You are on page 1of 25

Forensic Psychology:

Criminal Profilers in the Courtroom


Adam Y. Zentner, J.D.
January 2011

This article summarizes the field of forensic psychology and the sub-specialty of criminal profiling in the United States and the United Kingdom, and discusses the admissibility of expert testimony by criminal profilers in American courts.

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

"There is all the difference in the world between evidence proving that the accused is a bad man and evidence proving that he is the [bad] man." -Lord Sumner1 I. Introduction In recent years, the field of forensic psychology has experienced massive growth in the United States and abroad. This continued growth is pushing academic institutions and the legal field to expand their usage of these practitioners. This article focuses on a specialized practice area within forensic psychology: criminal profiling (aka offender profiling). As forensic psychology burgeons with new graduates, the availability of criminal profiler testimony will likely also increase. This would naturally lead to increasingly frequent instances of judges encountering and evaluating whether such testimony may be admitted in evidence. This article will (1) summarize the history and growth in forensic psychology and criminal profiling; and (2) analyze whether the courtroom testimony of the criminal profiler should be admissible in evidence. This article will conclude that criminal profiles are not admissible, but the testimony of a criminal profiler might be admissible so long as it is carefully limited to reliable psychological theories. Distinctions between psychology and psychiatry are less important to the issues at hand, so both fields will henceforth be referred to without distinction. Although the field of forensic psychology requires reference on national and international levels, this article will attempt to narrow the legal inquiry to Kansas jurisprudence. Other pervasive legal standards will also be discussed. II. What is Forensic Psychology? Simply stated, forensic psychology is a field of psychology that operates within the legal system. Forensic psychologists provide professional psychological expertise to the judiciary, by working as clinical forensic examiners, correctional or forensic mental health consultants, researchers who testify about the reliability of a scientific theory or technique applied to a psycholegal issue, or trial consultants.2 This list is (1)

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

not exhaustive, but should provide some guidance. At the behest of judges, prosecutors, or defense attorneys during various stages of litigation, forensic psychologists are often asked to perform evaluations such as mental health diagnoses.3 Forensic psychology has matured into an increasingly widespread and accepted field of study. An article published by the American Psychological Association (APA) explains that there has been rapid growth over the past 30 years within the field of forensic psychology, and opportunities for postdoctoral fellowships, continuing education, and respecialization have become increasingly more available.4 For instance, in 19735 the University of Nebraska College of Law developed the first law-psychology joint-degree graduate program in the United States, wherein *s+tudents may obtain a J.D. and either a Ph.D. or an M.A. in Psychology.6 In 2001, the APA approved forensic psychology as an applied area of psychological specialization.7 Today, forensic psychology is the fastest growing speciality *sic+ within the discipline of psychology.8 This upsurge in forensic psychology is not isolated to the United States. The field thrives in the United Kingdom (U.K.), mainland Europe, and Australia.9 In a newly published text by the British Psychological Society, the authors noted: The past decade has been one of boom in forensic psychological practice with record numbers of posts in health and criminal justice . . . . [T]here has been a growth of staffing in the Higher Education sector to accommodate course growth. There has also been a concomitant expansion in the breadth and depth of academic work.10 Well-known mainstream writers have published articles in widely circulated periodicals about the intriguing uses of forensic psychologists within the legal community.11 Dr. John Horgan, forensic psychologist and professor in the Department of Applied Psychology at the University College Cork, Ireland, attributes this trend to mass media. Horgan explained:

(2)

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

Since the mid-1990s, forensic psychology has enjoyed an exponential rise in popularity . . . . [T]he primary driving force behind student and public interest has been the plethora of popular crime fiction since that period, especially in film and television. Hollywood blockbusters such as Silence of the Lambs, and more recently Red Dragon have successfully whetted the appetites of would-be investigators drawn to psychology with expectations of becoming the next Will Graham or Clarice Starling.12 The protagonists in many of these novels and films are based on real-world forensic psychologists who usually specialize as criminal profilers. For example, FBI profiler John Douglas, who pursued the infamous BTK Killer (Bind Torture Kill), was allegedly the model for Agent Jack Crawford in Thomas Harris novel The Silence of the Lambs.13 One of the better-known fictional characters is Sir Arthur Conan Doyles Sherlock Holmes.14 Outfitted with his iconic hat and curved pipe, Sherlock Holmes voiced a warning: It is a capital mistake to theorize before you have all the evidence. It biases the judgment.15 Unfortunately, clever criminals may not leave behind very much evidence to be judged. The battle of wits between the criminal profiler and his quarry has certainly allured the public interest. While public interest in forensic psychology may be thriving, the courts react more hesitantly toward this relatively new field. Forensic psychologists have fought a long and difficult battle to be accepted in U.K. courts, but eventually they succeeded.16 In 1991, the Court of Appeal allowed forensic psychologist testimony concerning personality traits.17 A decade later, forensic psychologists were heard by the House of Lords for the first time in the case of Donald Pendleton,18 whose conviction was overturned on the basis of fresh psychological evidence.19 III. What is Criminal (or Offender) Profiling? As a sub-specialty of forensic psychology, criminal profiling involves the use of behavioral data collected from crime scenes and witness accounts to predict likely characteristics of the perpetrator. Their goal is to narrow the scope of an investigators search.20 Profilers look for clues such as: geography of the (3)

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

crime, weapons, restraints (or lack thereof), domestic and work settings, time of day, sequence of injuries and events, the manner in which injuries or damage is inflicted, any information about the victims habits and personality, signs of struggle, and autopsy reports.21 Next, profilers draft psychosocial composites based on inferences about the perpetrators likely behavioral characteristics exhibited at one or more crime scenes.22 The underlying assumption is that a perpetrators behavior will be consistent from scene to scene.23 The more crimes a perpetrator commits, the more clues can be gathered to fashion a more accurate profile, thus increasing the likelihood of apprehending a serial criminal or repeat offender.24 For instance, one of these keystone successes began in November of 1940, when a series of bombs began appearing and sometimes detonating in Manhattan, New York.25 Although the perpetrator, later referred to as the Mad Bomber, left handwritten notes and other pieces of evidence at the scenes, he eluded police and continued his crime spree for the next sixteen years.26 The long-term failure of police to catch the bomber led to public outrage. Desperate, detectives approached psychiatrist and former Federal Bureau of Investigation (FBI) spy-hunter Dr. James Brussel.27 Dr. Brussel commented about his initial meeting with investigators who were clearly skeptical towards forensic psychologists. Dr. Brussel wrote, Id seen that look before . . . on the faces of hard, old-line, field-grade officers who were sure this newfangled psychiatry business was all nonsense.28 Nevertheless, Dr. Brussel agreed to help. After reviewing the evidence, Dr. Brussel gave detectives an eerily specific profile of the perpetrator. Dr. Brussel told police to search for a heavyset, unmarried, middle-aged male of Eastern European descent with an exemplary work record, living with a mother-like figure, and educated but used a stilted word choice.29 Then, Dr. Brussel did something that would make him a legend. He added, When you catch himand I have no doubt you willhell be wearing a double-breasted suit . . . . And it will be buttoned.30 About a month later, police identified a suspect named George Metesky, who matched the

(4)

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

profile in almost every respect. When police arrived to arrest Metesky at his home during the night, Metesky readily confessed to the crimes but asked for leave to change out of his pajamas before going to the station. The officers granted his request, and when Metesky emerged from his bedroom he was wearing a double-breasted suit . . . buttoned.31 Dr. Brussels and others successes eventually prompted the FBI to form the Behavioral Analysis Unit (BAU), which is probably one of the better known criminal profiling units in the world.32 BAU profilers have refined the principles created by Dr. James A. Brussel,33 whom many forensic psychologists credit as the father of criminal profiling.34 How a criminal profile is formulated from behavioral data depends on which approach the profiler utilizes.35 Some common approaches include: Diagnostic Evaluation; Crime Scene Analysis (known as Crime Action Profiling in the U.K.36); and Investigative Psychology.37 Howard Teten and the FBIs Behavior Analysis Unit (BAU) are credited with the development of one approach called Criminal Investigation Analysis,38 which gained popularity after it aided in the capture of infamous serial killers Ted Bundy and Gary Ridgway.39 This approach is often used in the United States to profile arsonists, sexual offenders, and sexual homicides.40 Techniques and theories unique to each of these approaches will not be discussed herein, but it will suffice this article that the prevailing attitude in the field is that these approaches are experimental, and frequently lack authoritative support41 and empirical validation.42 As the Program Manager of the FBIs Profiling and Consultation Program, John E. Douglas, explained, Investigators traditionally have learned profiling through brainstorming, intuition, and educated guesswork.43 Profiles based on intuition and guesswork will not withstand the prevailing evidentiary standards on expert testimony.

(5)

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

Profiling in England developed earlier than in America,44 but British profilers still havent seen the inside of a courtroom.45 Interestingly, while forensic psychology is increasingly welcomed into English courts, criminal profiles remain barred.46 One British commentator surmised that profiling has never been admissible in the British legal system as expert evidence, because of definitional problems and disagreements about the scientific knowledge base.47 Another possible reason for this view is that many profiles closely resemble cold reading techniques used by astrologers and psychics who make numerous vague and un-testable predictions, which no one can prove are either true or false.48 Some of the suppositions are retrospectively construed as accurate predictions, while inaccurate ones are quickly forgotten.49 Despite a well-documented history and the impressive recent developments within criminal profiling, forensic psychologists in general still view profiling as a largely experimental field.50 Dr. Horgan admonished: We ought to treat with caution declarations or claims to the validity and successes assumed of current incarnations of profiling. While its immense popularity may be welcome in drawing attention to the work of forensic psychologists more generally, failure to critically consider the practice, extent and accountability of offender profiling (and also the profilers) may have unfortunate repercussions for the broader development of forensic psychology.51 While the magic of real-world anecdotes may awe the public, many courts are more guarded, especially when asked whether these profiles should be admitted in evidence against a criminal defendant. IV. Admissibility of a Profilers Testimony in the Courtroom A. Frye and Daubert From 1923 until 1993, Frye v. United States defined the standard for admitting expert testimony based on scientific evidence.52 According to Frye: The rule is that the opinions of experts or skilled witnesses are admissible in evidence . . . [w]hen the question involved does not lie within the range of common experience or common knowledge, but requires special experience or special knowledge, then the opinions of witnesses skilled in that particular science, art, or trade to which the question relates are admissible in evidence.53 (6)

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

And: [W]hile courts will go a long way in admitting expert testimony deduced from a well-recognized scientific principle or discovery, the thing from which the deduction is made must be sufficiently established to have gained general acceptance in the particular field in which it belongs.54 The above text establishes some key elements in whether expert testimony may be admitted, namely: (1) is the testimony helpful to the trier of fact, and (2) is it based upon scientific principles that are generally accepted as reliable in the field.55 An experts testimony is usually considered helpful, when a trier of fact is asked to weigh evidence that they do not comprehend. The inverse is also true. As the Kansas Supreme Court explained, *w+here the normal experience and qualifications of jurors permit them to draw proper conclusions from given facts and circumstances, expert conclusions or opinions are not necessary.56 The helpfulness standard varies among jurisdictions. For example, an experts opinion in Tennessee must substantially assist a factfinder, otherwise the expert will be excluded.57 In addition to being helpful to a trier of fact, evidence must also be relevant to the issues or facts of the case. Evidence is relevant in Kansas when it [has] any tendency in reason to prove any material fact.58 Many other states have adopted a form of the Federal Rules of Evidence, which defines relevant evidence as evidence having any tendency to make the existence of any fact that is of consequence to the determination of the action more probable or less probable than it would be without the evidence.59 Relevancy is a matter of logic and experience, not a matter of law. Nevertheless, there must be some logical connection between the facts and the inference they are intended to establish.60 Concerning general acceptance within the field, courts have considered a variety of indicators to evaluate the overall maturity of the particular field and the reliability of techniques or theories used therein. Some common factors include: the availability of formal academic programs for the particular fields (7)

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

specialization;61 uniform practice standards and ethics rules;62 practitioner certification;63 peer-reviewed research;64 standardized terminology;65 and formal recognition by authoritative organizations within the field.66 The Frye Court acknowledged the difficulty in determining whether a practice has become generally accepted: Just when a scientific principle or discovery crosses the line between the experimental and demonstrable stages is difficult to define. Somewhere in this twilight zone the evidential force of the principle must be recognized.67 As forensic psychology displays most of the benchmarks of a mature, well-established field, there should be little question anymore as to whether the field is generally accepted by the psychological community.68 However, courts struggle when evaluating whether criminal profilers are basing their opinions on reliable, generally accepted theories and techniques. Some courts have erroneously qualified criminal profilers as experts, based upon the assumption that the experts testimony is premised upon generally accepted theories and techniques.69 The risk here is that some profilers may gravitate towards unverified theories which they presumptuously designate as indisputable fact, which the unwitting jurist may accept.70 Also, the area of criminal profiling suffers from a plethora of discordant terminology and acronyms, leading to ambiguity and dissonance among profilers.71 These issues are the principle barriers preventing criminal profilers from testifying in court. After Frye was decided in 1923, the generally accepted test dominated federal jurisprudence on this issue for the next 70 years, until the rule was superseded in 1993 by Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc.72 Daubert backed away from the restrictive generally accepted test and liberalized the use of expert testimony by focusing on reliability and results of empirical evidence for both scientific73 and technical74 evidence.75 Thus, general acceptance still plays an important role in the inquiry, but is no longer the dispositive factor.76 However, Kansas, among other states, refused to adopt the liberal Daubert

(8)

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

standard77 and instead continues following Frye.78 Any expert testimony based on unverified or experimental techniques must pass a Frye evaluation, otherwise it will be inadmissible in Kansas courts.79 1. Frye and Kansas Statute Annotated 60-456(b) Since Kansas courts still follow Frye, they are more restrictive with experts than courts that follow Daubert.80 The party offering the expert testimony has the burden of proving both general acceptance and reliability of the underlying scientific theory upon which the opinion is based.81 Any opinion based on a theory or technique that is still considered new or experimental and has not yet gained general acceptance, will be excluded.82 Even if the underlying principles are reliable, the experts testimony is excluded if the principles are not generally accepted in the field. Kansas courts apply Frye to both physical scientific evidence and psychiatric diagnosis.83 Kansas Statute Annotated (K.S.A.) 60-456(b) applies to expert testimony: (b) If the witness is testifying as an expert, testimony of the witness in the form of opinions or inferences is limited to such opinions as the judge finds are (1) based on facts or data perceived by or personally known or made known to the witness at the hearing and (2) within the scope of the special knowledge, skill, experience or training possessed by the witness.84 As a threshold matter, K.S.A. 60-456(b) is not difficult to hurdle. An expert in a Kansas court must personally obtain information about the case before testifying. Presumably, this would bar experts who are unfamiliar with the facts of the case. 2. Daubert and Federal Rule of Evidence 702 Daubert is the prevailing standard for experts in many state and federal courts.85 Justice Blackmun wrote for the Daubert majority, and held: (1) that the Federal Rules of Evidence as amended in 1976 supersede the Frye general acceptance test, thereby lowering legal barriers for expert testimony;86 and (2) that the trial judge has the task of ensuring that expert testimony is reliably founded, is relevant to the issues, and will be helpful to the fact-finder.87 In other words, an experts testimony does not have to be generally

(9)

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

accepted as reliable in the field, so long as the trial judge finds that the underlying theories and techniques are indeed reliable.88 In 1999, the Court decided Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, which clarified Daubert by entrusting trial judges as gatekeepers to exclude all forms of unreliable or irrelevant expert testimony.89 Soon thereafter, Federal Rule of Evidence 702 was amended in 2000 to incorporate the holdings of Daubert and Kumho.90 With those amendments, Rule 702 now states: If scientific, technical, or other specialized knowledge will assist the trier of fact to understand the evidence or to determine a fact in issue, a witness qualified as an expert by knowledge, skill, experience, training, or education, may testify thereto in the form of an opinion or otherwise, if (1) the testimony is based upon sufficient facts or data, (2) the testimony is the product of reliable principles and methods, and (3) the witness has applied the principles and methods reliably to the facts of the case.91 Daubert, or variations thereof, is probably the predominant standard in American courts.92 Daubert relaxed the rules of evidence to encourage courts to allow more experts to testify. Criminal profilers have better chances of testifying in Daubert jurisdictions, than in Frye jurisdictions. Trial courts asked to admit a criminal profilers testimony face a confusing proposition. Evidence of specific instances of someones conduct is inadmissible to show that the person committed the offense presently charged.93 One reason for this per se prohibition is to avoid scapegoat prosecutions, where someone is accused of committed the present offense because they committed a similar offense in the past. The obvious fallacy is that just because someone did something similar in the past, does not necessarily mean that the same person committed the present offense.94 As Lord Sumner famously remarked: "[T]here is all the difference in the world between evidence proving that the accused is a bad man and evidence proving that he is the [bad] man."95 A critical component of most criminal profiles are specific instances of the perpetrators conduct, which are used to show that the defendant is also guilty of the present offense. ( 10 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

To avoid this evidentiary barricade, some prosecutors have successfully offered a profilers expert opinion about characteristics from the crime scene and the defendants psychological evaluation, rather than the profile itself.96 When a party offers this kind of expert opinion as evidence, Frye or Daubert must be satisfied. Unwary trial courts may permit the profile, crime scene analysis, or the defendants psychological evaluation, when the proper evidentiary standard has not been satisfied. The following cases will examine both standards as they were applied in cases involving criminal profilers who testified as expert witnesses. B. Admissibility of Criminal Profiler Testimony Under Frye 1. Drake v. Portundo Drake v. Portuondo,97 illustrates how a trial court may be tricked into allowing expert testimony based on an unreliable theory that was not generally accepted within the field.98 In Drake, the defendant appeals his convictions of two counts of second degree murder for ambushing and killing a young couple and performing post-mortem sexual acts on the female.99 The motive of the murderer in Drake was likely related to sexual fantasy.100 New York courts follow Frye.101 During Drake's trial, the prosecution called Dr. Richard D. Walter, a criminal profiler who became a founding member of the Vidocq Society,102 a Pennsylvania-based criminal profiler consulting group. Walter testified concerning the defendants motive for the murders,103 based on a fictional sexual dysfunction syndrome he dubbed picquerism, which is, medically speaking, nonsense, but appeared to account for the particular, gruesome circumstances of the shooting.104 On appeal, the court concluded that Dr. Walters theory was not generally accepted in the field, nor was it reliable. Dr. Walter also improperly bolstered his credentials,105 presumably to enhance his own credibility. Dr. Walters opinion so clearly drew a connection between the defendant and the murder, that the jury was likely to be impressed (if not inflamed) by

( 11 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

testimony that the defendant was a picquerist who killed, mutilated, and abused his victims to satisfy a warped sexual urge.106 The Second Circuit Court of Appeals reversed and remanded for a new trial.107 2. Commonwealth of Pennsylvania v. Distefano In 2002, the Supreme Court of Pennsylvania affirmed a trial courts decision to admit the testimony of a profiler under the Frye test.108 The profiler had limited his opinion to a crime scene analysis based on theories that were generally accepted as reliable in the field, and he avoided any reference to the criminal profile or any outright assertions of the defendants guilt.109 This case demonstrates that profilers may testify, so long as they leave any inference of guilt for the jury to decide. C. Admissibility of Criminal Profiler Testimony Under Daubert 1. Masters v. People of the State of Colorado In 2001, Colorado abandoned Frye and opted for a combination of Daubert and the older version of Federal Rule of Evidence 702 (pre-1976 amendment).110 Thereupon, Colorado courts only needed to inquire as to the reasonable reliability of the experts scientific evidence, emphasizing the broad and liberal attitude toward those inquiries.111 In 2002, the Supreme Court of Colorado rendered an opinion in Masters v. People that dealt with expert testimony of a criminal profiler.112 In Masters, the defendant was convicted of murder based in part on the testimony of criminal profiler Dr. Reid Meloy.113 Physical indications at the scene led investigators to conclude that the victim had been standing at the street curb outside of the defendants home when she was ambushed from behind and stabbed with a serrated blade.114 The victims body was then dragged over 100 feet into a field near the defendants home, where the victim was stabbed, her face was scratched in a distinct fashion, and her genitals were mutilated.115

( 12 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

Investigators had copious circumstantial evidence linking the defendant to the crime. Inside the defendants home and in his high school locker, police recovered hundreds of writings and drawings that conveyed the defendants hatred of women, his fascination with brutally stabbing women and then dragging their bodies into remote places to be dismembered.116 He had two maps of the crime scene and a sizable cache of survival knives similar to those used to kill the victim.117 Many of the writings and drawings depicted victims killed from behind in a surprise attack with distinctive scratch marks on their faces.118 One particular drawing found in the defendant's backpack on the day after the murder aroused suspicion.119 It depicted someone dragging a body by the armpits with blood dripping from its back, leaving a bloody trail.120 This was precisely the method that investigators determined the victim had been moved from the street to the open field.121 When investigators inquired about the drawing, the defendant explained that he drew it to get something out of his system because it was bothering him.122 Lastly, when investigators asked the defendant if he had ever thought about committing a murder like this one, he responded in the affirmative.123 When investigators asked the defendant for advice concerning the investigation, the defendant suggested that investigators search a ditch near a particular bridge.124 A survival knife with a serrated blade was eventually recovered from the ditch near the bridge.125 Notwithstanding this circumstantial evidence, no direct evidence was found to link the defendant to the crime, so he was not immediately charged.126 Nearly a decade after the murder, police retained forensic psychologist Dr. Reid Meloy, who then reviewed the evidence and wrote a report which implicated the defendant as the perpetrator.127 The prosecution put Dr. Meloy on the witness stand to testify about the identity of the perpetrator, the motivation and premeditation of the defendant, the defendants planning of the crime, his opportunity to commit the crime, and his subsequent knowledge of the crime.128

( 13 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

The defense objected to Dr. Meloys testimony on the grounds that it was improper character evidence.129 The trial judge overruled the defendants objection, allowing Dr. Meloy to testify. Dr. Meloy testified about his training and experience investigating sexual homicides, and he explained some of the techniques he used to identify evidence of fantasies in relation to sexual homicide.130 Dr. Meloy explained that fantasies often trigger homicide and that the defendants writings and drawings tended to show that he had such fantasies.131 Dr. Meloy explained the relevance and definition of fantasy, pointing out the writings and drawings which supported his opinion.132 The trial court prohibited Dr. Meloy from testifying that the defendant fit the profile of a sexual murderer.133 The defendant was convicted of first degree murder.134 The court of appeals affirmed the trial courts decision.135 On appeal, the Supreme Court of Colorado held that the trial court did not abuse its discretion in admitting Dr. Meloys testimony.136 The Colorado Supreme Court limited its review to whether the scientific principles underlying Dr. Meloy's testimony were reasonably reliable.137 The court recognized that because social science attempts to understand highly complex behavior patterns, it is necessarily inexact. However this does not make it per se inadmissible.138 The court based its conclusion on the fact that Dr. Meloy had been qualified five times as an expert in sexual homicide, and that the prosecution established that the sexual homicide evidence elicited from Dr. Meloy was generally accepted within the forensic community.139 Dr. Meloy testified about the extensive body of specialized literature dealing with sexual homicide, and that researchers have been studying sexual homicide for over 100 years.140 He also noted that research had developed to include the systematic comparison of groups of sexual homicide perpetrators to control groups.141 Dr. Meloys opinion never made the inferential leap for the jurors as to the defendants guilt. Dr. Meloy opined that characteristics found at the crime scene indicated a sexual homicide motivated by fanta-

( 14 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

sy, which was a well-documented theory and generally accepted in his field.142 These expert crime scene analyses are also admissible in Frye jurisdictions as pure opinion based on experience, rather than science.143 It was also Dr. Meloys opinion that many of the defendants writings and drawings strongly indicated that he fantasized about committing sexual homicide. To again borrow Lord Sumners comparison: the defendants writings and drawings strongly suggested that he was a bad man, but Dr. Meloy never opined that the defendant was the bad man. The final step in the syllogism, connecting the murder to the defendant, was left within the province of the jury to decide. Dr. Meloy never told the jury that the defendant probably committed the murder.144 Had the trial court not limited the profilers testimony, this case would likely have been reversed. Dr. Meloy never implied that the defendant did anything improper in the past. An innocent person may have fantasies about committing sexual homicide, but that doesnt mean he or she is a murderer.145 Here, Dr. Meloy was asked to review evidence that the defendant had written and drawn. The images were disturbing, but without Dr. Meloys testimony a reasonable juror would likely not have appreciated the significance or the relevance of the material. Dr. Meloy analyzed the murder and the materials separately, but he left the inferential leap between the two for the jury to make. V. Conclusions The field of criminal profiling has made considerable progress towards becoming a legitimate subspecialty of forensic psychology. As research progresses, profilers will have increasingly reliable scientific foundations for their opinions. Many theories and techniques underlying criminal profiles are presently regarded as experimental and unreliable by most forensic psychologists. Thus, profilers must hurdle Frye or Daubert before their opinions may be admissible in court. As Frye is more restrictive, testimony admiss-

( 15 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

ible under Frye will almost certainly be admissible under Daubert. The Drake, Distefano, and Masters cases provide some guidance as to how profilers may successfully, and properly, overcome Frye and Daubert challenges. From Drake, in a Frye jurisdiction, the court properly rejected Dr. Walters testimony, because: (1) he based his opinion on experimental, fictitious, and altogether unreliable theories; and (2) his testimony sought to show the defendants guilt through the behavioral characteristics at the scene. Distefano showed that if: (1) a profilers testimony remains within the realms of reliable psychological theory; and (2) avoids making inferences of guilt, then the profilers testimony may be admissible under Frye. Masters, in a Daubert jurisdiction, properly admitted Dr. Meloys testimony, because: (1) he based his opinion on reliable theories of forensic psychology; and (2) he refrained from drawing conclusions of the defendants guilt or drawing a direct connection between the defendant and the crime. In these three cases, the fact that the expert is a criminal profiler is irrelevant. What is important are the scientific research and theoretical foundations. Assuming a criminal profiler in a Kansas court limits his or her opinion to crime scene analysis and defendant psychological evaluation or diagnosis, but avoids any attempt to directly connect the defendant to the crime scene based on behavioral similarities, then that profilers testimony will likely be admissible. Although profiler testimony is nearly nonexistent in Kansas case law today, the recent growth in forensic psychology suggests that Kansas courts, among others, will experience an influx of profiler opinion in the near future.
1

Thompson v. The King, [1918] A.C. 221, 236.

Terence W. Campbell & Demosthenes Lorandos, Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists, 2 CROSS EXAM. EXP. IN BEH. SCI. 12:2 (Am. Psychl Assn Comm. on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists, Mar. 9, 1991); see also, Brent E. Turvey & Wayne A. Petherick, Forensic Criminology 13 (Wayne A. Petherick ed., Elsevier Acad. Press 2010).

( 16 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

Campbell & Lorandos, supra, at 12:2. David DeMatteo, et al, Educ. & Training Models in Forensic Psych., 3 TRAINING & EDUC. IN PROFL PSYCH. 184 (Am. Psychl Assn 2009). Id. Univ. of Neb. Coll. of Law, Nov. 22, 2010, http://law.unl.edu/academics/degree_programs.shtml#lap.

Kirk Heilbrun & Stephanie Brooks, Forensic Psych. & Forensic Sci.: A Proposed Agenda for the Next Decade, 16 PSYCH. PUB. POL'Y & L. 219, 220 (2010).
8

John Horgan, Understanding Crim. Beh.: Beyond Red Dragon, UCC SCI. FACULTY PUB. LECTURE SERIES 2002-03, Feb. 11, 2002, http://understandingscience.ucc.ie/naturalworld/Understanding_Criminal_behaviour.pdf.
9

Id. Graham J. Towl & David A. Crighton eds., et al, Forensic Psych. 3 (British Psychl Socy & Blackwell Publg Ltd. 2010).

10

11

E.g. Malcolm Gladwell, Dangerous Minds: Crim. Profiling Made Easy, THE NEW YORKER, Nov. 12, 2007, http://www.newyorker.com/reporting/2007/11/12/071112fa_fact_gladwell.
12

Horgan, supra.

13

Thomas Harris, The Silence of the Lambs 1-5 (1988); The Silence of the Lambs (Orion Pictures 1991); see also, James A. George, Offender Profiling & Expert Testimony: Scientifically Valid or Glorified Results?, 61 VAND. L. REV. 221, 223 (2008); see also, Gladwell, supra, at 2.
14

See generally, Arthur C. Doyle, The Complete Sherlock Holmes (Barnes & Noble Inc. 1991). Arthur C. Doyle, A Study in Scarlet, in, THE COMPLETE SHERLOCK HOLMES 27 (Barnes & Noble Inc. 1991) (1887).

15

16

Gisli H. Gudjonsson, Psych. Brings Justice: The Sci. of Forensic Psych., 13 CRIM. BEH. & MENTAL HEALTH 159, 160 (2003) (Professor Gudjonsson wrote this article based upon his inaugural lecture following his appointment to a personal chair in Forensic Psychology at the Institute of Psychiatry, Kings College, London).
17

Id. Pendleton v. The King, [2001] UKHL 66 (discussed in Gudjonsson, supra, at 160). Gudjonsson, supra, at 160. Towl & Crighton, supra, at 148.

18

19

20

( 17 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

21

John E. Douglas, et al., Crim. Profiling from Crime Scene Analysis, 4 BEH. SCI. & THE LAW 401, 406 (John Wiley & Sons Inc. 1986). Towl & Crighton, supra, at 48. Id.

22

23

24

George B. Palermo, Homicidal Syndromes: A Clinical Psychiatric Perspective 1922, CRIM. PROFILING: INTL THEORY, RESEARCH, & PRAC. (Richard N. Kocsis ed., Humana Press Inc. 2007).
25

George, supra, at 222; see also, Gladwell, supra, at 1. Id. Id. Gladwell, supra, at 1. See George, supra, at 222; see also, Gladwell, supra, at 1. Id. Id. See George, supra, at 223; Horgan, supra, at footnote 2. George, supra, at 223. Towl & Crighton, supra, 14950. Damon A. Muller, Crim. Profiling: Real Sci. or Just Wishful Thinking?, HOMICIDE STUDIES Vol. 4 No. 3, 236 (Sage Publn Inc. 2000). Towl & Crighton, supra, at 144. Muller, supra, at 236. Towl & Crighton, supra, at 149. Id. at 14950. Id. at 149. See Muller, supra, at 234.

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

33

34

35

36

37

38

39

40

41

( 18 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

42

See Richard N. Kocsis, Crim. Psychl Profiling: Validities & Abilities, 47 INTL J. OF OFFENDER THERAPY & COMPARATIVE CRIMINOLOGY 126, 127-28 (Sage Publn Inc. 2003).
43

Douglas, supra, at 405; see also, John Douglas & Mark Olshaker, Journey Into Darkness 26 (Simon & Schuster Inc. 1997). Towl & Crighton, supra, at 150.

44

45

Caroline B. Meyer, Crim. Profiling as Expert Evid.? An Intl Case Law Perspective 235, CRIM. PROFILING: INTL THEORY, RESEARCH, & PRAC. (Richard N. Kocsis ed., Humana Press Inc. 2007) (Citing The King v. Stagg, Cent. Crim. Court, London, 14th Sept. 1994.); see also, Towl & Crighton, supra, at 150 (Writing that U.K. courts are increasingly resistant toward criminal profilers).
46

Id.

47

Nathan Gregory, Offender Profiling: A Review of the Literature, THE BRITISH J. OF FORENSIC PRAC., Vol. 7, No. 3, pp. 2934 (Pier Profl 2005).
48

Gladwell, supra, at 3; Towl & Crighton, supra, at 152. Id. Towl & Crighton, supra, at 9 and 157. Horgan, supra. Frye v. U.S., 293 F. 1013 (D.C. Cir. 1923). Id. at 1014. Id. Id. Falls v. Scott, 249 Kan. 54, 63, 815 P.2d 1104, 1112 (1991). State v. Stevens, 78 S.W.3d 817, 833 (Tenn. 2002). KAN. STAT. ANN. 60-401(b) (Current as of 2010). FED. R. EVID. 401 (Current as of 2010).

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

57

58

59

60

State v. Trammell, 278 Kan. 265, 282, 92 P.3d 1101 (2004); see also, State v. Woolverton, 35 Kan. App. 2d 478, 485, 131 P.3d 1253, 1259 (Kan. Ct. App. 2006) aff'd, 284 Kan. 59, 159 P.3d 985 (2007).

( 19 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

61

Heilbrun & Brooks, supra, at 219. Id. Id. Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., Inc., 509 U.S. 579, 594, 113 S. Ct. 2786, 2797 (1993). Heilbrun & Brooks, supra, at 219. Id. Frye v. U.S., 293 F. 1013, 1014 (D.C. Cir. 1923). See generally, Heilbrun & Brooks, supra, at 219. Terrence W. Campbell, Challenging Psychologists & Psychiatrists As Witnesses, MICH. B.J., Jan. 1994, at 68. Id. Towl & Crighton, supra, at 157. Daubert, supra, at 579. Id. Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, 526 U.S. 137, 119 S. Ct. 1167 (1999). See Heilbrun & Brooks, supra, at 220. Jennifer Sparks, Admissibility of Expert Psychl Evid. in the Fed. Courts, 27 ARIZ. ST. L.J. 1315, 1318 (1995).

62

63

64

65

66

67

68

69

70

71

72

73

74

75

76

77

State v. Heath, 264 Kan. 557, 957 P.2d 449 (1998); Armstrong v. City of Wichita, 21 Kan. App. 2d 750, 757, 907 P.2d 923, 929 (Kan. Ct. App. 1995) (stating at 757: We decline to apply Daubert.).
78

State v. Price, 30 Kan. App. 2d 569, 43 P.3d 870 (Kan. Ct. App. 2002) rev'd on other grounds, 275 Kan. 78, 61 P.3d 676 (2003); see also Smith v. Deppish, 248 Kan. 217, 807 P.2d 144 (1991).
79

State v. Witte, 251 Kan. 313, 836 P.2d 1110 (1992). State v. Canaan, 265 Kan. 835, 848, 964 P.2d 681, 691-92 (1998).

80

( 20 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

81

Id.; see Witte, supra; see also, Michael A. Barbara, Lawyers Guide to Kan. Evid. 162, KAN. LAW & PRAC. (2007). Id.

82

83

State v. Marks, 231 Kan. 645, 654, 647 P.2d 1292, 1299 (1982); see also, State v. Washington, 229 Kan. 47, 53, 622 P.2d 986 (1981).
84

KAN. STAT. ANN. 60-456(b) (Current as of 2010). Meyer, supra, at 210. Daubert, supra, at 579. Id. Meyer, supra, at 210. Kumho Tire Co., supra. FED. R. EVID. 702 (Advisory Comm. Notes on 2000 Amendments) (Current as of 2010). FED. R. EVID. 702 (Current as of 2010). Meyer, supra, at 210. KAN. STAT. ANN. 60-447 (Current as of 2010). State v. Price, 275 Kan. 78, 89-90, 61 P.3d 676, 684-85 (2003). Thompson, supra, at 236 (Emphasis in original). See George, supra, at 242. Drake v. Portuondo, 553 F.3d 230 (2d Cir. 2009). Id. Id. at 233. See id. at 236.

85

86

87

88

89

90

91

92

93

94

95

96

97

98

99

100

( 21 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

101

E.g. Nonnon v. City of New York, 32 A.D.3d 91, 101, 819 N.Y.S.2d 705, 712 (N.Y. App. Div. 2006) aff'd, 9 N.Y.3d 825, 874 N.E.2d 720 (2007).
102

Founders of the Vidocq Socy, Dec. 2, 2010, http://www.vidocq.org/who.html#Walter. Drake, supra, at 233. Id. (citing Drake v. Portuondo (Drake I ), 321 F.3d 338, 346 (2d Cir. 2003)). See Drake, supra, at 237-38. Id. at 245 (citing Drake v. Portuondo (Drake I ), 321 F.3d 338, 346 (2d Cir. 2003)). Drake, supra, at 24748.

103

104

105

106

107

108

Com. of Pa. v. DiStefano, No. 96-CR-737, Apr. 5, 1999, partly printed in PA. DISCOVERY & EVID. REP., Vol. 6, No. 12, Feb. 18, 2000, affd, Com. of Pa. v. DiStefano, 2001 PA Super 238, 782 A.2d 574 (Pa. Super. Ct. 2001); see also, Hank Grezlak, Profiling Testimony Inadmissible in Murder Trial, PA. LAW WEEKLY, Apr. 12, 1999, http://www.corpus-delicti.com/court_hazelwalter.html.
109

Id. People v. Shreck, 22 P.3d 68, 70 (Colo. 2001). Id. Masters v. People, 58 P.3d 979 (Colo. 2002) (en banc). Id. Id. Id. Id. Id. at 98384. Id. at 984. Id. Id.

110

111

112

113

114

115

116

117

118

119

120

( 22 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

121

Id. Id. Id. Id. at 985. Id. Id. Id. Id. Id. Id. Id. at 987. Id. Id. Id. at 988. Id. Id. Id.

122

123

124

125

126

127

128

129

130

131

132

133

134

135

136

137

138

Id. at 989 (citing Stevens v. People, 796 P.2d 946, 956 (Colo. 1990); see also, Christopher B. Mueller & Laird C. Kirkpatrick, EVID. 7.22, at 766 (2d ed. 1999)).
139

Masters, supra, at 989. Id. Id.

140

141

( 23 )

FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY

ADAM Y. ZENTNER

142

See Dion Gee & Aleksandra Belofastov, Profiling Sexual Fantasy: Fantasy in Sexual Offending & the Implications for Crim. Profiling 52, CRIM. PROFILING: INTL THEORY, RESEARCH, & PRAC. (Richard N. Kocsis ed., Humana Press Inc. 2007) (Concluding, that there appears to be a consensus among researchers that supports the presumptive role of fantasy as a causal mechanism in sexually aberrant behavior.).
143

E.g. State v. Price, 30 Kan. App. 2d 569, 579, 43 P.3d 870, 877-78 (Kan. Ct. App. 2002) rev'd on other grounds, 275 Kan. 78, 61 P.3d 676 (2003) (Explaining that Frye only applies when science is the basis of the experts testimony, rather than experience which is admissible as pure opinion.).
144

C.f. Sparks, surpa, at 1319 (In reference to expert testimony concerning the reliability of eyewitness accounts: Greater concerns about admissibility arise when the expert goes beyond discussing general psychological trends and renders a clinical opinion as to whether a particular witness behaved in accordance with the observed trend, and thus, renders an opinion on whether an alleged event actually occurred.).
145

See id.; see generally, Gee, supra, at 4971.

( 24 )

You might also like