You are on page 1of 18

Developmental Psychology Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

2000, Vol. 36, No. 5, 596-613 0012-1649/00/S5.00 DOI: 10.1037//OOI2-1649.36.5.596

Development of Emergent Literacy and Early Reading Skills in Preschool


Children: Evidence From a Latent-Variable Longitudinal Study

Christopher J. Lonigan, Stephen R. Burgess, and Jason L. Anthony


Florida State University

Although research has identified oral language, print knowledge, and phonological sensitivity as
important emergent literacy skills for the development of reading, few studies have examined the
relations between these aspects of emergent literacy or between these skills during preschool and during
later reading. This study examined the joint and unique predictive significance of emergent literacy skills
for both later emergent literacy skills and reading in two samples of preschoolers. Ninety-six children
(mean age = 41 months, SD = 9.41) were followed from early to late preschool, and 97 children (mean
age = 60 months, SD = 5.41) were followed from late preschool to kindergarten or first grade. Structural
equation modeling revealed significant developmental continuity of these skills, particularly for letter
knowledge and phonological sensitivity from late preschool to early grade school, both of which were the
only unique predictors of decoding.

Reading skills provide a crucial piece of the foundation for in three children experience significant difficulties in learning to
children's academic success. Children who read early and well read (Adams, 1990). There is strong continuity between the skills
experience more print exposure and consequent growth in numer- with which children enter school and their later academic perfor-
ous knowledge domains (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997; Echols, mance. Those children who do experience early difficulties in
West, Stanovich, & Zehr, 1996; Morrison, Smith, & Dow- learning to read are likely to continue to experience reading
Ehrensberger, 1995). In contrast, children who lag behind in their problems throughout the school years (Baydar, Brooks-Gunn, &
reading skills receive less practice in reading than other children Furstenberg, 1993; Felton, 1998; Stevenson & Newman, 1986;
do (Allington, 1984), miss opportunities to develop reading com- Tramontana, Hooper, & Selzer, 1988) and into adulthood (Bruck,
prehension strategies (Brown, Palincsar, & Purcell, 1986), often 1998). For instance, Juel (1988) reported that the probability that
encounter reading material that is too advanced for their skills children would remain poor readers at the end of the fourth grade
(Allington, 1984), and may acquire negative attitudes about read- if they were poor readers at the end of the first grade was .88.
ing itself (Oka & Paris, 1986). Such processes may lead to what Children who enter school with limited reading-related skills are at
Stanovich (e.g., 1986) termed a Matthew effect, in which poor high risk of qualifying for special education services. In fact, the
reading skills impede learning in other academic areas (Chall, majority of school-age children referred for special education
Jacobs, & Baldwin, 1990), which increasingly depend on reading evaluation are referred because of unsatisfactory progress in read-
across the school years. ing (Lentz, 1988).
Although the development of skilled reading occurs without
Whereas more traditional approaches to the study of reading
significant problems for the majority of children, an estimated one
often take as their starting point children's entry into the formal
school environment, an emergent literacy approach conceptualizes
Christopher J. Lonigan, Stephen R. Burgess, and Jason L. Anthony, the acquisition of literacy as a developmental continuum with its
Department of Psychology, Florida State University. origins early in the life of a child, rather than as an all-or-none
Stephen R. Burgess is now at the Department of Psychology, South- phenomenon that begins when children start school. An emergent
western Oklahoma State University. literacy approach departs from other perspectives on reading ac-
Preparation of this article was supported, in part, by grants from the quisition in suggesting that there is no clear demarcation between
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (HD36067, reading and prereading. Emergent literacy consists of the skills,
HD36509) and the Administration for Children and Families (90-YF-
knowledge, and attitudes that are presumed to be developmental
0023); the views expressed herein are the authors' and have not been
cleared by the grantors. precursors to conventional forms of reading and writing (Sulzby &
We wish to acknowledge the contributions of the child-care centers, the Teale, 1991; Teale & Sulzby, 1986; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998),
directors, and the personnel who assisted with this project as well as the and thus it suggests that significant sources of individual differ-
children and parents who made it possible. We thank Sarah Dyer, Brenlee ences in children's later reading skills are present prior to school
Bloomfield, Crystal Carr, Tracy Ferguson, Kimberly Ingram, Danielle entry. Previous research has identified a number of potentially
Karlau, Nikki Sutton, Emily Shock, and other students at Florida State important components of emergent literacy. Whitehurst and Loni-
University for their assistance with data collection.
gan (1998) recently outlined different components of emergent
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Christo-
pher J. Lonigan, Department of Psychology, Florida State University, literacy skills and identified three factors that appear to be asso-
Tallahassee Florida 32306-1270. Electronic mail may be sent to ciated with preschool children's later word-decoding abilities: oral
lonigan@psy.fsu.edu. language, phonological processing abilities, and print knowledge.

596
EMERGENT LITERACY AND EARLY READING 597

Reading is a process of translating visual codes into meaningful product names from signs and logos) reflects their early print
language. In the earliest stages, reading in an alphabetic system awareness by demonstrating the ability to derive the meaning of
involves decoding letters into corresponding sounds and linking text within context (e.g., Goodman, 1986).
those sounds to single words. A substantial body of research has Despite some evidence for associations between emergent liter-
demonstrated positive correlations and longitudinal continuity be- acy and later reading, there have been relatively few studies
tween individual differences in oral language skills and later examining the relations between these multidimensional aspects of
differences in reading (e.g., Bishop & Adams, 1990; Butler, emergent literacy or between these components during the pre-
Marsh, Sheppard, & Sheppard, 1985; Pikulski & Tobin, 1989; school period and later reading skills. As noted above, aspects of
Scarborough. 1989; Share, Jorm, MacLean, & Mathews, 1984). oral language appear to be related to phonological sensitivity.
Whereas the connection between oral language and reading is clear Children's letter knowledge also appears to be associated with
for reading comprehension (e.g., Snow, Barnes, Chandler, Hernp- some aspects of phonological sensitivity (Bowey, 1994; Stahl &
hill, & Goodman, 1991), some studies indicate that vocabulary Murray, 1994) and growth in these skills (Burgess &. Lonigan,
skills also have a significant impact on decoding skills very early 1998; Wagner et al., 1994, 1997). Evidence from school-age
in the process of learning to read (e.g., Wagner et al.. 1997). children indicates that these three components of emergent literacy
Additionally, oral language appears to be related to a second are causally related to each other and to later reading (e.g., Wagner
emergent literacy skill, phonological sensitivity, as defined below. et al., 1997); however, how they relate to each other during the
Studies of both preschool (e.g., Burgess & Lonigan, 1998; Chaney, preschool period is not known. Consequently, it is not clear
1992; Lonigan, Burgess, Anthony, & Barker, 1998) and early whether there are interactions between these different emergent
elementary school children (e.g., Bowey, 1994; Wagner, Torgesen, literacy skills or whether they are relatively independent of one
Laughon, Simmons, & Rashotte, 1993; Wagner et al., 1997) have another, and thus a well-elaborated developmental model of pre-
demonstrated significant concurrent and longitudinal correlations school emergent literacy and its relation to conventional literacy
between children's vocabulary skills and their phonological cannot be advanced. Moreover, basic questions concerning the
sensitivity. nature of preschool phonological sensitivity currently remain un-
answered, as discussed below.
Phonological sensitivity refers to sensitivity to and ability to
manipulate the sound structure of oral language. Research with a The majority of evidence linking phonological sensitivity in
variety of populations and using diverse methods has converged on prereaders with the development of reading has come from studies
the finding that phonological sensitivity plays a critical and causal that have assessed phonological skills at the point of school entry
role in the normal acquisition of reading (e.g., Adams, 1990; Byrne but prior to formal reading instruction (e.g., Bradley & Bryant,
& Fielding-Bamsley. 1991; Slanovich, 1992; Wagner & Torgesen, 1983, 1985; Share et al., 1984; Stanovich, Cunningham, & Cra-
1987). Children who are better at detecting and manipulating mer, 1984; Wagner etal., 1994, 1997). Compared with research on
syllables, rhymes, or phonemes are quicker to learn to read, and school-age children's phonological sensitivity, there has been sig-
this relation is present even after variability in reading skill owing nificantly less systematic study of preschool children's phonolog-
to factors such as IQ, receptive vocabulary, memory skills, and ical sensitivity, and many of these studies have been limited by
social class is partialed out (e.g., Bryant, MacLean, Bradley, & small sample sizes, use of only one or two measures of phonolog-
Crossland, 19%; Wagner & Torgesen, 1987; Wagner, Torgesen, & ical sensitivity, and other methodological weaknesses.
Rashotte, 1994). Moreover, studies of disabled and poor readers In one of the more extensive studies to date, MacLean, Bryant,
indicate that there is a core phonological deficit in nearly all poor and Bradley (1987) administered a rhyme detection task and a
readers regardless of whether their reading abilities are consistent knowledge-of-nursery-rhymes task to a group of 66 three-year-old
or inconsistent with their general cognitive abilities (Stanovich, children. When the children were 4'/2 years old, their ability to
1988; Stanovich & Siegel, 1994; Torgesen, 1999). read 12 simple high-frequency words was assessed. Compared
In addition to oral language and phonological sensitivity, as- with nonreaders, children who could read some of these words
pects of children's print knowledge seem to be important emergent scored higher on the earlier rhyme and alliteration measures.
literacy skills. For example, knowledge of the alphabet (i.e., know- Bryant et al. (1990) reported additional data on these children, who
ing the names of letters and the sounds they represent) at entry into completed additional rhyme and alliteration detection tasks when
school is one of the strongest single predictors of short- and they were about 4'/i years old, phoneme deletion and phoneme
long-term success in learning to read (e.g., Adams, 1990; Steven- tapping tasks when they averaged about 6 years of age, and reading
son & Newman, 1986). Understanding the conventions of print and spelling tests when they averaged about 6V2 years of age.
(e.g., left-to-right and top-to-bottom orientation of print, the dif- Bryant et al. found that the rhyme and alliteration detection tests
ference between pictures and print on a page; Clay, 1979a, 1979b) that had been administered when the children were AV2 were
and the functions of print (e.g., that the print tells a story or gives correlated with the later phoneme deletion and phoneme tapping
directions; Purcell-Gates, 1996; Purcell-Gates & Dahl, 1991) also tasks (average r = .48), and scores on these rhyme and alliteration
appears to aid in the process of learning to read. For example, tasks significantly added to the prediction of reading and spelling
Tunmer, Herriman, and Nesdale (1988) found that children's scores independent of mothers' educational level, child age, IQ,
scores on Clay's (1979a) Concepts About Print (CAP) Test at the receptive vocabulary, and the score on either the phoneme deletion
beginning of first grade predicted their reading comprehension and or phoneme tapping tasks.
decoding abilities at the end of second grade even after Tunmer et Evidence suggests that there is a developmental hierarchy of
al. controlled for differences in vocabulary and metalinguistic children's sensitivity to linguistic units at different levels of com-
awareness. Some emergent literacy advocates have also suggested plexity. Children achieve syllabic sensitivity earlier than they
that children's faculty with environmental print (e.g., recognizing achieve sensitivity to phonemes, and children's sensitivity to in-
598 LONIGAN, BURGESS, AND ANTHONY

trasyllabic units (i.e., onset-rime) also precedes sensitivity to pho- and phoneme sensitivity (see also Anthony & Lonigan, 2000). In
nemes (Fox & Routh, 1975; I. Liberman, Shankweiler, Fischer, & contrast to these findings, however, Hoien, Lundberg, Stanovich,
Carter, 1974; Lonigan et al., 1998; Treiman, 1992). However, and Bjaalid (1995) reported evidence for the distinction between
there is controversy concerning whether sensitivity to lower levels sensitivity to phonemes and sensitivity to rhyme and syllables.
of linguistic complexity (i.e., syllables, onset-rime) represents pro- They found separate factors for phonemic sensitivity, syllabic
cesses important for reading. Sensitivity to phonemes is often sensitivity, and rhyme sensitivity in 6- and 8-year-old Norwegian
assumed to have special status in the relation between phonolog- children, and scores on all three factors independently predicted
ical sensitivity and reading both because it is at this level that reading abilities for the older group of children. However, it is
graphemes correspond to speech sounds in reading and because
difficult to interpret the results of Hoein et al. because only one
individual phonemes do not have separable physical reality (e.g.,
task defined the Rhyme Sensitivity and Syllabic Sensitivity
A. Liberman, Cooper, Shankweiler, & Studdert-Kennedy, 1967;
factors.
Morais, 1991; Muter, Hulme, Snowling, & Taylor, 1997; Nation &
Hulme, 1997; Tunmer & Rohl, 1991). Other authors have sug- Several predictive studies also support a unitary view of pho-
gested that children's abilities to detect rhyme facilitate reading nological sensitivity. For example, Lonigan et al. (1998) demon-
through a mechanism different than sensitivity to phonemes (e.g., strated that preschool children's performance on tasks measur-
Goswami & Bryant, 1990, 1992). ing phonological sensitivity at the syllable, onset-rime, and pho-
Both of these views, however, assume that there is more than neme levels was associated with measures of letter knowledge and
one type of phonological sensitivity and that the different types decoding. Similarly, studies of school-age children by Wagner and
may be more or less related to reading. Most extant studies colleagues (Wagner et al., 1993, 1994, 1997) found that a latent
compared the ability of different phonological sensitivity measures variable defined by rhyme, syllable-level, and phoneme-level tasks
(e.g., measures of rhyme vs. phonemic sensitivity) to predict was associated strongly, concurrently, and longitudinally with
reading. In these studies, when one measure of phonological children's reading skills. These findings indicate that the variance
sensitivity predicts reading better—typically defined as a signifi- common to phonological sensitivity tasks measuring different lev-
cant semi-partial correlation obtained while controlling for the els of linguistic complexity represents the predictive aspect of the
other measure of phonological sensitivity—the results are taken as phonological sensitivity construct. However, the majority of pre-
supporting the crucial importance of the skill supposedly measured dictive studies have involved either school-age children or rela-
by that task (e.g., phonemic sensitivity). For example, in support of tively small samples. Consequently, little is known about the
the importance of phonemic sensitivity, both Muter et al. (1997) nature and predictive importance of preschool children's develop-
and Nation and Hulme (1997) reported that children's abilities to ing phonological sensitivity.
perform a phoneme segmentation task were more strongly related
to reading and spelling than were their abilities to detect and Questions concerning the nature of preschool phonological sen-
produce rhyme. Goswami and Bryant (1992) also reported data sitivity (i.e., whether it is a unitary or a multidimensional con-
consistent with separate domains of phonological sensitivity. In struct), the independence of phonological sensitivity, oral lan-
their study, when they controlled for phonemic sensitivity, rhym- guage, and print knowledge, as well as the significance of these
ing abilities facilitated children's abilities to make use of analogy three components of emergent literacy for later reading are impor-
in reading unfamiliar words. There are several problems with this tant because studies demonstrate that there are highly stable indi-
predictive approach. First, it assumes a priori that there are differ- vidual differences in these abilities from kindergarten forward
ent types of phonological sensitivity. Second, it ignores the fact (Wagner et al., 1994, 1997). Such findings suggest that the pre-
that the most predictive element in such studies is the overlap school period is an important source of development in skills
between measures (i.e., the degree of shared predictive variance is associated with later reading. Our goals in this study were to
substantially larger than the degree of unique predictive variance examine the nature of preschool emergent literacy as well as the
for a given test). Finally, these types of analyses fail to take into joint and unique predictive significance of preschool emergent
account the effects of differential reliability of the measures. That
literacy skills for later reading. We examined the development of
is, the more reliable of two variables measuring equally predictive
emergent literacy and early reading longitudinally in two samples
constructs (or the same construct) generally will capture the unique
of preschool children who overlapped in age at different assess-
predictive variance.
ment points and in the measures they completed. We used struc-
Prior to examining differential predictive validity, therefore, it is tural equation modeling to address questions about the nature of
important that we determine whether there is more than one type preschool phonological sensitivity, the independence of different
of phonological sensitivity. Extant evidence suggests that sensitiv- emergent literacy skills, and the developmental significance of
ity to onset-rimes, syllables, and phonemes represents the same these skills across time from the early preschool period to kinder-
underlying ability. For example, Stahl and Murray (1994) admin- garten and first grade.
istered to 113 kindergarten and first-grade children four different
tasks varying in linguistic complexity. Separate factor analyses of
the four tasks across linguistic complexity and of the four levels of Method
linguistic complexity across tasks each yielded a single-factor
solution that explained a majority of the variance in children's Participants
performance on the measures. Using confirmatory factor analysis,
Anthony et al. (2000) found that a single factor provided the best Data from two groups of preschool-age children, recruited through 13
fit to preschool children's scores on measures of rhyme, syllable, different preschools and child-care centers serving middle- to upper-
EMERGENT LITERACY AND EARLY READING 599

income families, were used in this study.1 One group of children consisted sessions within a 2-3-week period to ensure optimal performance on all
of 96 younger preschoolers who completed follow-up testing (i.e., Time 2) tasks. Children in the younger sample completed four standardized tests of
approximately 18 months after their initial (i.e., Time 1) assessments. oral language, four tests of phonological sensitivity, and two tests of
These children ranged in age from 25 to 61 months (M = 41.02 months, nonverbal cognitive ability during Time 1 testing, and they completed four
SD = 9.41) at Time 1. The majority of the younger group of children were tests of phonological sensitivity, two tests of letter knowledge, an envi-
White (89.6%), and 56.3% were girls. The second group of children ronmental print task, and a print concepts task during Time 2 testing.
consisted of 97 older preschoolers who completed follow-up tests (i.e., Children in the older sample completed one test of oral language, four tests
Time 2) approximately 12 months after their initial (i.e., Time 1) testing. of phonological sensitivity, two tests of letter knowledge, an environmental
This group of children ranged in age from 48 to 64 months (M = 60.04 print task, and a print concepts task during Time 1 testing, and they
months, SD = 5.41) at Time 1. Most of the older group of children were completed four tests of phonological sensitivity, two tests of letter knowl-
White (96.9%), and 52.6% were girls. Only children who had completed all edge, a print concepts task, and two text decoding tasks during Time 2
measures at both Time 1 and Time 2 were included in these samples. An testing.
additional 58 children (18 from the older sample) who had incomplete data Phonological sensitivity measures. Each of the four phonological sen-
on measures at Time 1 or Time 2 either because they refused continued sitivity tasks was preceded by practice trials to teach children the task (e.g.,
participation or because they could not be located for the Time 2 testing blending or deleting word sounds). For all tasks, corrective feedback was
were excluded from the results reported in this study. With the exception given during the practice trials, but no feedback was given during the test
that excluded children in the younger sample scored lower on the rhyme trials. Many items on the phonological sensitivity tasks used pictures to
oddity task (p = .04), excluded children did not differ from included reduce memory demands on the children. Except as noted below, tasks
children on any measure. administered at Time 1 and Time 2 and to younger and older children
These two samples of children originally were recruited for separate but included the same items. Previous analyses of these four tasks indicated
related research projects, and there was substantial overlap in the primary that they had moderate to high internal consistencies for 4-year-olds (as =
measures administered in each project. The grouping used in this study .47 to .96) and 5-year-olds (as = .69 to .94) but lower internal consisten-
maintained the original division between samples because of the different cies for 2- and 3-year-olds (see Lonigan et al., 1998).
follow-up periods associated with each and the commonality of measures
A rhyme oddity detection task and an alliteration oddity detection task,
administered at each assessment within a sample. Information concerning
patterned after the tasks developed by MacLean et al. (1987) and using
children's family and home literacy experiences obtained on a majority of
their word lists, required children to demonstrate awareness of rhyme or
participants (85%) revealed that the samples were similar to each other on
awareness of singleton word onsets. In both tasks, children were presented
these variables. English was the primary language spoken in the home for
with three pictured words (e.g., boat, sail, nail; car, cat, sun), which were
all children, and fewer than three mothers or three fathers reported that
named by the examiner, and were asked to select the one that did not rhyme
English was not their native language. Mothers and fathers of children in
with (or that sounded different from) or did not sound the same at the
both samples had completed on average 16 years of education (in both
beginning of the word as (or that sounded different at the beginning of the
samples over 70% of mothers and over 63% of fathers were college
word from) the other two words. Two practice trials and 11 test trials were
graduates). Parents reported a significant number of children's books in the
presented to all children.
home for both the younger (M = 89.47, SD = 67.5) and older (M =
A blending task required children to combine word elements to form a
137.75, SD = 92.9) samples. Children in both samples were reported to be
word. Three practice items and the first eight test trials were presented both
read to frequently at home (younger sample, M = 6.69 times per week,
verbally and with pictures; the remaining test trials were presented verbally
SD = 3.22; older sample, M = 5.71, SD = 2.35), and shared reading had
only. In both picture and nonpicture trials, the first five items required
started early for children in both the younger (M = 6.37 months,
blending single-syllable words to form compound words, and the remain-
SD = 6.18) and older (M = 7.33 months, SD = 4.48) samples.
ing items required blending syllables or phonemes. For picture items
involving compound words, the examiner showed the child two pictures,
Preschool and Child-Care Centers named them, and then asked the child what word would be produced if he
or she said them together (e.g., "What do you get when you say cow . . .
Although we did not conduct formal observations of the centers, our boy together?"). All practice items required the blending of compound
multiple opportunities for observation allowed us to identify salient fea- words, and during the practice the examiner emphasized the nature of the
tures of their educational environments. There was significant variability task by putting the pictures together. For the Time 1 assessment of the older
between centers in terms of materials available to the children and activity sample and both Time 1 and Time 2 assessments of the younger sample,
structure. Generally, the curriculum in the centers was designed to foster there were 18 test trials, consisting of 10 word-blending items, 4 syllable-
social and interpersonal skill growth and to introduce the children to a blending items, and 4 phoneme-blending items. At the Time 2 assessment
variety of educationally relevant concepts such as letters, numbers, and of the older children, there were 37 test trials, consisting of all of the
storybooks. We never observed any explicit attempts to teach the children Time 1 items followed by 3 additional syllable-blending items and 16
to read in any center. Some of the centers had at least some letter additional phoneme-blending items. These additional syllable and pho-
knowledge instruction, but most of this was informal. A number of direc- neme items were included in the older children's Time 2 assessment to
tors commented that they discouraged explicit teaching. The centers had reduce the chances of children's scoring at ceiling levels. During both
similar daily activity schedules, including free play, storytime, and small- assessments, testing was discontinued after a child missed 5 consecutive
group arts and crafts projects. Each of the centers incorporated some
trials.
teacher-directed classroom activities (typically arts and crafts); however,
the majority of children's time was spent in self-directed activities in and An elision task required children to say a word minus a specific sound.
out of the classroom. Two practice items and the first eight test trials were presented both

1
Procedures and Measures These children represent a subset of the children from middle-income
families included in Lonigan et al. (1998). The results reported previously
After parents provided informed consent for their children to participate, concerned age- and SES-related performance differences in phonological
trained research assistants tested children individually in their centers. Test sensitivity tasks from the children's initial assessment (i.e., Time 1 in the
administration for individual children was conducted over two to four present study).
600 LONIGAN, BURGESS, AND ANTHONY

verbally and with pictures; the remaining test trials were presented verbally Mastery Test—Revised (Woodcock, 1987) and a task requiring them to
only. In both picture and nonpicture trials, the first four items required decode 25 frequent words printed individually on 3 X 5 in. index cards.
deleting a single-syllable word from a compound word to form a new word.
Subsequent items in both picture and nonpicture trials required deletion of
a syllable or a phoneme from a word to form a new word. For picture items
Results
involving compound words, the examiner showed the child two pictures,
named them (e.g., "This is a bat, and this is a man."), asked the child to say
Descriptive Statistics and Preliminary Analyses
the compound (i.e., "batman"), and then asked the child to delete part of it.
Separate scores for word, syllable, and phoneme items on the
During both practice trials, which used compound words, the examiner
blending and elision tasks were computed. Descriptive statistics
emphasized the nature of the task by removing the picture of the word to
be deleted. For the Time 1 assessment of the older children and both for raw scores on all variables for the younger sample at both
Time 1 and Time 2 assessments of the younger children, there were 17 test Time 1 and Time 2 are shown in Table 1. Descriptive statistics for
trials, consisting of 10 word-level items, 4 syllable-level items, and 3 raw scores on all variables for the older sample at both Time 1 and
phoneme-level items. At the Time 2 assessment of the older children, there Time 2 are shown in Table 2. The tables also list the internal
were 34 test trials, consisting of all of the Time 1 items followed by an consistency reliabilities for the eight phonological sensitivity
additional 17 phoneme-level items. These additional phoneme items were scores at both assessments; with a few exceptions, these reliabili-
included in the older children's Time 2 assessment to reduce the chances ties were at least moderate. Analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
of children's scoring at ceiling levels. During both assessments, testing was revealed that the older sample scored substantially higher than the
discontinued after a child missed 5 consecutive trials.
younger sample on the phonological sensitivity measures at
Oral language and cognitive ability measures. At Time 1, children in Time 1 (all ps < .001). For all tasks that were the same between
the younger sample completed four standardized tests of oral language. Time 1 and Time 2 assessments (i.e., all phonological sensitivity
Receptive vocabulary was assessed with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary
tasks for the younger sample and the letter knowledge and word-
Tests—Revised (PPVT-R; Dunn & Dunn, 1981). Expressive vocabulary
level phonological sensitivity tasks for the older sample), within-
was assessed with the Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test—
Revised (EOWPVT-R; Gardner, 1990). The Verbal Expression subtest of subject ANOVAs revealed that there was significant growth from
the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities (ITPA-VE; Kirk, McCarthy, Time 1 to Time 2 (all ps < .001).
& Kirk, 1968) was used to assess children's descriptive use of language, Standardized scores for both samples were computed by re-
and the Grammatical Closure subtest of the Illinois Test of Psycholinguis- gressing chronological age onto the raw score for each variable
tic Abilities (ITPA-GC; Kirk et al., 1968) was used to assess children's within sample and time of assessment (i.e., Time 1 and Time 2) to
expressive grammar. At Time 1 for the older children, oral language was remove statistically the reliable variance that was due to children's
assessed with the ITPA-GC. In addition to these oral language measures, chronological age from the scores on the observed variables.
children in the younger sample completed the Picture Completion and
Inspection of the distribution of scores for each variable revealed
Object Assembly subtests from the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales
some moderate departures from normality (i.e., skew) but no
of Intelligence—Revised (Wechsler, 1989) at Time 1.
obvious outliers. Further inspection revealed that positive skew for
Letter knowledge measures. For the Time 2 assessment of the younger
the younger children's Time 1 scores was due to a moderate
sample and both Time 1 and Time 2 assessments of the older sample, two
tasks assessed different aspects of letter knowledge. A letter-name knowl- number of children scoring at low levels on the blending and
edge task required children to name all 26 uppercase letters that were elision tasks, whereas negative skew for the older children's scores
presented individually in random order on individual 3 X 5 in. index cards. was due to a moderate number of children scoring at high levels on
A letter-sound knowledge task required children to name the sound made the word blending, word elision, and letter knowledge tasks. Al-
by each letter when it appeared in a word. All 26 uppercase letters were though these distributions accurately reflect task difficulty for the
presented individually in random order on individual 3 X 5 in. index cards. age ranges included in the samples, the use of nonnormal data may
If children responded with the letter name or a word that started with the attenuate relations among variables and compromise model fits;
letter (e.g., "dog" for D), they were prompted to provide the letter sound; consequently, we conducted confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)
however, credit for a correct response was given if children provided the
using robust maximum-likelihood estimation, the Satorra-Bentler
long vowel sound for vowels.
scaled chi-square (S-B;^2), and adjustments to the standard errors
Environmental print measures. The older sample at Time 1 and the
to account for nonnormality in model fit statistics and significance
younger sample at Time 2 completed an environmental print task. On this
testing (Bentler & Dudgeon, 1996).
task, children were shown 11 pictures of print in environmental context
(e.g., a stop sign, a Coke machine, a McDonald's sign) and were asked
what each said. Children were also shown the same print as printed text out Evaluation of Measurement Models
of context and were asked what it said.
Print concepts measure. At Time 2 for the younger sample and at both We conducted separate CFAs using EQS (Bentler, 1995) to
Time 1 and Time 2 for the older sample, portions of Clay's (1979a) CAP evaluate measurement models for both samples at Time 1 and
test (the"Sand" task) were used to assess the children's print knowledge. Time 2. All CFAs were conducted on covariance matrices. Prior to
Items in this test require children to demonstrate understanding of the evaluating the adequacy of measurement models that included all
left-to-right and top-to-bottom direction of print in a book, the sequence
emergent literacy tasks, we evaluated the adequacy of a one-factor
and direction in which print progresses from front to back across pages, the
model to explain scores on the phonological sensitivity tasks at
difference between the covers and the pages of a book, the difference
each measurement period for both samples. In all models, task
between pictures and print on a page, and the meaning of elements of
punctuation, including spaces between words and periods at the ends of variance for the different phonological sensitivity tasks was mod-
sentences. eled by allowing correlated residuals between similar tasks (i.e.,
Word decoding measures. At Time 2, children in the older sample parameter estimates for covariances between error terms for the
completed the Word Identification subtest of the Woodcock Reading two oddity tasks, three blending tasks, and three elision tasks were
EMERGENT LITERACY AND EARLY READING 601

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for Younger Sample of Children at Time 1 and Time 2 Assessments

Time 1 Time 2

Variable M SD a M SD a

Age (in months) 41.05 9.36 57.56 10.09


Rhyme oddity 4.54 2.00 .30 6.93 2.52 .90
Alliteration oddity 3.45 1.81 .18 5.55 2.66 .85
Blending words 2.57 4.47 .97 7.30 5.88 .98
Blending syllables 0.68 1.35 .90 1.51 1.34 .89
Blending phonemes 0.25 0.62 .52 1.31 1.48 .87
Elision words 1.77 2.77 .91 5.73 3.83 .96
Elision syllables 0.43 0.95 .79 1.95 1.59 .88
Elision phonemes 0.22 0.70 .86 1.12 1.20 .85
PPVT-R (MA) 42.54 11.60 —
EOWPVT-R (MA) 42.71 12.90 — —
ITPA-VE (MA) 48.54 13.09 — —
ITPA-GC (MA) 45.48 14.87 — —
WPSSI Object Assembly 12.40 5.31 — —
WPSSI Picture Completion 10.48 6.77 — —
Letter names — — 14.51 10.06
Letter sounds — — 6.84 8.32
Concepts About Print Test — — 7.26 3.32
Environmental print: pictures — — 5.22 2.69
Environmental print: text — — 0.97 2.13

Note. N = 96. All means are for raw scores unless otherwise noted. Internal consistency reliabilities (alphas)
are provided only for phonological sensitivity measures. Dashes indicate tasks not administered at an assessment
period. PPVT-R = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised; MA = mental age score; EOWPVT-R =
Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised; ITPA-VE = Verbal Expression subscale of the
Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities; ITPA-GC = Grammatical Closure subtest of the Illinois Test of
Psycholinguistic Abilities; WPPSI = Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence.

specified in the models).2 For the younger sample at Time 1, phonological sensitivity, letter knowledge, and environmental
S-B^CB, N = 96) = 13.15, p > .25, RCF1 (robust comparative print measures were compared. These models also included the
fit index) = 1.00, and Time 2, S-Bx^B.iV = 96) = 5.23,p > .25, CAP test as a separate measured variable. A three-factor model
RCFI = 1.00, and for the older sample at Time 1, S-Bx^B, N = that included different Phonological Sensitivity, Letter Knowl-
97) = 17.89, p > .10, RCFI = .98, and Time 2, S - B ^ D , N = edge, and Environmental Print factors provided a significantly
97) = 10.78, p > .25, RCFI = 1.00, a one-factor model provided better fit than the one-factor model, XdiffC5- N - 96) = 49.34, p <
an excellent fit to the data. Following these analyses, different .001, a two-factor model with phonological sensitivity and letter
one-, two-, and three-factor measurement models that included all knowledge measures represented by a single factor, Xdiff(3, N =
emergent literacy tasks were compared in the younger and older 96) = 42.07, p < .001, and a two-factor model with phonological
samples at the Time 1 and Time 2 assessments. sensitivity and environmental print measures represented by a
Younger sample. Fit indices for the different measurement single factor, *jjiff(3, N = 96) = 39.12, p < .001. The two-factor
models for the younger sample of children are shown in Table 3. model with letter knowledge and environmental print measures
For the Time 1 assessment (upper half of Table 3), the fits of represented by a single factor was not significantly different from
models that included different combinations of phonological sen- the three-factor model (p > .10); however, when the CAP measure
sitivity, oral language, and nonverbal IQ measures were compared. was excluded from the model, the three-factor model provided a
A three-factor model with separate Phonological Sensitivity, Oral better fit to the data, xjiiff(2, N = 96) = 6.40, p < .05, supporting
Language, and Nonverbal IQ factors provided a significantly better
the use of three separate factors to represent phonological sensi-
fit than all of the alternative models (all ps < .01 for chi-square
tivity, letter knowledge, and environmental print.
difference tests) except the model with the phonological sensitivity
and oral language measures represented by one factor. The differ- Older sample. Fit indices for the different measurement mod-
ence (diff) between the three-factor model and this two-factor els for the older sample of children are shown in Table 4. For the
model was only marginally significant, ^ iff <2, N = 96) = 4.09, Time 1 assessment (see upper half of Table 4), the fits of models
p = .11; however, examination of the other fit indices (Bentler &
Bonett, 1980; see Table 3) and factor loadings, which indicated 2
The structure of all measurement and longitudinal models was identical
that the majority of phonological sensitivity tasks did not load
whether or not these correlated residuals were included in the models;
significantly on the factor, supported the superiority of the three-
however, model fits were improved when correlated residuals were in-
factor model. cluded because they accounted for significant covariance between items
For the younger sample's Time 2 assessment (see lower half of that was due to similar task methods (e.g., blending vs. deleting word
Table 3), the fits of models that included different combinations of sounds) or other sources of systematic variance.
602 LONIGAN, BURGESS, AND ANTHONY

Table 2
Descriptive Statistics for Older Sample of Children at Time 1 and Time 2 Assessments

Time 1 Time 2

Variable M SD a M SD a

Age (in months) 60.04 5.41 72.88 5.71


Rhyme oddity 6.49 2.75 .71 8.89 2.13 .71
Alliteration oddity 5.46 2.64 .68 8.73 2.42 .80
Blending words 7.73 2.95 .93 9.44 1.16 .75
Blending syllables 2.70 1.39 .69 10.16 2.70 .61
Blending phonemes 1.78 1.40 .67 6.96 3.65 .90
Elision words 5.59 2.37 .80 7.56 0.69 .50
Elision syllables 2.13 1.43 .70 2.32 0.97 .44
Elision phonemes 1.14 1.06 .57 6.42 3.87 .88
Letter names 20.02 7.37 24.72 3.68
Letter sounds 9.09 8.91 20.45 6.68
Concepts About Print Test 7.63 3.32 11.41 1.70
Environmental print: pictures 5.73 2.17 — —
Environmental print: text 0.97 1.91 —
ITPA-GC (MA) 68.64 16.22
Decoding frequent words — — 11.98 8.46
WRM Word ID — — 14.32 12.12

Note. N = 97. All means are for raw scores unless otherwise noted. Internal consistency reliabilities (alphas)
are provided only for phonological sensitivity measures. Dashes indicate tasks not administered at an assessment
period. ITPA-GC = Grammatical Closure subtest of the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities; MA = mental
age score; WRM Word ID = Word Identification subtest of the Woodcock Reading Mastery Test—Revised.

that included different combinations of phonological sensitiv- sensitivity and letter knowledge measures represented by a
ity, letter knowledge, and environmental print measures were single factor, )&i{f(3, N = 97) = 10.89, p < .05, a two-factor
compared. These models also included the CAP test as a sep- model with phonological sensitivity and environmental print
arate measured variable. Both chi-square difference tests and measures represented by a single factor, Xdiff(3, N =
evaluation of the other fit indices indicated that a three-factor 97) = 24.39, p < .001, and a two-factor model with letter
model that included separate Phonological Sensitivity, Letter knowledge and environmental print measures represented by a
Knowledge, and Environmental Print factors provided a signif- single factor, Xdiff(3, N = 97) = 18.91, p < .001, supporting the
icantly better fit than the one-factor model, ^ i f f ( 5 , N = use of three separate factors to represent phonological sensitiv-
97) = 32.49, p < .001, a two-factor model with phonological ity, letter knowledge, and environmental print.

Table 3
Fit Indices for Measurement Models for Younger Sample at Time 1 and Time 2 Assessments

Model (and factors) S-B*2 df CFI RCFI TLI RMSEA AIC

Time 1 Assessment
1-factor (PS + OL + IQ) 102.70** 70 .89 .91 .85 .10 -9.57
2-factor (PS + OL, IQ) 89.82* 69 .91 .94 .88 .09 -18.20
2-factor (PS + IQ, OL) 130.77*** 69 .86 .82 .82 .11 3.74
2-factor (PS, OL + IQ) 99.43** 69 .93 .91 .90 .08 -29.98
3-factor (PS, OL, IQ) 85.73 67 .94 .95 .92 .07 -36.14

Time 2 Assessment
1-factor (PS + LK + EP) 123.05*** 58 .88 .88 .84 .11 7.44
2-factor (PS + LK, EP) 115.78*** 56 .89 .89 .84 .11 5.25
2-factor (PS + EP, LK) 112.83*** 56 .90 .90 .85 .11 2.51
2-factor (PS, LK + EP) 80.45* 56 .96 .96 .94 .07 -32.85
3-factor (PS, LK, EP) 73.71* 53 .96 .96 .94 .07 -32.26

Note. All models include correlated residuals between like phonological sensitivity tasks. All models of Time 2
assessment include scores on the Concepts About Print Test as a measured variable. N = 96. PS = Phonological
Sensitivity; OL = Oral Language; IQ = Nonverbal IQ; LK = Letter Knowledge; EP = Environmental Print;
S-B^2 = Satorra-Bentler chi-square; CFI = comparative fit index; RCFI = robust comparative fit index; TLI =
Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; AIC = Akaike information criterion.
*p<.05. **p<.0l. ***/><.001.
EMERGENT LITERACY AND EARLY READING 603

Table 4
Fit Indices for Measurement Models for Older Sample at Time 1 and Time 2 Assessments

Model (and factors) S-B*2 df CFI RCFI TLI RMSEA AIC

Time 1 Assessment
1-factor 117.15*** 58 .87 .86 .82 .10 -0.34
2-factor (PS + LK, EP) 95.55*** 56 .91 .91 .88 .09 -18.16
2-factor (PS + EP, LK) 109.05*** 56 .88 .87 .84 .10 -4.68
2-factor (PS, LK + EP) 103.57*** 56 .89 .89 .85 .09 -10.24
3-factor (PS, LK, EP) 84.66** 53 .93 .92 .90 .08 -22.76

Time 2 Assessment
1-factor (PS + LK + RD) 103.00*** 58 .90 .90 .86 .10 -6.59
2-factor (PS + LK, RD) 69.08 56 .96 .97 .95 .06 -36.89
2-factor (PS + RD, LK) 79.18* 56 .95 .95 .93 .07 -30.50
2-factor (PS, LK + RD) 77.09* 56 .95 .95 .92 .07 -28.19
3-factor (PS, LK, RD) 51.50 53 1.00 1.00 1.00 .02 -51.38

Note. All models include scores on the Concepts About Print Test as a measured variable and correlated
residuals between like phonological sensitivity tasks. N = 97. PS = Phonological Sensitivity; LK = Letter
Knowledge; EP = Environmental Print; RD = Word Reading (decoding); S-B^2 = Satorra-Bentler chi-square;
CFI = comparative fit index; RCFI = robust comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root
mean square error of approximation; AIC = Akaike information criterion.
* p < . 0 5 . **p<. 01. ***/><.001.

For the older sample's Time 2 assessment (see lower half of rendered nonsignificant in an analysis of covariance controlling for
Table 4), the fits of models that included different combinations of chronological age (p = .16); however, the differences for letter
phonological sensitivity, letter knowledge, and text decoding were knowledge and syllable blending remained significant (ps <
compared. These models also included the CAP test as a separate .001).3
measured variable. Both chi-square difference tests and evaluation Multisample CFA was carried out on the data from the younger
of the other fit indices indicated that a three-factor model that children's Time 2 data and the older children's Time 1 data to
included different Phonological Sensitivity, Letter Knowledge, examine structural invariance of the three-factor measurement
and Reading (Decoding) factors provided a significantly better fit model across samples (see Table 5). A multisample model with
than the one-factor model, ^ i f f ( 5 , N = 97) = 51.50, p < .001, a separate Phonological Sensitivity, Letter Knowledge, and Envi-
two-factor model with phonological sensitivity and letter knowl- ronmental Print factors, with the CAP test as a separate measured
edge measures represented by a single factor, Xaiff(3, N =
variable and with none of the parameters across groups constrained
97) = 17.58, p < .001, a two-factor model with phonological
to equality, served as a basis for testing whether adding constraints
sensitivity and decoding measures represented by a single factor,
to the model across groups would yield a significantly worse fit.
x3iff(3, N = 97) = 27.68, p < .001, and a two-factor model with
letter knowledge and decoding measures represented by a single A significant change in the chi-square when factor loadings
factor, ^ i f f ( 3 , N = 97) = 25.59, p < .001, supporting the use of were constrained across groups suggested there was a statistically
three separate factors to represent phonological sensitivity, letter significant lack of invariance. However, fit indices that are more
knowledge, and text decoding. robust to sample size supported the invariance of factor loadings,
factor correlations (including correlations with the CAP measure),
Sample comparisons. To facilitate comparisons between
and correlated residuals. The comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-
younger and older samples and to allow preliminary hypotheses
Lewis Index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation
concerning the development of reading-related skills across the
(RMSEA), and Akaike information criterion (AIC) remained es-
age range covered by both samples (i.e., continuity between the
sentially unchanged when these invariance constraints were im-
younger sample's Time 1 assessment and the older sample's
Time 2 assessment), we compared raw scores and measurement posed, and the imposition of all of these constraints did not result
models for the emergent literacy measures from the younger in a significant reduction in the overall model chi-square from the
children at the Time 2 assessment with those from the older unconstrained model, ^ i f f (25, N = 193) = 32.79, p > .10.
children at the Time 1 assessment. ANOVA revealed that children Consequently, the majority of fit indices indicated that the slight
in the younger sample at Time 2 were somewhat younger than lack of invariance noted for the factor loadings was of little
children in the older sample at Time 1, F(l, 191) = 4.54, p = .03.
ANOVAs on children's raw scores also revealed that children in 3
These significant differences may have been the result of the age range
the younger sample at Time 2 scored lower on letter knowledge,
of the younger group. That is, the youngest child in the older sample
F(l, 191) = 18.86, p < .001, syllable blending, F(l, 191) = 36.87,
was 60 months old, whereas the youngest child in the younger sample
p < .001, and phoneme blending, F(l, 191) = 5.18, p = .02, than was 38 months old. Alternatively, these differences may have been a
did children in the older sample at Time 1 (see Table 1 and Table 2 function of the different preschool environments of the younger and older
for descriptive statistics). Differences on phoneme blending were samples.
604 LONIGAN, BURGESS, AND ANTHONY

Table 5
Fit Indices for Multisample Analysis of Three-Factor Measurement Model for Younger Sample
at Time 2 and Older Sample at Time 1

Model constraints x2 df CFI TLI RMSEA AIC Xdiff df


None (unconstrained) 156.98*** 106 .95 .92 .05 -55.02
Factor loadings 176.89*** 118 .94 .92 .05 -59.11 19.91* 10
Factor loadings and factor
correlations 185.34*** 124 .94 .92 .05 -62.66 8.45 6
Factor loadings, factor
correlations, and correlated
residuals 189.77*** 131 .94 .93 .05 -72.23 4.43 7
Factor loadings, factor
correlations, correlated
residuals, and residual 12
variances 221.87*** 143 .92 .91 .05 -64.13 32.10**

Note. All models include scores on the Concepts About Print Test as a measured variable and correlated
residuals between like phonological sensitivity tasks. Chi-square difference tests reflect comparison of a model
with the previous model and thus reflect the change associated with the addition of the specified constraint. N =
193. CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of
approximation; AIC = Akaike information criterion.
* p < . 0 5 . ** p < . 01. ***/?<.001.

practical importance and was outweighed by the large gain in the Time 2 Phonological Sensitivity factor, the Time 2 Letter
parsimony. In contrast, all fit indices decreased substantially when Knowledge factor, the Time 2 Environmental Print factor, and the
item residuals were constrained. Thus, the results indicated that the Time 2 CAP variable also were included in the model. Finally, a
measurement model explained children's emergent literacy skills path from the Nonverbal IQ factor to the Time 2 CAP variable was
well across both the younger and older samples of children (i.e., included.
the factor structure was equivalent) but that there may have been On the basis of Wald tests, we dropped the paths from the
systematic sample differences in measurement errors that were of Time 1 Phonological Sensitivity factor to the Time 2 Phonological
little substantive interest to the present study. Sensitivity factor, from the Time 1 Oral Language factor to the
Time 2 Environmental Print factor, and from the Time 1 Noverbal
Longitudinal Prediction Models IQ factor to the Time 2 CAP variable. On the basis of LM tests, we
added paths from the Time 2 Letter Knowledge factor to the
Structural equation modeling in EQS was used to examine the
Time 2 Phonological Sensitivity factor, the Time 2 Environmental
longitudinal relations between emergent literacy and either later
Print factor, and the Time 2 CAP variable. The resultant model for
emergent literacy skills (younger sample) or both later emergent
the younger sample is shown in Figure 1, S-B^2(305, N = 96) =
literacy skills and text decoding (older sample). The measurement
383.05, p < .01, RCFI = .93, RMSEA = .06. Time 2 Phonolog-
models identified in the previous analyses served as the basis for
ical Sensitivity was significantly predicted by Oral Language at
the longitudinal models. We first calculated the cross-time zero-
order correlations between latent constructs. Because our interest Time 1 and Letter Knowledge at Time 2 (R2 = .25). Letter
was in identifying significant sources of influence on children's Knowledge was significantly predicted by both Time 1 Phonolog-
development, we began by examining models that included au- ical Sensitivity and Time 1 Oral Language (R2 = .20). Environ-
toregressive paths (i.e., paths between the same factors at different mental Print was significantly predicted by Time 2 Letter Knowl-
time points). Inclusion of other paths was guided by results from edge only (R2 = .45). Finally, scores on the CAP measure at
analyses of zero-order correlations as well as theoretical consid- Time 2 were significantly predicted by both Time 1 Oral Language
erations. Modifications to these base models were made by exam- and Time 2 Letter Knowledge (R2 = .23).
ining the results of both (a) Lagrange multiplier (LM) tests, to Because the absence of significant cross-time stability in the
determine the value of adding parameters to the models that would Phonological Sensitivity factor suggested a problem with the mea-
significantly increase the model fit at the p < .05 level, and (b) surement of phonological sensitivity at Time 1 for the younger
Wald tests, to determine the statistical necessity of parameters sample, we examined a model with a Time 1 Phonological Sen-
whose elimination would not significantly decrease the model fit at sitivity factor that included only those tasks with significant cross-
the/? > .10 level. time stability (syllable blending and all three elision measures).
Younger sample. Zero-order correlations between the latent Evaluation of the measurement model supported three separate
variables at Time 1 and the latent variables at Time 2 for the factors to represent Phonological Sensitivity, Oral Language, and
younger sample of children are shown in the upper half of Table 6. Nonverbal Cognitive Abilities, S-B^2 (30, N = 96) = 28.83, p =
For the younger sample of children, the base longitudinal predic- .53, RCFI = 1.00. There was a significant cross-time correlation
tion model included paths from the Phonological Sensitivity factor between this reduced Phonological Sensitivity factor and the
at Time 1 to the Phonological Sensitivity and Letter Knowledge Time 2 Phonological Sensitivity factor (r = .35, p < .01). We
factors at Time 2. Paths from the Time 1 Oral Language factor to examined the full longitudinal model starting with the same base
EMERGENT LITERACY AND EARLY READING 605
Table 6
Zero-Order Correlations Between Time I Emergent Literacy Skills and Time 2 Emergent
Literacy and Reading Skills for Younger and Older Samples

Time 2 variables

Phonological Letter Environmental Concepts About


Time 1 variables sensitivity knowledge print Print

Younger sample
Phonological sensitivity .14 .33** .23 .14
Oral language .36*** .39*** .33** 37***
Nonverbal cognitive .16 .15 .19 .32*

Older sample
Reading
Phonological sensitivity 1.00*** .48*** .60*** 44***
Environmental print .59*** .42*** .51** .18
Letter knowledge .64*** .80*** .51*** .37**
Concepts About Print Test .60*** .35*** .40*** .62***

Note. Correlations are between latent variables for each construct, except for the Concepts About Print Test,
which is a measured variable.
* p < . 0 5 . **/><.01. ***/?<.001.

model described previously. The resultant final model is shown in Time 1 CAP measure and Time 1 Phonological Sensitivity (R2 =
Figure 2, S-B^(212, N = 96) = 265.74, p < .01, RCFI = .94, .44). Finally, Time 2 Phonological Sensitivity and Time 2 Letter
RMSEA = .06. In this modified model, the reduced Time 1 Knowledge were the only significant predictors of reading (R2 =
Phonological Sensitivity factor was significantly related to Oral .54). 56 As would be expected from the results of the Wald and LM
Language. Time 2 Phonological Sensitivity was significantly pre- tests, when paths between the Time 1 Environmental Print factor,
dicted by both Phonological Sensitivity and Oral Language at the Time 2 CAP measure, and the Reading factor were included in
Time 1 (R2 = .17). Letter Knowledge was significantly predicted this model, these paths were not significant, indicating that neither
by both Time 2 Phonological Sensitivity and Time 1 Oral Lan- the Environmental Print factor nor the CAP measure added unique
guage (R2 = .26). Environmental Print was significantly predicted variance to the Reading factor once the Phonological Sensitivity
by Time 2 Letter Knowledge only (R2 = .49). Finally, scores on and Letter Knowledge factors were in the model.
the CAP measure at Time 2 were significantly predicted by To confirm these findings and to ensure that our model devel-
both Time 1 Oral Language and Time 2 Phonological Sensitivity opment strategy was not biased against finding significant effects
(R2 = .20).
on reading for the environmental print and print concepts mea-
Older sample. Zero-order correlations between the latent vari- sures, we conducted model testing starting with a base model that
ables at Time 1 and the latent variables at Time 2 for the older included just the autoregressive paths and paths from both the
sample of children are shown in the lower half of Table 6. For the Time 1 Environmental Print factor and the Time 2 CAP variable to
older sample of children, the base longitudinal prediction model the Time 2 Reading factor. The resultant final model following
included paths from the Time 2 Phonological Sensitivity and Wald and LM tests was the model shown in Figure 3, with the
Letter Knowledge factors to the Reading factor, from the Time 1 exception that the path between Time 2 Letter Knowledge and
Phonological Sensitivity factor to the Time 2 Phonological Sensi-
tivity and the Time 2 Letter Knowledge factors, from the Time 1
Letter Knowledge factor to the Time 2 Letter Knowledge and 4
The identical final model was obtained when the base model included
Phonological Sensitivity factors, and from the Time 1 CAP mea- paths for all of the Time 1 variables with significant zero-order associations
sure to the Time 2 CAP measure.4 with Time 2 variables.
5
The paths between the Time 1 Phonological Sensitivity factor As would be expected given the high correlations between the Time 1
and the Time 2 Letter Knowledge factor and between the Time 1 and the Time 2 Phonological Sensitivity and Letter Knowledge factors,
Letter Knowledge factor and the Time 2 Phonological Sensitivity when the Time 1 Phonological Sensitivity and Letter Knowledge factors
were used to predict the Reading Factor at Time 2, they also accounted for
factor were dropped on the basis of Wald tests. On the basis of LM
54% of the variance in decoding.
tests, a path between the Time 1 Phonological Sensitivity factor 6
and the Time 2 CAP variable was added. The resultant model for We also tested the unique variance associated with each of the blending
and elision measures by adding (in separate sequential models) a path from
the older sample is shown in Figure 3, S-B^2(276, N = 91) =
each measure's residual to the Reading factor. In none of these models was
428.18, p < .001, RCFT = .87, RMSEA = .08. Time 2 Phono- there an improvement in model fit or increment in the R2 for the Reading
logical Sensitivity was perfectly predicted by Phonological Sensi- factor. These results suggest that, in these data, it was the variance common
tivity at Time 1 (R2 = 1.00). Time 2 Letter Knowledge was to the eight phonological sensitivity measures that was predictive of
predicted by Time 1 Letter Knowledge only (R2 = .72). Scores on decoding rather than the variance unique to the manipulation of phonemes,
the CAP measure at Time 2 were predicted by scores on the syllables, or words.
606 LONIGAN, BURGESS, AND ANTHONY

-.61
.48

Figure 1. Structural equation model of longitudinal relations between emergent literacy abilities for younger
sample of children. Circles represent latent variables, and rectangles represent observed variables. Variables on
the left of the figure are from the Time 1 (Tl) assessment (mean age = 41.1 months, SD = 9.4); variables on
the right of the figure represent Time 2 (T2; mean age = 57.6 months, SD = 10.1), reflecting development over
an 18-month period. All paths shown as solid lines are significant at p < .05. Wrd = word-level items, Syl =
syllable-level items, Phon = phoneme-level items, Ltr = letter; Env = environmental; Pics = pictures; CAP =
Concepts About Print Test; PPVT-R = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Tests—Revised; EOWPVT-R = Expres-
sive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised; ITPA-VE = Verbal Expression subtest of the Illinois Test
of Psycholinguistic Abilities; ITPA-GC = Grammatical Closure subtest of the ITPA; PicComplet and ObjAs-
sem = Picture Completion and Object Assembly subtests of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of
Intelligence—Revised.

Reading was not included. An additional model examined the dergarten and first grade can be found in the preschool period. A
influence of ITPA-GC scores on the Time 2 factors. In this model, number of the emergent literacy skills present during the preschool
ITPA-GC scores were not a significant predictor of any Time 2 period (i.e., phonological sensitivity, letter knowledge) reflect
factor and did not alter the significance of the paths shown in highly stable individual differences and have substantial unique
Figure 3. Finally, we also examined the independence of phono- predictive relations with later reading abilities. Together, phono-
logical sensitivity from oral language by regressing ITPA-GC logical sensitivity and letter knowledge accounted for 54% of the
scores from both reading measures. In this analysis, both the variance in kindergarten and first-grade children's decoding abil-
Phonological Sensitivity and Letter Knowledge factors continued ities. In contrast, other emergent literacy skills, such as environ-
to be significant and substantial predictors of the Reading factor mental print and print concepts, although present during the pre-
(R2 = .39). school period and relatively stable, do not appear to be uniquely
important for children's later reading. Taken together, these results
Discussion highlight the developmental continuity between emergent literacy
The results of this study demonstrate that the developmental and later reading from the early preschool period to the early
origins of a large component of children's reading skills in kin- elementary school period. Additionally, these results provide im-
EMERGENT LITERACY AND EARLY READING 607

Blend Syl
-.40

jElision W r d i * ^
-.16
-.39 " - ^Elision Syl
\ -.25'
Vision Phog

PicComplet« -,g

ObjAssem ..98.

CAP

2. Structural equation model of longitudinal relations between emergent literacy abilities for younger
sample of children including modified Phonological Sensitivity factor for Time 1 assessment. Time 1 measures
included in the modified Phonological Sensitivity factor were those with significant cross-time stability. Circles
represent latent variables, and rectangles represent observed variables. Variables on the left of the figure are from
Time 1 (Tl) assessment (mean age = 41.1 months, SD = 9.4); variables on the right of the figure represent
Time 2 (T2; mean age = 57.6 months, SD = 10.1), reflecting development over an 18-month period. All paths
shown as solid lines are significant at p < .06. Wrd = word-level items, Syl = syllable-level items, Phon =
phoneme-level items, Ltr = letter; Env = environmental; Pics = pictures; CAP = Concepts About Print Test;
PPVT-R = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Tests—Revised; EOWPVT-R = Expressive One-Word Picture
Vocabulary Test—Revised; ITPA-VE = Verbal Expression subtest of the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic
Abilities; ITPA-GC = Grammatical Closure subtest of the ITPA; PicComplet and ObjAssem = Picture
Completion and Object Assembly subtests of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence—
Revised.

portant information concerning issues of the development and the older children by Wagner and colleagues (Wagner et al., 1994,
measurement of several key emergent literacy skills. 1997). For example, Wagner et al. (1997) reported that year-to-
Perhaps the most striking finding from the present study con- year stability coefficients for their latent phonological sensitivity
cerned the high level of stability in children's phonological sensi- variable ranged from .83 (kindergarten to first grade) to .95 (sec-
tivity. The latent variable representing the phonological sensitivity ond grade to third grade and third grade to fourth grade). Our
of 5-year-old children attending preschool perfectly predicted the results indicate that this high degree of stability is present earlier in
latent variable representing phonological sensitivity of 6-year-old development and is not the result of formal reading instruction.
children attending kindergarten and first grade. These results in- In contrast to the extraordinary stability of phonological sensi-
dicate that there was no change in the ordering or spacing of tivity from late preschool to early elementary school, phonological
children's performance from preschool to kindergarten and first sensitivity was less stable from early preschool to late preschool.
grade despite the fact that there was significant growth in these In fact, very early phonological sensitivity, represented by all eight
skills (see Table 2). These findings are similar to those found with measures of the construct we administered, was not a strong or
608 LONIGAN, BURGESS, AND ANTHONY

Rhyme
26 \
^—^Alliteration L .57

Figure 3. Structural equation model of longitudinal relations between emergent literacy abilities and reading
for older sample of children. Circles represent latent variables, and rectangles represent observed variables.
Variables on the left of the figure are from Time 1 (Tl) assessment (mean age = 60.4 months, SD = 5.4);
variables on the right of the figure represent Time 2 (T2; mean age = 72.9 months, SD = 5.7), reflecting
development over a 13-month period. All paths shown as solid lines are significant atp < .05. Wrd = word-level
items, Syl = syllable-level items, Phon = phoneme-level items, Ltr = letter; Env = environmental; Pic =
pictures; CAP = Concepts About Print Test; ID = identification; Freq = frequent.

unique predictor of phonological sensitivity in the late preschool early to late preschool. Interestingly, the variables that defined this
period. There was some developmental continuity between this reduced factor were mainly those with weak relations to the factor
early phonological sensitivity construct and later phonological defined by all eight measures, indicating that the variance shared
sensitivity; however, this continuity appeared to be mediated by between these four variables, and hence the construct represented,
later letter knowledge, which was a significant concurrent predic- was distinct from that included in the original factor.
tor of phonological sensitivity. These results indicate that there Taken together, the results from these two models are similar to
were problems with the measures of phonological sensitivity for the results from other studies of young children that have found a
the early preschool group. That is, whatever variance was shared predictive relation between phonological sensitivity and later letter
across all eight measures in the early preschool period was not knowledge (Burgess & Lonigan, 1998; Wagner et al., 1994) and
phonological sensitivity. Based on the longitudinal empirical rela- between letter knowledge and both current and subsequent pho-
tions of this factor, it is possible that the shared variance repre- nological sensitivity (Bowey, 1994; Burgess & Lonigan, 1998;
sented letter knowledge or a proxy measure of print exposure. Johnston, Anderson, & Holligan, 1996; Stahl & Murray, 1994;
When we examined the longitudinal relations of a reduced Pho- Wagner et al., 1994, 1997). The mechanisms by which phonolog-
nological Sensitivity factor that included only Time 1 measures ical sensitivity influences the development of letter knowledge and
with significant cross-time stability, there was evidence for devel- letter knowledge influences the development of phonological sen-
opmental continuity of the Phonological Sensitivity factor from sitivity are not clear. It is possible that the development of these
EMERGENT LITERACY AND EARLY READING 609

skills simply indexes exposure to literacy-related activities. Alter- children across different ages provides strong support for this
natively, it is possible that children with greater sensitivity to the position. Even in the reduced factor for the younger children's
phonological structure of words and more letter knowledge may Time 1 assessment, Phonological Sensitivity was represented by
benefit more from the formal and informal exposure to print that sensitivity to words, syllables, and phonemes. Across analyses
many preschoolers receive (e.g., Lonigan, 1994; Whitehurst & from the late preschool and early grade school periods, only one
Lonigan, 1998). Perhaps the ability to discriminate word and index of phonological sensitivity did not have a significant asso-
syllable boundaries makes the significance of letters more trans- ciation with the phonological sensitivity construct. The word-
parent. Similarly, understanding the significance of letters may blending measure did not contribute to the latent variable at the
facilitate the segmentation of language. Time 2 assessment for the older group. This effect was likely due
In addition to the effects of letter knowledge on phonological to the fact that scores on the word-blending measure for the older
sensitivity, oral language had direct and indirect effects (depending children were at near ceiling levels. Regardless, this same analysis
on the model) on phonological sensitivity in the late preschool demonstrated that word-level and syllable-level blending were
period. This finding is consistent with results from a number of associated with manipulation of phonemes (i.e., alliteration, pho-
other studies of both preschool (e.g., Burgess & Lonigan, 1998; neme blending, phoneme elision), which supports the broadly
Chaney, 1992; Lonigan et al., 1998) and early elementary school defined phonological sensitivity construct.
children (e.g., Bowey, 1994; Wagner et al., 1993, 1997) that have Two additional aspects of our results support the importance of
demonstrated significant concurrent and longitudinal correlations the broader construct of phonological sensitivity. First, whereas
between children's vocabulary skills and their phonological pro- the measures of phonological sensitivity for the younger group's
cessing skills. These results suggest that oral language develop- Time 1 and Time 2 assessments and for the older group's Time 1
ment has an influence on the acquisition of this key emergent assessment were weighted heavily in favor of lower levels of
literacy skill. Past studies of preschool children have suggested linguistic complexity (i.e., words, syllables, onset-rime), the mea-
that productive phonology (i.e., speech intelligibility) is related to sures of phonological sensitivity for the older children's Time 2
performance on phonological sensitivity tasks (e.g., Webster & assessment were weighted heavily in favor of higher levels of
Plante, 1995). As discussed by Metsala and Walley (1998; see also linguistic complexity (i.e., phonemes). The fact that the earlier
Fowler, 1991), this evidence suggests that lexical representations Phonological Sensitivity factor perfectly predicted the later Pho-
become more segmental in early childhood as a result of vocabu- nological Sensitivity factor for the older group of children indi-
lary growth. The emergence of phonological sensitivity may be cates that sensitivity to lower and higher levels of linguistic com-
limited by these speech representations. plexity represents a continuum rather than distinct abilities. These
Despite direct and indirect effects of early oral language and findings are consistent with the results obtained by Stahl and
phonological sensitivity skills, all measured factors accounted for Murray (1994), who found that a single-factor solution explained
only 17% to 25% of the variance in phonological sensitivity a majority of the variance in kindergarten children's performance
measured in the late preschool period. Although these results on four tasks that varied by linguistic complexity.
indicate that children's phonological sensitivity in the late pre- Finally, the global construct of phonological sensitivity, defined
school period is partially a function of early phonological sensi- by variance common to sensitivity to words, syllables, onset-rime,
tivity, oral language skills, and letter knowledge, they highlight the and phonemes, was a significant and strong predictor of children's
fact that the origins of the majority of children's reading-related decoding skills. This finding demonstrates that this global phono-
phonological sensitivity are unknown. Like the results of our logical sensitivity, rather than just phonemic sensitivity, is influ-
earlier cross-sectional study (Lonigan et al., 1998), these findings ential in the development of children's decoding skills. Moreover,
indicate that significant growth in phonological sensitivity occurs like other studies (e.g., Bryant et al., 1990; Lonigan et al., 1998;
between 3 and 4 years of age. Consequently, efforts to identify the MacLean et al., 1987; Wagner et al., 1994, 1997), our analyses
origins of phonological sensitivity are likely to be most productive demonstrated that this relation was not the result of variance
during this period. Our results also suggest, however, that screen- shared between the global construct of phonological sensitivity
ing of children for phonological sensitivity deficits is unlikely to and oral language. That is, the predictive relation between the
be productive prior to the late preschool period, at least with the global construct of phonological sensitivity and reading is not the
present measures because of their limited predictive power for result of children with more developed oral language skills, such as
later phonological sensitivity. vocabulary, or general cognitive abilities simply having greater
The results of this study are also informative concerning the faculty with tasks assessing broad levels of phonological sensitiv-
nature of preschool phonological sensitivity. As noted previously, ity and also having better decoding skills. It is important to note,
phonemic sensitivity is often given special status in relation to however, that our assessment of oral language skills in the older
reading, with a number of authors arguing that phonemic sensitiv- sample was limited to a single measure. It is possible that other
ity is the critical influence on reading skills (e.g., Morais, 1991; oral language measures may have shared more predictive variance
Muter et al., 1997; Nation & Hulme, 1997; Tunmer & Rohl, 1991). with both decoding and phonological sensitivity. However, given
In contrast, we have argued elsewhere (Anthony & Lonigan, 2000; the independence of these constructs demonstrated in the younger
Anthony et al., 2000; Lonigan et al., 1998) that it is children's sample and the significant loading of the ITPA-GC on the broader
general sensitivity to the sound structure of language that is im- Oral Language factor, it seems unlikely that additional oral lan-
portant for learning to read an alphabetic system. Our finding that guage measures would have substantially weakened the strong
children's phonological sensitivity, broadly defined (i.e., sensitiv- relation between phonological sensitivity and decoding.
ity to words, syllables, onset-rime, and phonemes), was best char- Whereas a number of previous studies have interpreted findings
acterized as a unitary construct at each of the four assessments of that one measure of phonological sensitivity (e.g., phoneme seg-
610 LONIGAN, BURGESS, AND ANTHONY

mentation) predicts reading better than another (e.g., onset-rime administer the measure to the younger children at Time 1. Conse-
sensitivity) to indicate that one type of phonological sensitivity is quently, we were unable to represent it as a latent variable, and we
more important to reading than another (Goswami & Bryant, 1990, could not estimate its influence on the development of other
1992; Muter et al., 1997; Nation & Hulme, 1997), these analyses emergent literacy skills from the early to the late preschool period.
make the explicit or implicit assumption that there are different Although future studies should address these limitations, our find-
types of phonological sensitivity. Our results, as well as the results ings indicate that what is measured by print concepts that is
of other large studies (e.g., Wagner et al., 1993, 1994, 1997), independent of letter knowledge and phonological sensitivity is
demonstrate that such assumptions are incorrect, at least as expla- unrelated to early decoding abilities.
nations of the normal development of reading. That is, our anal- Our analyses revealed that both the measurement models and
yses of different tasks that varied in linguistic complexity, which the scores obtained by both groups of children during the late
indicated that a single-factor solution provided an excellent fit to preschool period were nearly identical. Consequently, these find-
the data, established that these tasks tap the same underlying ings provide a preliminary means of examining the developmental
ability, phonological sensitivity. Moreover, this single factor pre- continuity of emergent literacy and early reading skills from early
dicted a majority of the variance in later decoding skills. These preschool to early grade school. This cross-sample analysis high-
results are consistent with those of Wagner and colleagues and lights the significance of individual differences in both oral lan-
Stahl and Murray (1994) in demonstrating that phonological sen- guage and phonological sensitivity. That is, individual differences
sitivity is a unitary construct represented by sensitivity to onset- in oral language skills, such as vocabulary, appear to be an im-
rimes, syllables, and phonemes and in showing that the variance portant influence on later emergent literacy skills that are crucial
common to children's abilities to perform tasks requiring sensitiv- components for children's development of decoding skills (i.e.,
ity to onset-rime, syllables, and phonemes is a substantial predictor phonological sensitivity and letter knowledge). Individual differ-
of decoding skills. ences in phonological sensitivity measured at an early age also
Our results indicated that, like phonological sensitivity from late appear to have a significant influence on these key emergent
preschool to early grade school, letter knowledge was a very stable literacy skills that is independent of oral language abilities.
individual difference, and at every assessment, letter knowledge Despite these significant findings, a number of caveats concern-
represented an emergent literacy skill that was independent of ing this study are required. Although the samples used in this study
phonological sensitivity, environmental print, and decoding. Letter were larger than those used in most prior studies of preschool
knowledge in the late preschool period, indexed by knowledge of emergent literacy (e.g., Bryant et al., 1990; Chaney, 1992; Fox &
both letter names and letter sounds, predicted 72% of the variance Routh, 1975; Maclean et al., 1987), they were marginally adequate
in kindergarten and first-grade children's letter knowledge. More- for structural equation modeling. The broad age range in the
over, this level of stability was likely attenuated because of the younger sample of children may have obscured potentially impor-
near-ceiling performance of the older children on both the measure tant relations between some emergent literacy variables. For in-
of letter name knowledge and the measure of letter sound knowl- stance, it may be that greater stability in phonological sensitivity
edge at the Time 2 assessment (see Table 2). emerges at an earlier age but was not apparent because of the age
Another significant finding of our study was that measures of range of our younger sample. Additionally, our reliance on differ-
variables that have been the focus of traditional emergent literacy ent samples of children to explore the developmental continuity of
approaches (i.e., print concepts, environmental print) had no emergent literacy from early preschool to kindergarten and first
unique predictive relation to later reading skills or other later grade was not optimal. Although results of multisample analyses
emergent literacy skills. Some emergent literacy advocates have indicated that scores on the measures and the measurement models
argued that children's faculty with environmental print demon- were nearly identical across both groups during the late preschool
strates their ability to derive the meaning of text within context period, indicating that interpretations across samples were justi-
(e.g., Goodman, 1986); however, other research has not generally fied, conclusions derived from the same sample would be stronger.
supported a direct causal link between the ability to read environ- Importantly, it is unlikely that our main findings concerning the
mental print and later decoding skills (Gough, 1993; Masonhei- significant relations between emergent literacy skills within and
mer, Drum, & Ehri, 1984). Although these variables were associ- between assessment phases were the result of the age range of
ated with later reading and later emergent literacy when considered children within the samples because all analyses were conducted
in isolation (see Table 6), they were not significant unique predic- using scores from which the reliable variance associated with
tors in the context of letter knowledge and phonological sensitiv- children's chronological age was statistically removed. However,
ity. Concepts of print and environmental print might reflect very these results provide a preliminary examination of how these
early knowledge of literacy; however, our analyses demonstrated different emergent literacy skills relate to each other from the early
that measures of environmental print reflected a construct that was preschool period to kindergarten and first grade.
distinct from letter knowledge and phonological sensitivity. The
Not all domains of emergent literacy were measured in this
fact that both the environmental print variable and the CAP vari-
study (Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). For instance, some writers
able were predicted by phonological sensitivity and letter knowl-
have suggested that the constructs of emergent reading or emer-
edge suggests that they may best be conceptualized as proxy
gent writing reflect children's developing conceptualizations of
measures for these other emergent literacy skills, reflect more
literacy (e.g., Pappas & Brown, 1988; Purcell-Gates, 1988; Sulzby,
exposure to print and other literacy-related activities (e.g., see
1985, 1986, 1988). Although we believe that these skills are likely
Purcell-Gates, 1996), or both. Two limitations to the conclusions
to be related to concepts of print and understanding of narratives,
that can be made concerning print concepts in this study are that
and therefore either reflect dimensions similar to letter knowledge
we had only a single indicator of the construct and that we did not
and phonological sensitivity or relate more strongly to reading
EMERGENT LITERACY AND EARLY READING 611

comprehension rather than to decoding (Whitehurst & Lonigan, Anthony, J. L., & Lonigan, C. J. (2000). The nature of phonological
1998), future studies should address the relative independence and sensitivity: Converging evidence from four studies of preschool and
specific influences of these emergent literacy skills. In addition to early-grade school children. Manuscript submitted for publication.
phonological sensitivity, components of phonological processing, Anthony, J. L., Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., Driscoll Bacon, K., Phillips,
such as phonological memory and phonological naming, have been B. M., & Bloomfield, B. G. (2000). Structure of preschool phonological
identified in older children as significant correlates of reading sensitivity: Overlapping sensitivity to rhyme, words, syllables, and pho-
skills (e.g., Bowers & Wolfe, 1993; Wagner et al., 1994, 1997; nemes. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Baydar, N., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Furstenberg, F. F. (1993). Early warning
Wolfe, 1991). A complete account of emergent literacy will re-
signs of functional illiteracy: Predictors in childhood and adolescence.
quire an understanding of the development of these skills and their
Child Development, 64, 815-829.
significance, if any, during the preschool years.
Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS structural equations program manual. Encino,
Although the results of this study highlight the developmental CA: Multivariate Software.
continuities and discontinuities in emergent literacy and the sig- Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of
nificant linkage between emergent literacy skills and later decod- fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88,
ing, they do not address the question of the origins of these skills. 588-606.
Given the significant linkages found in this study, future studies Bentler, P. M., & Dudgeon, P. (1996). Covariance structure analysis:
should address questions concerning the developmental origins of Statistical practice, theory, and directions. Annual Review of Psychology,
key skills such as phonological sensitivity and letter knowledge. 47, 563-592.
Such information will expand our knowledge of emergent literacy Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the
and provide clues for the development of interventions designed to relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disor-
ders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
help children at risk for developing later reading difficulties.
atry and Allied Disciplines, 31, 1027-1050.
Finally, our results concern the development of emergent literacy
Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed,
and decoding in children from English-speaking and middle-class
precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading
families. Consequently, our results are most relevant to children & Writing, 5, 69-85.
learning to read an alphabetic language, and the degree to which Bowey, J. A. (1994). Phonological sensitivity in novice readers and non-
these findings translate to children who may be at risk for reading readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 134-159.
difficulties because of conditions associated with poverty or be- Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to
cause their native language is not English is unknown. read—A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419-421.
In summary, the results of this study have extended previous Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and
spelling. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
work on the development of emergent literacy skills and early
Brown, A. L., Palincsar, A. S., & Purcell, L. (1986). Poor readers: Teach,
reading in several ways. First, our results highlight the develop-
don't label. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The school achievement of minority
mental continuity between early preschool emergent literacy skills, children: New perspectives (pp. 105-143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
later preschool emergent literacy skills, and early reading abilities Bruck, M. (1998). Outcomes of adults with childhood histories of dyslexia.
of children. Second, these results clarify the nature of reading- In C. Hulme, R. Joshi, & J. Malatesha (Eds.), Reading and spelling:
related phonological sensitivity. Contrary to the dominant view Development and disorders (pp. 179-200). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
that it is phonemic sensitivity that is critical for decoding, our Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme
results clearly establish that it is children's global sensitivity to and alliteration, phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental
phonological features of language that relates to decoding. Third, Psychology, 26, 429-438.
this study partially explains the status of other emergent literacy Burgess, S, R., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Bidirectional relations of phono-
skills in explanatory accounts of the development of reading. Skills logical sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool
such as print concepts or the ability to "read" environmental print sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 117-141.
do not appear to have independent predictive associations with Butler, S. R., Marsh, H. W., Sheppard, M. J., & Sheppard, J. L. (1985).
later reading; rather, their predictive relations with later reading Seven-year longitudinal study of the early prediction of reading achieve-
appear to reflect the development of other emergent literacy skills ment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 349-361.
Byrne, B., & Fielding-Bamsley, R. F. (1991). Evaluation of a program to
such as letter knowledge and phonological sensitivity. Finally, the
teach phonemic awareness to young children. Journal of Educational
results of this study highlight the significance of the study of the
Psychology, 82, 805-812.
origins of preschool emergent literacy skills. The high level of
Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V., & Baldwin, L. (1990). The reading crisis: Why
stability of emergent literacy skills from late preschool to early poor children fall behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
grade school, coupled with the lower degree of stability of emer- Chaney, C. (1992). Language development, metalinguistic skills, and print
gent literacy skills from early preschool to late preschool, suggests awareness in 3-year-old children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 485-
that efforts to identify significant sources of variability between 514.
children in these skills should be directed toward the preschool Clay, M. M. (1979a). The early detection of reading difficulties (3rd ed.).
years. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Clay, M. M. (1979b). Reading: The patterning of complex behavior.
Auckland, New Zealand: Heinemann.
References Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition
and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Devel-
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about opmental Psychology, 33, 934-945.
print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-
Allington, R. L. (1984). Content, coverage, and contextual reading in Revised. Circle Pines, NM: American Guidance Service.
reading groups. Journal of Reading Behavior, 16, 85—96. Echols, L. D., West, R. F., Stanovich, K. E., & Zehr, K. S. (1996). Using
612 LONIGAN, BURGESS, AND ANTHONY

children's literacy activities to predict growth in verbal cognitive skills: ness. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes
A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, in literacy (pp. 5-27). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
296-304. Morrison, F. J., Smith, L., & Dow-Ehrensberger, M. (1995). Education and
Felton, R. H. (1998). The development of reading skills in poor readers: cognitive development: A natural experiment. Developmental Psychol-
Educational implications. In C. Hulme, R. Joshi, & J. Malatesha (Eds.), ogy, 31, 789-799.
Reading and spelling: Development and disorders (pp. 219-233). Mah- Muter, V., Hulme, C , Snowling, M., & Taylor, S. (1997). Segmentation,
wah, NJ: Erlbaum. not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. Journal of
Fowler, A. E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 370-398.
stage for phoneme awareness. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler Nation, K., & Hulme, C. (1997). Phonemic segmentation, not onset-rime
(Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 97-117). Hillsdale, NJ: segmentation, predicts early reading and spelling skills. Reading Re-
Erlbaum. search Quarterly, 32, 154-167.
Fox, B., & Routh, D. K. (1975). Analyzing spoken language into words, Oka, E., & Paris, S. (1986). Patterns of motivation and reading skills in
syllables, and phonemes: A developmental study. Journal of Psycholin- underachieving children. In S. Ceci (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive, social,
guistic Research, 4, 331-342. and neuropsychological aspects of learning disabilities (Vol. 2). Hills-
Gardner, M. F. (1990). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test- dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Revised. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy. Pappas, C. C , & Brown, E. (1988). The development of children's sense
Goodman, K. S. (1986). What's whole in whole language? Portsmouth, of the written story language register: An analysis of the texture of
NH: Heinemann. "pretend reading. " Linguistics & Education, I, 45-79.
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to Pikulski, J. J., & Tobin, A. W. (1989). Factors associated with long-term
read. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. reading achievement of early readers. In S. McCormick, J. Zutell, P.
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1992). Rhyme, analogy, and children's Scharer, & P. O'Keefe (Eds.), Cognitive and social perspectives for
reading. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading literacy research and instruction. Chicago: National Reading Confer-
acquisition (pp. 49-62). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ence.
Gough, P. B. (1993). The beginning of decoding. Reading and Writing: An Purcell-Gates, V. (1988). Lexical and syntactic knowledge of written
Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 181-192. narrative held by well-read-to kindergartners and second graders. Re-
Hoien, T., Lundberg, I., Stanovich, K. E., & Bjaalid, I. (1995). Components search in the Teaching of English, 22, 128-160.
of phonological awareness. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Purcell-Gates, V. (1996). Stories, coupons, and the TV Guide: Relation-
Journal, 7, 171-188. ships between home literacy experiences and emergent literacy knowl-
Johnston, R. S., Anderson, M , & Holligan, C. (1996). Knowledge of the edge. Reading Research Quarterly, 31, 406-428.
alphabet and explicit awareness of phonemes in prereaders: The nature Purcell-Gates, V., & Dahl, K. L. (1991). Low-SES children's success and
of the relationship. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, failure at early literacy learning in skills-based classrooms. Journal of
8, 217-234. Reading Behavior, 23, 1-34.
Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 Scarborough, H. (1989). Prediction of reading dysfunction from familial
children, from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psy- and individual differences. Journal of Educational Psychology. 81,
chology, 80, 437-447. 101-108.
Kirk, S. A., McCarthy, J. J., & Kirk, W. D. (1968). Illinois Test of Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., MacLean, R., & Mathews, R. (1984). Sources of
Psycholinguistic Abilities. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of Educational
Lentz, F. E. (1988). Effective reading interventions in the regular class- Psychology, 76, 1309-1324.
room. In J. L. Graden, J. E. Zins, & M. J. Curtis (Eds.), Alternating Snow, C. E., Barnes, W. S., Chandler, J., Hemphill, L., & Goodman, I. F.
educational delivery systems: Enhancing instructional options for all (1991). Unfulfilled expectations: Home and school influences on liter-
students (pp. 351-373). Washington, DC: National Association of acy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
School Psychologists. Stahl, S. A., & Murray, B. A. (1994). Defining phonological awareness and
Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86,
(1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431- 221-234.
461. Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F. W., & Carter, B. (1974). of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research
Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in young children. Journal Quarterly, 21, 360-407.
of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201-212. Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic
Lonigan, C. J. (1994). Reading to preschoolers exposed: Is the emperor and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-
really naked? Developmental Review, 14, 303-323. difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590-612.
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., Anthony, J. L., & Barker, T. A. (1998). Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of
Development of phonological sensitivity in two- to five-year-old chil- individual differences in early reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C.
dren. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 294-311. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 307-342). Hillsdale,
MacLean, M., Bryant, P., & Bradley, L. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes, NJ: Erlbaum.
and reading in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255-282. Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Cramer, B. B. (1984). Assessing
Masonheimer, P. E., Drum, P. A., & Ehri, L. C. (1984). Does environ- phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task compa-
mental print identification lead children into word reading? Journal of rability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 175-190.
Reading Behavior, 16, 257-27'1. Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of
Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the
segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phone- phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational
mic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L Metsala & L. C. Ehri Psychology, 86, 24-53.
(Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89-120). Mahwah, Stevenson, H. W., & Newman, R. S. (1986). Long-term prediction of
NJ: Erlbaum. achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. Child Develop-
Morais, J. (1991). Constraints on the development of phonological aware- ment, 57, 646-659.
EMERGENT LITERACY AND EARLY READING 613

Sulzby, E. (1985). Children's emergent reading of favorite storybooks: A processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psy-
developmental study. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 458-481. chological Bulletin, 101, 192-212.
Sulzby, E. (1986). Writing and reading: Signs of oral and written language Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte,
organization in the young child. In W. H. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.), C. A. (1993). Development of young readers' phonological processing
Emergent literacy: Reading and writing (pp. 50-87). Norwood, NJ: abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 83-103.
Ablex. Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). The develop-
Sulzby, E. (1988). A study of children's early reading development. In ment of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence
A. D. Pelligrini (Ed.), Psychological bases for early education (pp. of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study.
39-75). Chichester, England: Wiley. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73-87.
Sulzby, E., & Teale, W. (1991). Emergent literacy. In R. Barr, M. Kamil, Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker,
P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research T. A., Burgess, S. R., Donahue, J., & Garon, T. (1997). Changing
(Vol. 2, pp. 727-758). New York: Longman. relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level read-
Teale, W. H., & Sulzby, E. (Eds.). (1986). Emergent literacy: Writing and ing as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year
reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468-479.
Torgesen, J. K. (1999). Phonologically based reading disabilities: Toward Webster, P. E., & Plante, A. S. (1995). Productive phonology and phono-
a coherent theory of one kind of learning disability. In R. J. Sternberg & logical awareness in preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16,
L. Spear-Swerling (Eds.), Perspectives on learning disabilities (pp. 43-57.
231-262). Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Wechsler, D. (1989). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelli-
Tramontana, M. G., Hooper, S., & Selzer, S. C. (1988). Research on gence—Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
preschool prediction of later academic achievement: A review. Devel- Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emer-
opmental Review, 8, 89-146. gent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848-872.
Treiman, R. (1992). The role of intrasyllabic units in learning to read and Wolfe, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the
spell. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 123-141.
acquisition (pp. 107-143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests-Revised.
Tunmer, W. E., Herriman, M. L., & Nesdale, A. R. (1988). Metalinguistic Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
abilities and beginning reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 134-158.
Tunmer, W. E., & Rohl, M. (1991). Phonological awareness and reading
acquisition. In D. Sawyer & B. Fox (Eds.), Phonological awareness in
reading: The evolution of current perspectives (pp. 1-29). New York: Received December 22, 1998
Springer-Verlag. Revision received March 29, 2000
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological Accepted April 7, 2000

Call for Papers: Violent Children


Developmental Psychology will publish a special issue in 2002 on the topic of Violent
Children: Bridging Development, Prevention, and Policy. The problem of chronic vio-
lence in children has reached a new level of public awareness. Developmental science
has much to contribute to the understanding of this problem and how it can be ad-
dressed in the public domain. This special issue will highlight original empirical re-
search that contributes to this understanding.

Three kinds of articles will be considered: those that contribute to knowledge of how
chronic conduct problems or violent behaviors develop in children or adolescents,
those that evaluate rigorous experiments in the prevention or treatment of chronic
aggression, and those that evaluate public policies relevant to child conduct problems.
Together, these articles will provide a bridge between developmental science and pub-
lic policy.

Inquiries should be directed to the section coeditors, Gregory S. Pettit


(gpettit@auburn.edu) and Kenneth A. Dodge (kenneth.dodge@duke.edu). Manuscripts
should be standard length (fewer than 40 pages) and should be submitted for standard
peer review prior to October 1,2000, to the editorial office of Developmental Psychol-
ogy, University of Wisconsin, Waisman Center, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, WI
53705-2280. Please include the phrase "Special Issue on Violent Children" in the cover
letter that accompanies the submission.

You might also like