macdonald et al. 1989). For example, some studies adolescents with ASD by using the FEFA. (b) To study
report that children with AS are more focused on the lower whether emotion recognition skills improve with age in
part (i.e., the mouth) than the upper part (e.g., eyes) of the individuals with and without ASD, and (c) to study what
face in making conclusions about facial emotion (e.g., emotions are identified by individuals with ASD when they
Gross 2004; Klin et al. 2002). However, some researchers rate emotions incorrectly (i.e., what emotions people with
suggest that children with autism also use the upper part of ASD identify when they get the task wrong).
the face in making conclusions about facial expression
(e.g., van der Geest et al. 2002; Laihaie et al. 2006), the eye
region avoidance being rather an attentional strategy in Methods
individuals with autism. In the end, children with ASD
have been shown to ignore or neglect especially negative Participants
facial affects (i.e., distress, fear, and discomfort) demon-
strated by adults or by photos as early as at age three (e.g., ASD probands were gathered from an epidemiological
Dawson et al. 2004b; Sigman 1992). Difficulties in rec- study conducted in 2000–2003 (see Mattila et al. 2007) and
ognizing negative emotion, such as fear and sadness from from the patient records of Oulu University Hospital in
photos of faces are evident even in adults with ASD (e.g., 2003. Controls were recruited from randomly selected
Ashwin et al. 2007; Boraston et al. 2007; Pelphrey et al. mainstream schools in Oulu in 2006 (Kuusikko et al.
2002). In accordance with atypical face processing, recent 2008a, b).
studies have shown that processing of basic emotions from The ASD diagnoses were confirmed by a trained diag-
speech is impaired in individuals with AS (Kujala et al. nostician clinical psychologist (KJ) as well as a
2005; Korpilahti et al. 2007). Kujala et al. 2005 reported pediatrician (M-LM) based on the ICD-10 criteria (WHO
that adults with ASD recognized commanding and scornful 1993). Diagnoses of AS and HFA were distinguished from
auditory stimuli as well as controls, but not sad ones. each other by using the participants’ history of delays or
Findings are not, however, so far conclusive, since some abnormal functioning prior to the age of three (i.e., delays
reports have shown that children with autism are equally or abnormal functioning prior to the age of three = HFA,
able as typically developing children to recognize emotions no delays or abnormal functioning prior to the age of
or even complex mental states from dynamic and static three = AS). Diagnostic confirmation was obtained with
faces (e.g., Back et al. 2007; Castelli 2005; Buitelaar et al. the autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R; Lord et al.
1999) and that is why more research data of emotion rec- 1995) and the autism diagnostic observation schedule
ognition in individuals with ASD are still needed. (ADOS; Lord et al. 2000). All participants with ASD had a
Because impairment in emotion recognition seems to be full-scale IQ greater than 75 (Wechsler Intelligence Scale
rather permanent in individuals with ASD, intervention for Children—Third Revision, Wechsler 1991) and they
tools for improving emotional recognition skills of indi- did not meet criteria for other severe developmental dis-
viduals with ASD are desirable. One computer-aided orders (e.g., Fragile X).
instrument to test and teach the recognition of facial Parents of control children were asked to complete the
emotions is the Frankfurt Test and Training of Facial autism spectrum screening questionnaire (ASSQ; Ehlers
Affect Recognition (FEFA; Bölte et al. 2003). Its effects et al. 1999). To be included in the further analyses, the
have been demonstrated on both behavioral and neuro- parent-rated ASSQ score had to be less than seven.
biological level in adults (Bölte et al. 2006). However, it is A total of 26 subjects with HFA aged 9.9–24.3 years (M =
not yet well known how typically developing children and 13.7 ± 3.5 years), 31 subjects with AS aged 9.8–21.2 years
adolescents perform on the FEFA. Also, we wanted to (M = 13.3 ± 2.7 years), and 33 typically developing control
focus on basic emotions as they are universal and primitive subjects aged 10.5 and 16.7 years (M = 14.3 ± 1.5 years

123
940 J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:938–945

took part in the study. The difference between the mean ages The Pure Emotions scale consists of six pictures of
of each diagnostic group was significant; the participants in happy eyes, four pictures of sad eyes, three pictures of
the control group were thus older than participants in the AS fearful eyes, five pictures of angry eyes, one picture of
and HFA groups (v2 = 11.6, df = 2, p \ .01). In addition, the surprised eyes, two pictures of disgusted eyes, and four
proportion of boys was higher in the AS (23 boys, 8 girls) and pictures of neutral eyes. In addition, one picture was
HFA (21 boys, 5 girls) groups than in the control group (15 removed from the sadness scale and from further analyses
boys, 18 girls). The mean age for boys in the AS, HFA, and because most of the participants (i.e., 80.0% of the ASD
control groups were 9.8 (SD = 3.0), 9.9 (SD = 3.8), and 11.5 group and 93.9% of the controls) misclassified it as hap-
(SD = 1.5). There were no significant age difference among piness. Thus, the maximum score was 39 on the FEFA
boys (v2 = 3.5, df = 2, p = ns). For the girls, mean ages Total scale, 24 on the Pure Emotion scale, and three on the
were, respectively 10.6 (SD = 1.0), 11.3 (SD = 0.8), and sadness scale.
10.5 (SD = 1.6) yielding girls with AS and HFA being sta- The Blended Emotion scale consists of four blends of
tistically significantly older than control girls (v2 = 12.3, surprise-fear, three blends of sadness-neutral, two blends of
df = 2, p \ .01). anger-disgust, one blend of sadness-fear; sadness-anger;
fear-anger; neutral-happiness; neutral-anger; and happi-
Procedure ness-surprise. The maximum score on the Blended
Emotion scale was 15.
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Oulu
University Hospital, Finland, and the control sample was Statistical Analyses
approved by the Chief of Education, the principals of the
schools involved, and the teachers of randomly selected The analyses were performed with the Statistical Package
classes at the schools taking part in the study. After for Social Sciences (SPSS/Win 14.0). Variables were
parental consent, the children, accompanied by one or both normally distributed, but due to some other violations of
parents, were invited to the out-patient clinic, where the parametric testing (i.e., small sample sizes and small
study task was administered to the child in the presence of number of variables in some sum scores of the data) sub-
a study researcher (KK, KL). sequent analyses were performed using nonparametric
tests. Mann–Whitney U-test (Z) was used to calculate the
Instrument group differences between gender, diagnostic status, and
age group on the FEFA scales. Chi-square test (v2) was
The FEFA is a computer-based emotion recognition test, used to compare incorrectly rated emotions and blended
which investigates the face processing and emotion rec- emotions in different diagnostic groups. p values lower
ognition with black and white pictures presenting six basic than .01 are considered as significant due to correction of
emotions (i.e., happiness, sadness, fear, anger, surprise, and multiple testing. However, also results with p value lower
disgust) and neutral (Bölte et al. 2003). The FEFA com- than .05 are reported. All p values reported are two-tailed.
prises two submodules: 50 pictures of faces (i.e., the Face
Submodule) and 40 pictures of eyes (i.e., the Eye Sub-
module). Each picture is shown in the computer screen Results
separately within all six emotions written side of the pic-
ture as an answer option (See photo in Bölte and Poustka Diagnose and Gender Differences on FEFA Within
2003). Participant is asked to select correct emotion by ASD Probands and Controls
clicking it with the computer mouse. Normative data for
FEFA have been collected in Germany with typically The AS and HFA groups performed similarly on the FEFA.
developed adults (N = 22, M = 29.7 years), yielding a The only statistically significant difference between par-
mean score of 42.9 for the Face Submodule and 34.5 for ticipants with AS or HFA was seen on the disgust scale:
the Eye Submodule (Bölte et al. 2006). The psychometric participants with AS scored higher than participants with
properties of FEFA have been reported to be excellent HFA (M = 1.6 vs. 1.2, z = 2.8, df = 1, p \ .01). There-
(Bölte and Poustka 2003). fore, we combined AS and HFA participants into a single
In this study the Eye Submodule was used. The answers group (i.e., ASD group) for all subsequent analyses. A
are rated on a binary scale (i.e., 0 = not correct and gender difference was found in the ASD on the Anger
1 = correct). Of the 40 pictures (i.e., FEFA Total scale), 25 scale: boys with ASD scored higher than girls with ASD
pictures have one correct answer (i.e., Pure Emotion scale) (M = 0.5 vs. 0.4, z = 2.2, df = 1, p \ .05). In the control
and 15 have two alternative correct answers (i.e., Blended group significant gender differences were found on the
Emotion scale). Pure positive scale (i.e., scales of Happiness ? Surprise)

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:938–945 941

and Happiness scale: control girls scored higher on these In addition, younger children with ASD scored lower
scales than did control boys (M = 6.1 vs. 5.0, z = 2.8, than older participants with ASD on the FEFA Total scale
df = 1, p \ .01; M = 0.9 vs. 0.7, z = 2.8, df = 1, p \ .01, (M = 23.8 vs. 26.9, z = 2.3, p \ .05) and significantly
respectively). Due to significant gender differences in the lower on the Blended Emotion scale (M = 10.6 vs. 12.0,
ASD and control groups, genders were examined sepa- z = 3.0, df = 1, p \ .01) (See Fig. 2).
rately on the Pure positive scale, on the Happiness scale
and on the Anger scale. In addition, surprise and disgust Differences Between Diagnostic Groups on
were the easiest and anger the most difficult emotion to Discrimination of Blended Emotions
recognize in the ASD group, whereas surprise was the
easiest and anger the most difficult emotion to recognize in When the emotion was a blend between surprise and fear,
the control group (See Fig. 1). participants with ASD interpreted the blend significantly
more often as fear compared to the controls (56.1 vs. 18.2%,
Age Differences on The FEFA Within ASD Probands v2 = 12.8, df = 1, p \ .001) (See Fig. 3). Controls also
and Controls decoded sadness-neutral blended emotions as neutral more
often than ASD probands (56.3 vs. 31.6%, v2 = 6.0, df = 1,
Age was included in the statistical models to examine whe- p \ .05). There were no significant differences between
ther performance on the FEFA improves with age among the ASD probands and controls decoding the blends of anger-
groups. Children were divided into two age groups, those disgust, sadness-fear, sadness-anger, fear-anger, neutral-
under 12 years of age and those 12 years or older. There happiness, neutral-anger, or happiness-surprise.
were no significant differences on FEFA scales between the
younger ASD (n = 14, M = 10.9 years) and younger con- Analysis of Incorrectly Classified Emotions in ASD
trol group (n = 3, M = 11.2 years). However, the older Probands and Controls
ASD group (n = 43, M = 14.3 years) scored lower than the
older control group (n = 30, M = 14.6 years) on the FEFA We were interested in finding out with what emotion the
Total scale (M = 26.9 vs. 28.9, z = 2.4, df = 1, p \ .05) and participants confused the correct emotion when the clas-
on the FEFA Pure Emotion scale (M = 14.9 vs. 16.6, sification was incorrect on the Pure Emotion scale. We
z = 2.4, df = 1, p \ .05) (see Table 1). Older girls with divided the six emotions into positives (i.e., happiness and
ASD performed significantly lower than older control girls surprise) and negatives (i.e., sadness, fear, anger, and dis-
on the Pure positive scale, on the Happiness scale, and on the gust). We were also interested in seeing with what emotion
Anger scale (M = 4.1 vs. 6.1, z = 2.9, df = 1, p \ .01; the participants confused the neutral faces if the classifi-
M = 3.4 vs. 5.2, z = 3.3, df = 1, p \ .001; M = 1.8 vs. 3.0, cation was incorrect.
z = 3.2, df = 1, p \ .01, respectively). There were no sig- In their incorrect answers, the ASD probands rated anger
nificant differences between younger groups or between as the other negative emotion (44.8%) significantly more
older boy groups on the Pure positive scale, Happiness scale often compared to the controls (26.1%) (v2 = 20.9, df = 1,
or the Anger scale. p \ .001) (See Fig. 4). On the other hand, the ASD pro-
bands misclassified disgust as a positive emotion (i.e.,
0,9
Pure emotions surprise) (17.5%) more often than the controls (6.1%)
0,8 means for ASD
(v2 = 6.0, df = 1, p \ .05) in their incorrect answers.
Pure emotions
0,7 means for controls Furthermore, in their incorrect answers the controls inter-
preted a neutral expression more often as a negative
0,6
(14.4%) than a positive (3.0%) emotion, whereas ASD
0,5 probands divided their false answers more equally between
0,4 negative (17.1%) and positive (11.8%) emotions (v2 = 4.2,
df = 1, p \ .05).
0,3

0,2

0,1 Discussion
0
Our aim was to study emotion recognition in children with
ss

l
ss

er
ar

ed

tra
us
ne

Fe

g
ne

is

ASD and typically developing children. Similarly as found


eu
An

pr

is
pi

N
D
Sa

r
p

Su
Ha

earlier in children with autism, the ASD probands per-


Fig. 1 Means of pure emotion answers divided by number of items in formed significantly lower on the FEFA emotion
each emotion recognition test compared to the controls. Specifically, for

123
942 J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:938–945

Table 1 Mean scores of the FEFA scales on ASD and control groups
FEFA Scales (maximum score) M (SD) z (df = 1) M (SD) z (df = 1)
Participants under 12 years old Participants on or above 12 years old
ASD N = 14 Controls N = 3 ASD N = 43 Controls N = 30

FEFA total (39) 23.9 (4.4) 25.7 (5.0) 1.4 26.9 (3.7) 28.9 (3.5) 2.4*
Pure emotion total (24) 13.1 (3.2) 14.3 (4.5) 1.0 14.9 (3.0) 16.6 (3.0) 2.4*
Pure positive (7) 3.7 (1.7) 5.3 (1.5) 0.5 4.5 (1.5) 5.6 (1.1) 3.0
Pure negative (13) 6.9 (1.6) 5.3 (4.0) 1.2 7.4 (1.8) 7.7 (1.8) 0.6
Happiness (6) 3.1 (1.8) 4.3 (1.5) 0.6 3.8 (1.5) 4.8 (1.1) 3.0
Sadness (3) 1.5 (0.8) 1.7 (0.6) 0.8 1.7 (0.8) 1.7 (0.5) 0.2
Fear (3) 1.6 (1.4) 1.0 (1.7) 0.9 2.0 (1.1) 1.7 (1.1) 1.3
Anger (5) 2.3 (0.6) 1.7 (1.2) 1.7 2.3 (0.9) 2.7 (0.7) 2.1*
Surprise (1) 0.6 (0.5) 1.0 (0.0) 0.8 0.8 (0.4) 0.8 (0.4) 0.7
Disgust (2) 1.4 (0.6) 1.0 (1.0) 1.2 1.4 (0.7) 1.6 (0.7) 1.0
Neutral (4) 2.6 (0.9) 3.7 (0.6) 0.5 2.9 (1.0) 3.3 (1.0) 1.5
Blended emotion total (15) 10.6 (1.4) 11.3 (0.6) 1.6 12.0 (1.2) 12.3 (1.2) 1.3

p \ .01, * p \ .05, two-tailed
Note: Old ASD group \ old control group on FEFA total scale; Pure emotion total scale; Pure positive emotion scale; Happiness scale; Anger
scale

0% 20 % 40 % 60 % 80 % 100 %
*
30,0
ASD < 12 years More fear
controls < 12 years
25,0 ASD 12 years No difference
controls 12 years
20,0 † More surprised

15,0

More neutral
10,0
No difference
5,0
More sadness

0,0
ASD Control
FEFA Total Blended Emotions

p < .01, * p < .05, 2-tailed. Fig. 3 Rates of answers on blended emotions of surprise-fear and
neutral-sadness in ASD and control groups
Fig. 2 Differences with age on the mean scores of the FEFA total
and the blended emotion scales. Note: The young ASD group \ the
old ASD group on the mean scores of the FEFA total and the blended In the case of blended emotions, children with ASD
emotion scales perceived in ambiguous stimuli more often negative emo-
tions, such as fear, than did the controls. Compared to
the girls with ASD, expressions of happiness and anger were controls, the ASD probands also misinterpreted anger in
more difficult to recognize from the eyes compared to the their incorrect answers more often to another negative
control girls even the girls with ASD were significantly emotion. These findings give rise to the question of whe-
older than the control girls. These findings may be due to the ther ASD probands have a tendency to interpret emotions
fact that individuals with autism need more exaggerated as negative rather positive. If so, is it due to their consid-
facial gestures to be able to interpret the emotional state of erable amount of negative experiences, difficulties with
others (Rutherford and McIntosh 2007) correctly, or that experiencing and containing their own aggressiveness, or
they need more cues, such as verbal or environmental because they often interpret the reactions of others as
contents, to decode the emotion correctly (Grossman et al. aggressive based on their own projective identification?
2002; Lindner and Rosén 2006). When the understanding and interpreting of others’

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:938–945 943

0% 20 % 40 % 60 % 80 % 100 % However, in their incorrect answers ASD probands


interpreted disgust more often as surprise, although this
Sadness Happiness
ASD
Control was not statistically significant (p \ .05), whereas control
interpreted it as another negative emotion, anger. This
ASD
finding may reflect the only difference that was found
Control
between children with HFA and AS: the emotion repre-
senting disgust was significantly more difficult to recognize
for children with HFA than for those with AS. This could
ASD
mean that children with AS are somewhat more able than
Fear

Control
children with HFA to differentiate between emotions as
well as to verbalize them, although the finding is rather
ASD
small and specific. Replications of this finding are thus
Anger

Control needed. Furthermore, ASD probands’ incorrect answers on


the neutral expression scale were fairly equally divided
Surprised

ASD between incorrect positive and incorrect negative emotions,


Control whereas the incorrect ratings by controls were more on the
negative side. This finding may reflect the fact that neutral
ASD expression was more difficult to recognize for the ASD
Disgust

Control group than the control group, and/or that individuals with
ASD tend to interpret all faces, even neutral ones, as
meaningful (Bölte et al. 2008).
Neutral

ASD
Control The skills to recognize emotions from the eye region
improved significantly with chronological age in individuals
Correct emotion Incorrect, Negative emotion with ASD. It may be that in individuals with ASD, emotion
Incorrect, Positive emotion Incorrect, Neutral expression recognition skills improve over a longer period of time than
in typically developing children. It thus seems that ASD
Fig. 4 Rates of correctly and incorrectly classified emotions on ASD probands do not achieve the level of their typically devel-
and control groups.Note: ASD [ controls anger as another negative oping control counterparts, which is why it would be very
emotion. ASD [ controls disgust as a positive emotion. ASD [
useful to teach emotion recognition in therapeutic settings
controls neutral as a positive emotion
(away from the natural settings) to individuals with ASD.
There are studies showing improvement of facial emotion
feelings is impaired or delayed, others’ aggressiveness as recognition with practice in individuals with autism (e.g.,
well as tenderness towards self may come as a surprise Bölte et al. 2006; Golan and Baron-Cohen 2006; Silver and
every time. Responses toward others may thus be atypical Oakes 2001). However, because generalizing adapted skills
considering the situation: individuals with ASD may overly from one situation to another is a well-known impairment in
interpret and anticipate negative responses from others. ASD, the intervention should start early and focus on
Atypical responses of individuals with ASD might be self- building a positive self-esteem in individuals with ASD.
protection from a situation or feeling, which is too difficult A larger sample of control children under age 12 years
to cope with. Atypical responses in social situations may be old may have enhanced our power to detect greater asso-
one reason why individuals with ASD are rejected by ciations between age group and FEFA outcomes. Partly
peers, causing negative experiences. Furthermore, rejection due to data power in control groups, no significant differ-
as well as withdrawal from social situations may cause a ences were found between younger and older control
critical lack of real-life practise in understand the feelings groups. Moreover, in the used measure, there were low rate
of others. Instead of reality, the interpretation of others’ of emotions in each scale. That may have prevented us
feelings may thus also be based on one’s imagination and from obtaining the statistically significant differences
self-esteem. In addition, social anxiety symptoms are between the groups. Finally, data samples were not FSIQ-
reported to be common in children and adolescents with matched, but all participants attended mainstream schools.
ASD (e.g., Kuusikko et al. 2008b; Melfsen et al. 2006). Previous studies have shown that high-functioning children
Therefore, it is also possible that children with ASD are with autism score higher than low-functioning children
more sensitive and more prone to focus their selective with autism on emotion recognition. This may reflect the
attention towards negative rather positive emotions, simi- finding that the children with ASD in our study performed
larly to individuals with social and generalized anxiety are better (M = 26.1) than for example in the study of
(e.g., Beck 1976; Eysenk 1992). Albertowski et al. (2007), where the mean score on the Eye

123
944 J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:938–945

Submodule was 19.3 in a group of 45 children with autism Bölte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., & Poustka, F. (2003). Frankfurter test
(M = 12.3 years, Raven-IQ = 98 ± 27.4) (Albertowski und training des erkennens von fazialem affekt, FEFA. Frankfurt
am Main: J.W. Goethe-Universitätsklinikum.
et al. 2007). Bölte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., & Poustka, F. (2008). Brief report:
In summary, children and adolescents with ASD seem to Emotional processing in high-functioning autism-physiological
have more difficulties than typically developing children reactivity and affective report. Journal of Autism and Develop-
and adolescents in recognizing emotions from the upper mental Disorders, 38(4), 776–781. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-
0443-8.
part of the face. However, the emotion recognition skills of Bölte, S., Hubl, D., Feineis-Matthews, S., Prvulovic, D., Dierks, T., &
individuals with ASD may to improve with age, even Poustka, F. (2006). Facial affect recognition training in autism:
thought they do not seem to achieve the level of typically can we animate the fusiform gyrus? Behavioral Neuroscience,
developing individuals. Thus, it is important to examine 120, 211–216. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.120.1.211.
Bölte, S., & Poustka, F. (2003). The recognition of facial affect in
and develop new intervention tools to teach emotion rec- autistic and schizophrenic subjects and their first-degree rela-
ognition strategies at an early stage to individuals with tives. Psychological Medicine, 33, 907–915. doi:10.1017/S003
ASD. Finally, helping children with ASD to understand the 3291703007438.
minds of others may prevent children with ASD from Boraston, Z., Blakemore, S.-J., Chilvers, R., & Skuse, D. (2007).
Impaired sadness recognition is linked to social interaction
experiencing negative series of vicious circles and possibly deficit in autism. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1501–1510. doi:
reduce their psychiatric comorbidity, such as social anxiety 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.010.
and depression, in adolescence and adulthood. Buitelaar, J. K., van der Wees, M., Swaab-Barneveld, H., & van der
Gaag, R. J. (1999). Theory of mind and emotion-recognition
Acknowledgments The research was supported by The Alma and functioning in autistic spectrum disorders and in psychiatric
K. A. Snellman Foundation, Oulu, Finland (PI: S. Kuusikko, H. Ha- control and normal children. Development and Psychopathology,
apsamo, M-L. Mattila); Child Psychiatric Research Foundation, 11, 39–58. doi:10.1017/S0954579499001947.
Finland (PI: S. Kuusikko, M-L. Mattila); Child Psychiatric Research Castelli, F. (2005). Understanding emotions from standardized facial
Foundation, Northern Ostrobothnia Hospital District Area, Finland expressions in autism and normal development. Autism, 9, 428–
(PI: S. Kuusikko, M-L. Mattila); Emil Aaltonen Foundation, Finland 449. doi:10.1177/1362361305056082.
(PI: H. Ebeling); the Rinnekoti Research Foundation, Espoo, Finland Custrini, R. J., & Feldman, R. S. (1989). Children’s social compe-
(PI: M-L. Mattila); Thule Institute, University of Oulu (PI: I. tence and nonverbal encoding and decoding of emotions.
Moilanen); and Juselius Foundation (PI: I. Moilanen). The Graduate Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 18, 336–342. doi:10.1207/
School of Circumpolar Wellbeing, Health and Adaptation is s15374424jccp1804_7.
acknowledged for their support. We want to thank the participants of Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A.,
the study, Clinical Psychologist Raija Ollikainen for translating the et al. (2004a). Early social attention impairments in autism:
FEFA in Finnish, Ms Kaisa Kouri, Ms Katja Larinen, and Mr Jukka Social orienting, joint attention and attention to distress.
Rahko for their help in data collection, and Leena Joskitt for her Developmental Psychology, 40, 271–283. doi:10.1037/0012-
statistical advises. 1649.40.2.271.
Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., Carver, L., Panagiotides, H., & McPartland,
J. (2004b). Young children with autism show atypical brain
responses to fearful versus neutral facial expressions of emotion.
References Developmental Science, 7, 340–359. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.
2004.00352.x.
Albertowski, K., Bölte, S., & Poustka, F. (2007). Basic affect Ehlers, S., Gillberg, C., & Wing, L. (1999). A screening questionnaire
identification in autism and other child and adolescent mental for Asperger syndrome and other high-functioning autism
disorders (article in German). 1. Wissenschaftliche Tagung spectrum disorders in school age children. Journal of Autism
Autismus-Spektrum-Störungen. Abstractband (p. 35). Berlin, and Developmental Disorders, 29, 129–141. doi:10.1023/A:102
Frankfurt/M:MPIB. 3040610384.
Ashwin, C., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., O’Riordan, M., & Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo
Bullmore, E. T. (2007). Differential activation of the amygdale Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.
and the ‘social brain’ during fearful face-processing in Asperger Eysenk, M. W. (1992). Anxiety: The cognitive perspective. Hove:
syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2–14. doi:10.1016/j.neuro Erlbaum.
psychologia.2006.04.014. Golan, O., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Systemizing empathy:
Attwood, A., Frith, U., & Hermelin, B. (1988). The understanding and Teaching adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning
use of interpersonal gestures by autistic and Down’s syndrome autism to recognize complex emotions using interactive multi-
children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, media. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 591–617. doi:
241–257. doi:10.1007/BF02211950. 10.1017/S0954579406060305.
Back, E., Mitchell, P., & Ropar, D. (2007). Do the eyes have it? Gross, T. F. (2004). The perception of four basic emotions in human
Inferring mental states from animated faces in autism. Child and nonhuman faces by children with autism and other
Development, 78(2), 397–411. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007. developmental disabilities. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
01005.x. ogy, 32, 469–480. doi:10.1023/B:JACP.0000037777.17698.01.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic Grossman, J. B., Klin, A., Carter, A. S., & Volkmar, F. R. (2002).
child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 2, 37–46. doi: Verbal bias in recognition of facial emotions in children with
10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. atry, and Allied Disciplines, 41, 369–379. doi:10.1111/1469-761
New York: International Universities Press. 0.00621.

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39:938–945 945

Howlin, P., & Asgharian, A. (1999). The diagnosis of autism and Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 30, 865–878.
Asperger syndrome: Findings from a survey of 770 families. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00288.x.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 834–839. Mattila, M.-L., Kielinen, M., Jussila, K., Linna, S. L., Bloigu, R.,
doi:10.1017/S0012162299001656. Ebeling, H., et al. (2007). An epidemiological and diagnostic
Kaland, N., Smith, L., & Mortensen, E. L. (2007). Response times of study of Asperger syndrome according to four sets of diagnostic
children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome on an criteria. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
‘advanced’ test of theory of mind. Journal of Autism and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 636–646. doi:10.1097/chi.0b013e3
Developmental Disorders, 37, 197–209. doi:10.1007/s10803- 18033ff42.
006-0152-8. Melfsen, S., Walitza, S., & Warnke, A. (2006). The extent of social
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). anxiety in combination with mental disorders. European Child &
Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social Adolescent Psychiatry, 15, 111–117. doi:10.1007/s00787-006-
situations as predictors social competence in individuals with 0510-2.
autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816. doi: Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and
10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809. manual gestures in human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78. doi:
Klucharev, V., & Sams, M. (2004). Interaction of gaze direction and 10.1126/science.198.4312.75.
facial expressions processing: ERP study. Neuroreport, 15(4), Montagne, B., Kessels, R. P., De Haan, E. H., & Perret, D. I. (2007).
621–626. doi:10.1097/00001756-200403220-00010. The emotion recognition task: A paradigm to measure the
Kohler, C. G., Turner, T., Stolar, N. M., Bilker, W. B., Brensinger, C. perception of facial emotional expressions at different intensi-
M., Gur, R. E., et al. (2004). Differences in facial expressions of ties. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 104, 589–598. doi:10.2466/
four universal emotions. Psychiatry Research, 128, 235–244. PMS.104.2.589-598.
doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2004.07.003. Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B.
Korpilahti, P., Jansson-Verkasalo, E., Mattila, M.-L., Kuusikko, S., D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism.
Suominen, K., Rytky, S., et al. (2007). Processing of affective Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–261.
speech prosody is impaired in Asperger syndrome. Journal of doi:10.1023/A:1016374617369.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1539–1549. doi: Rutherford, M. D., & McIntosh, D. N. (2007). Rules versus prototype
10.1007/s10803-006-0271-2. matching: Strategies of perception of emotional facial expres-
Kujala, T., Lepistö, T., Nieminen-von Wendt, T., Näätänen, P., & sions in the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and
Näätänen, R. (2005). Neurophysiological evidence for cortical Developmental Disorders, 37, 187–196. doi:10.1007/s10803-
discrimination impairment of prosody in Asperger syndrome. 006-0151-9.
Neuroscience Letters, 383, 260–266. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005. Sigman, M. D., Kasari, C., Kwon, J. H., & Yirmiya, N. (1992).
04.048. Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic,
Kuusikko, S., Pollock-Wurman, R., Ebeling, H., Hurtig, T., Joskitt, mentally retarded and normal children. Child Development, 63,
L., Mattila, M.-L., Jussila, K., & Moilanen, I. (2008a). Psycho- 796–807. doi:10.2307/1131234.
metric evaluation of social phobia and anxiety inventory for Silver, M., & Oakes, P. (2001). Evaluation of a new computer
children (SPAI-C) and social anxiety scale for children-revised intervention to teach people with autism or Asperger syndrome
(SASC-R). [Epub ahead of print]. to recognize and predict emotions in others. Autism, 5, 299–316.
Kuusikko, S. Pollock-Wurman, R., Jussila, K., Carter, A. S., Mattila, doi:10.1177/1362361301005003007.
M.-L., Ebeling, H., Pauls, D. L., & Moilanen, I. (2008b). Social Smith, L. S., Dmochowski, P. A., Muir, D. W., & Kisilevsky, B. S.
anxiety in high-functioning children and adolescent with autism (2007). Estimated cardiac vagal tone predicts fetal responses to
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental mother’s and stranger’s voices. Developmental Psychobiology,
Disorders, [Epub ahead of print]. 49, 543–547. doi:10.1002/dev.20229.
Laihaie, A., Mottron, L., Arguin, M., Berthiaume, C., Jemel, B., & Tonks, J., Williams, W. H., Frampton, I., Yates, P., & Slater, A.
Saumier, D. (2006). Face perception in high-functioning autistic (2007). Assessing emotion recognition in 9–15-years olds:
adults: Evidence for superior processing of face parts, not for Preliminary analysis of abilities in reading emotion from faces,
configural face-processing deficit. Neuropsychology, 20, 30–41. voices and eyes. Brain Injury: [BI], 21, 623–629. doi:10.1080/
doi:10.1037/0894-4105.20.1.30. 02699050701426865.
Lindner, J., & Rosén, L. (2006). Decoding of emotion through facial van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., & Engeland, H.
expression, prosody and verbal content in children and adoles- (2002). Gaze behaviour of children with pervasive developmen-
scents with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and tal disorder toward human faces: A fixation time study. Journal
Developmental Disorders, 36, 769–777. doi:10.1007/s10803- of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 43,
006-0105-2. 669–778. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00055.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2000). Autism Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children, third
diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western edition (WISC-III). New York: The Psychological Corporation.
Psychological Services. Woodhead, M., Barnes, P., Miell, D., & Oates, J. (1995). Develop-
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1995). Autism diagnostic mental perspectives on emotion. In P. Barnes (Ed.), Personal,
interview—revised (3rd ed.). Los Angeles: Western psycholog- social, and emotional development of children (pp. 136–176).
ical services. UK: Alden Press.
Macdonald, H., Rutter, M., Howlin, P., Rios, P., LeCouter, A., World Health Organization (WHO). (1993). Tenth revision of the
Evered, C., et al. (1989). Recognition and expression of international classification of disease. Geneva: World Health
emotional cues by autistic and normal adults. Journal of Child Organization.

123

You might also like