You are on page 1of 8

Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2000) 48:36–43 © Springer-Verlag 2000

O R I G I N A L A RT I C L E

Andreas Gumbert

Color choices by bumble bees (Bombus terrestris):


innate preferences and generalization after learning

Received: 9 November 1999 / Received in revised form: 19 March 2000 / Accepted: 31 March 2000

Abstract It is usually assumed that the choice behavior Key words Color vision · Color learning ·
of bees for floral colors is influenced by innate prefer- Generalization · Innate preferences · Bumble bee
ences only for the first flower visits prior to any experi-
ence. After visits to rewarding flowers bees learn to as-
sociate their colors with a reward. This learning process Introduction
leads to an acquired preference for the trained colors that
has been believed to dominate over previous experiences A bee that forages in a diverse plant community meets a
and over innate preferences. This work investigates how broad variety of flowers, differing in their appearance as
bumble bees (Bombus terrestris) chose among artificial well as in quality and quantity of reward. How does a
flowers of different colors after they had been extensive- bee select flowers out of this diverse food supply? An in-
ly trained to other colors. The bees chose novel colors dividual bee does not visit different flower species at
according to their similarity to the trained color if the random but usually restricts its visits to a single or a
trained color was similar to some of the test colors. This small number of species within a foraging bout, a behav-
was true also if trained colors and test colors were well ior that is generally termed flower constancy (Heinrich
distinguished, so their color choice reflected generaliza- 1976; for reviews see Menzel 1985; Waser 1986; Chittka
tion between colors. If the test colors were so different et al. 1999). This foraging behavior can be interpreted to
from the trained color that no generalization took place, be the outcome of an evaluation process in which a bee
choice behavior was not affected by the trained color and constantly selects between potential food sources when
reflected innate preferences. The differences in choice approaching flowers along its foraging path. What are
frequencies could not be explained by physical proper- the rules that determine this evaluation process? Flowers
ties of the test colors other than the dominant wave- are detected and discriminated by bees according to spe-
length, a parameter taken to reflect hue perception. Pre- cific signals such as color, size, shape, patterns, odor,
ferred dominant wavelengths correspond to those ob- and other characteristics. Of these, color appears to be
served in naive bumble bees and honeybees. Thus bum- particularly important for flower recognition (von Frisch
ble bees show innate preferences for certain colors not 1914; Menzel and Backhaus 1991; Menzel and Shmida
only prior to color learning but also after intensive learn- 1993). Both honeybees and bumble bees learn colors
ing when choosing among very different novel colors. quickly when they are rewarded according to color
Color choice among similar colors, however, is con- choice, but bees also need to make decisions somehow
trolled by generalization from the learned color. before any learning or after only a little learning. How
do innate and learned preferences interact?
The assumption that bumble bees exhibit color prefer-
Communicated by: R.F.A. Moritz ences, particularly for blue colors, dates back to the last
century and was based on observations of visitation fre-
A. Gumbert (✉) quency to differently colored flowers under natural con-
Institut für Biologie-Neurobiologie, Freie Universität Berlin, ditions (Müller 1881). Honeybees that had gained forag-
Königin-Luise Str. 28–30, 14195 Berlin, Germany ing experience in the field showed high choice rates for
Present address: artificial flowers of blue and/or yellow colors (reviewed
A. Gumbert, Institut für Neurobiologie, in Menzel 1985). Such observations led to the concept of
Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf, Universitätsstr. 1,
40225 Düsseldorf, Germany, a flower syndrome for bee pollination, describing the
e-mail: andreas.gumbert@uni-duesseldorf.de, floral colors of melittophilous flowers as being mostly
Tel.: +49-0211-8113413, Fax: +49-0211-8111971 blue or yellow (Fægri and Van der Pijl 1980). Such a
37

flower syndrome would arise by bilateral co-evolution, influenced by innate preferences, and whether experi-
with flowers presenting signals that their specific polli- ence on rewarding flowers really erases these innate
nators would utilize in order to identify suitable species. preferences. Alternatively, might innate and acquired
However, neither the visitation rate to flowers in na- processes also be kept apart for the control of choice be-
ture nor the choice rate of experienced bees for colors havior under conditions in which acquired information
prove the existence of innate color preferences, because does not help, e.g., when the animal is exposed to flower
such behavior may be the result of previous experience colors very different from those learned?
with highly rewarding flowers in the field (Menzel To investigate this question, it is necessary to quantify
1985). Since the experience of insects previous to these the color choice behavior of bees whose previous color
observations was not controlled, little can be said about experience has been carefully controlled. To this end
the innate nature of these preferences. The learning pro- flower-naive bumble bees (Bombus terrestris) were first
cess involved during visits to rewarding flowers is ap- trained extensively on artificial colored flowers and sub-
parent in experiments, where bumble bees and other bees sequently tested with colors that were novel to them. The
can be trained to collect sugar water from colored artifi- following questions were addressed:
cial flowers (e.g., Menzel 1967; Heinrich et al. 1977).
1. Is the choice rate of new colors influenced by forag-
These experiments showed that bees learn to associate a
ing experience on different colors? In other words, do
color with a reward and that different colors are acquired
bees simply generalize from previously learned colors
differently. Violet (400–410 nm) was found to be learned
to novel ones?
fastest, bluish-green (490 nm) slowest, and the other
2. Do experienced foragers show innate preferences for
colors including UV in between. Despite these subtle
particular new colors that are independent of the
differences, all colors are learned very well and control
learning process?
choice behavior equally strongly after saturated learning
3. If innate preferences for novel colors appear, do they
(5–7 visits). Since this period of learning is short com-
correspond to those of naive animals?
pared to the extensive foraging activity of an experi-
enced bee (Heinrich 1975), the majority of flower visits
are likely to be after saturated learning. Another finding Methods
was that color learning is extremely flexible. Any new
color experience at the feeding site seems to override Apparatus
previous experience, so that solely the new association
governs the choice behavior. Bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) were purchased from STB Con-
Since the learning process is so effective it might be trol (Aarbergen, Germany) and kept in a flight cage situated in a
glass house without any contact with colored or scented nectar
expected that choice behavior is solely determined by the sources. The animals were fed with odorless sugar solution from a
reward experience of bees at flowers. In experiments transparent acrylic feeder, and pollen. The flight cage was con-
where the amount of nectar in artificial feeders was con- nected to an experimental arena by a tunnel that could be selec-
trolled, bees chose among them according to the propor- tively closed (Fig. 1). In the experimental arena colored stimuli
were presented as circular disks of 9 cm diameter with a black dot
tion of reward rather than by their color (Giurfa 1991; of 1 cm diameter in the center. Eight stimuli were attached to a ro-
Greggers and Mauelshagen 1997). Similar observations tating plate behind a cover. Four openings in the cover allowed the
have also been made in the field among flower varieties animal to see either a single colored disk and three disks with the
differing in nectar content (Pankiew 1967). same color as the background (training) or four differently colored
disks at a time (test). The stimuli subtended a visual angle of 16.7°
Whether floral choices are affected by some kind of when seen from the entrance so that the color information is per-
innate behavior as well as such associative learning is ceptible already when entering the arena. Honeybees perceive the
not entirely clear. The very first color choices of flower color of stimuli that subtend an angle of 15° or more (Giurfa et al.
naive honeybees and bumble bees appear to be governed 1995). Bumblebees, having larger eyes than the honeybee, have a
by innate preferences (Lunau 1990; Giurfa et al. 1995; higher spatial acuity (Land 1989), and are likely to have an equal
or even smaller angular threshold for color vision, so they should
Lunau and Maier 1995). Both species show two peaks of perceive the stimuli as colored when entering the arena.
wavelength-dependent color choice behavior at 400–
420 nm and at 510–520 nm. The spontaneous choice
rates correlate with the acquisition rate of experienced Training
honeybees, in that colors that are innately preferred are The experiment was divided into three phases: a pretraining phase,
also learned fastest (Giurfa et al. 1995). in which individually marked animals learned to fly into the ex-
Thus it is fair to conclude that innate preferences for perimental arena, a training phase, in which each individual was
colors in the first choices of the foraging career of bees trained to forage on a single color only presented on the round
plate, and a test phase in which four different unrewarding colors
may exist, but after a few visits to a rewarding flower were presented on the plate in the absence of the training color.
such initial preferences may be subordinated to acquired In the pretraining phase black cardboard disks with drops of
preferences and may even be deleted. Contrarily, innate 1.5 M sucrose solution were presented in the flight cage and suc-
preferences may persist even in experienced bees but cessively moved through the tunnel into the experimental arena.
The individuals followed the disks until they learned to fly into the
may not be visible under normal foraging conditions. It experimental arena.
may thus be asked whether the very few visits to flowers In the training phase the bumblebees were rewarded on a sin-
prior to learning are the only foraging decisions that are gle disk of one of the five training colors (Tr1–5) with 2 M su-
38
rewarded test stimuli. If the bumblebees had immediate access to
the experimental arena they would spend only a few seconds in
front of the four stimulus positions and would fly off towards the
ceiling without any attempt to choose other stimuli. The gate was
opened when the animals had already entered the tunnel and
reached the end of the tunnel close to the entrance to the training
arena. Thus, it was ensured that the stimuli subtended a minimal
visual angle of 16.7° when seen by the entering bees. For 2 min
after the bee had entered the arena all significant approaches to-
wards the test stimuli, touches by the front legs, and landings on
the stimuli were recorded as choices. In the test phase bees were
not rewarded and could not leave the experimental arena but flew
constantly in front of the test stimuli.
After the test phase the rewarded training stimulus was pre-
sented again while the bees were still in the arena, and another
training phase followed in the same way as described above, but
reduced to three bouts (instead of six in the first training phase) of
five visits each. In total, the test phase was repeated three times,
with two training phases in between. Thus, each individual bum-
ble bee was observed for 2 min three times, adding up to a total of
6 min.

Statistics
Fig. 1 Experimental setup. Bees were trained to fly from the flight
cage to the experimental arena. Bees entering through the gates As the absolute numbers of choices differed among the three test
were presented with a cover with four holes (side view above, phases and among individuals, relative choice rates of the four test
frontal view below) giving a view of four disks attached to a rotat- colors were calculated for each test phase and individual and arc-
ing disk (wheel) behind the cover. Depending on the position of sine transformed. A repeated-measures ANOVA was performed
the rotating disk the bee could either see a single training stimulus with test phase number and training color as independent vari-
and three background-colored disks (training situation) or four dif- ables. After showing that test phase number did not affect relative
ferent test stimuli (test situation) choice rates the effect of training color was tested with the number
of visits pooled for each individual. The LSD test for post hoc
analysis was used to compare the effects among different training
colors. The ANOVA provides the value of F, the df (first value in
crose solution. This disk had the same training color throughout parentheses), the error df (second value in parentheses) and the
the training procedure. Henceforth I will call one flight into the value of P.
experimental arena and the subsequent return to the hive a “forag-
ing bout”. For the first five foraging bouts the disk was presented Experimental colors
horizontally on the ground of the experimental arena and the ani-
mals were allowed to drink ad libitum at each visit. This was done Stimuli and background cover were cut from standardized color
to increase the foraging motivation by high rewards. Next, the papers of the HKS-N-series (Hostmann-Steinberg K+E Druckfar-
disk was presented vertically at one of the four possible positions ben, H. Schmincke & Co., Germany). In each experiment the
in a random succession, while at the other three visible positions same set of four test colors (Te1: HKS33n – human violet, Te2:
disks of the green background color were attached. In this vertical HKS45n – human dark blue, Te3: HKS 46n – human light blue,
position a small plastic cap was placed in the central black dot of Te4: HKS59n – human blue-green) was used. The training colors
the training stimulus to hold the drop of sucrose solution. For the (Tr1: HKS51n – human blue, Tr2: HKS54n – human dark green,
following five foraging bouts the bees were allowed to drink ad Tr3: HKS65n – human light green, Tr4: HKS6n – human orange,
libitum. The mere motor task of keeping a grip on the vertical disk Tr5: HKS10n – human red) differed in each case. The reflectance
and finding the reward in the center was difficult, and often the spectra of the test colors (Fig. 2a) as well as the training colors
animals flew up to the ceiling and sat down if they failed to find a (Fig. 2b) and background (HKS58n – human green) were mea-
reward within a few seconds. In this case they were gently caught sured between 300–700 nm with a spectral flash photometer
and released in front of the four disks again until they learned to (SR01, Gröbel UV-Elektronik, resolution 1 nm). In order to assess
find the reward. Later, when the animals were familiar with the color similarities among the stimuli the color-opponent-coding
situation, the reward was reduced so that an individual had to visit (COC) model of bee color vision was used (Backhaus 1991). This
the training stimulus five times before it was satiated. In order to model has been shown to predict the color choice behavior as re-
change the position of the rewarding stimulus between visits the lated to color similarity with a high accuracy in the honeybee
bee was chased away from the stimulus with a piece of cardboard (Brandt and Vorobyev 1997), and it is assumed to apply also for
of the background color so that it could not see the next position bumble bees, which are phylogenetically closely related to the
of the rewarding stimulus before removal of the cardboard. This honeybee and possess a similar set of spectral photoreceptor types
procedure was repeated for six bouts with five flower visits within (Fig. 2c, smoothed spectral sensitivity functions as revealed by
each bout. Thus, at the end of the training phase each bumblebee physiological measurements by Peitsch et al. 1992). The COC
had made a total of 40 visits to the stimulus of the training color. model represents perceptual differences between colors by the dis-
tance of their loci in a two-dimensional color space (according to a
city-block metric). The two dimensions of the color space corre-
Test phase spond to two types of color opponent coding neurons, A and B,
the excitation of which yields the locus of a color stimulus in the
In the test phase four unrewarded stimuli of the different test col- color space. The coordinates A, B of such a stimulus are obtained
ors (Te1–Te4) were presented simultaneously. Before the test as in Menzel and Backhaus (1991):
phase started, the tunnel to the experimental arena was kept closed
for 10 min after the bumblebee left the arena, in order to increase
the foraging motivation of a bee willing to return to the experi- qi qi
A = ∑ ai , B = ∑ bi (1)
mental setup and, therefore, the number of choices towards the un- i =u,b,g qi + 1 i =u,b,g qi + 1
39

Fig. 2. Reflectance spectra of a the training colors (Tr1–5) and b Fig. 3 Loci of all experimental colors in the color-opponent-cod-
the test colors (Te1–4), measured with a flash spectrometer. Re- ing diagram (Backhaus 1991). The diagram represents the excita-
flectance ranges between 0 (no reflection) and 1 (full reflection of tion values in an arbitrary scale of two color-opponent-coding
the illuminating light). c The spectral sensitivity of bumble bee channels, A and B, that correspond to color-opponent-coding neu-
photoreceptors. These data were taken from electrophysiological rons identified in the visual pathway of the bee. Channel A re-
measurements by Peitsch et al. (1992) ceives positive input from the long-wavelength (green) photore-
ceptor type and negative inputs from the short- (UV) and medium-
where ai={–9.86, 7.70, 2.16}; bi={–5.17, 20.25, –15.08}, i=UV, (blue) wavelength photoreceptor types. Channel B receives posi-
blue or green receptor, respectively, and qi is defined as the recep- tive input from the blue photoreceptor type and negative from the
tor-specific contrast, which is determined by the relative number UV and green photoreceptor types. A color distance of 1.4 units
of absorbed quanta q for each receptor i with respect to the back- measured in a city-block metric corresponds to 75% discriminabil-
ground; qi was calculated as: ity between two colors. The origin represents the green back-
ground. The bold line gives the spectral boundary loci of spectral
qi = es,i / eb,i (2) colors in 10-nm steps, and the mixtures of 300 nm and 550 nm
marked in 10% steps. Training colors (Tr1–Tr5) are marked with
where e is the excitation of the photoreceptor i produced by the circles, test colors (Te1–Te4) are marked with squares. Dominant
stimulus s or the background b. The excitation e of each type of wavelengths for all stimuli were obtained by projecting the color
photoreceptor i by a colored stimulus or the background is deter- loci onto the spectral boundary (thin lines) and the values (nm) are
mined as: displayed around the spectral boundary. No dominant wavelength
700 nm was calculated for Tr4 and Tr5, which are bee-purple colored,
ei = ∫ I (λ ) R(λ )Si (λ )dλ (3) which is a mixture of short- and long-wavelength light
300 nm
where I(λ) is the spectral intensity distribution of the illuminating
light, measured in the glass house over 2 days and averaged; R(λ)
is the spectral reflectance of the stimulus or the background, re- highest spectral purity (Fig. 3). The dominant wavelength is taken
spectively; and Si(λ) is the spectral sensitivity of the receptor with as an indication of the hue of any colored stimulus. The spectral
the index i. purity p of a color stimulus was calculated as the distance of the
The excitation of the long-wavelength receptor is of special color locus c from the achromatic center relative to the locus of
importance because this receptor, in addition to its contribution to the corresponding dominant wavelength d:
the color vision system, mediates the recognition of stimuli whose A + Bc
visual angle is too small to allow their detection as colored objects p= c (5)
(Giurfa et al. 1996). The brightness contrast B of the stimulus s Ad + Bd
against the background b was calculated as: Spectral purity might be an indicator of the perceptual phenome-
∑ es,i non of saturation, but no assumptions are made here about the per-
i =u,b,g ception of saturation or hue by bumble bees. The color distance cd
B= (4)
∑ eb,i of each test color from the background was calculated as the dis-
i =u,b,g tance from the respective color locus to the background:
The loci of all experimental colors in the in the COC color space cd = |Ac|+|Bc| (6)
are shown in Fig. 3. The dominant wavelength of each test color
was determined by projecting the color locus from the achromatic The four test stimuli differed strongly in brightness contrast with
center of the color space onto the spectral boundary (Wyszecki the background, as well as in long wavelength receptor contrast to
and Stiles 1982). The spectral boundary is the line connecting the the background and in dominant wavelength. Differences in color
color loci of monochromatic light, which is the colored light of contrast with the background were less pronounced (Table 1).
40
Table 1 Physical properties of the test stimuli

Test Dominant Spectral Brightness Green Color Color distance from training colors
color wavelength purity contrast with contrast with distance from
(nm) background background background Tr1 Tr2 Tr3 Tr4 Tr5

Te1 406 0.61 3.55 1.19 5.37 4.39 6.07 9.29 9.46 11.47
Te2 411 0.46 3.17 1.51 4.77 3.10 4.78 8.95 9.12 11.13
Te3 438 0.29 2.44 2.34 4.47 1.11 3.25 8.65 8.82 10.83
Te4 480 0.67 1.14 1.83 5.10 1.42 3.88 9.29 9.45 11.46

Results
During the test phase bumblebees flew in front of the
vertical disk and showed a mean of 24.35 (SE=1.46)
choices of the test stimuli. Fewer choices were made in
the course of the three subsequent test phases but the rel-
ative choice rate did not change throughout these tests
(F(2, 88)=2.042, P=0.136). For further analysis the data
for each color were therefore pooled among the three test
phases.
The relative choice rate of new colors was significant-
ly influenced by the color of the training stimulus (cu-
mulative effect of test color and training color: (F(12,
99)=14.498, P<0.0001; Fig. 4). After training to Tr1, the
four test colors were not chosen equally (F(3, 24)=27.179,
P<0.0001; Fig. 4). The loci of all experimental colors in

Fig. 5. a Relative choice rates for stimuli of different dominant


wavelengths pooled after training to Tr3, Tr4, and Tr5 (means±
SE). b Relative choice rates for stimuli of different dominant
wavelengths by naive bumble bees, redrawn from Lunau (1990,
his Fig. 6) (means±SD). c Relative choice rates for stimuli of
different dominant wavelengths by naive honeybees after training
on uncolored gray (bold line and squares) and aluminum (thin line
and circles) stimuli, redrawn from Giurfa et al. (1995, their
Fig. 3). d Relative choice rate of honeybees trained to monochro-
matic light stimuli by one learning trial, redrawn from Menzel
(1967, his Fig. 9). A choice rate of 0.5 means a random choice be-
tween the trained and the untrained alternative color (thin line).
For the sake of comparison, only those data points from the three
publications were chosen that corresponded to the wavelength
range of 400–500 nm tested in the experiment presented here, and
the respective maxima were normalized to 1

the color space are shown in Fig. 3, and color distances


between the training and test stimuli are listed in Table 1.
The test color most similar to the training color Tr1 was
Fig. 4 Relative choice rates for the test colors Te1–Te4 after train- chosen most frequently (Te3). Te4 ranked second, while
ing to training colors Tr1–Tr5 (means+SE). Tests of differences in there was no difference between Te1 and Te2. In other
relative choice rates were performed for each training color sepa- words, bees might have simply generalized the trained
rately using ANOVA for repeated measurements. Levels of signif- color to the most similar ones.
icance are indicated by asterisks (*P<0.05, **P<0.01, A similar pattern was obtained after training to Tr2.
***P<0.001, ns P>0.05). The number of individuals used in each
group is indicated by N, and the total number of choices is indicat- In this case the choice frequencies differed also signifi-
ed by n cantly (F(3, 20)=17.471, P<0.0001, Fig. 4), and test color
41

Te4 was preferred over the others. No significant differ- tween color loci. Therefore, discrimination values after
ences were found in the choice frequencies between the differential training can be compared with perceptual
test colors Te1, Te2, and Te3. Te3 was chosen less fre- color distances. Furthermore, it is important to note that
quently than Te4, although is has a slightly shorter color generalization between stimuli does not mean that the
distance to Tr2 (Table 1). stimuli could not be discriminated (Honig and Urcuioli
After training to color Tr3, however, the choice fre- 1978). Animals often judge stimuli as being similar to
quencies showed a different pattern: Te1 was chosen each other even if they are able to discriminate them
most frequently, followed by Te2, whereas Te3 and Te4 very well, thus they generalize between stimuli. However,
were chosen at the same rate (F(3, 24)=16.823, P<0.0001). generalization would be expected to be lower for stimuli
The ranking of the choice frequencies does not reflect that differ more than stimuli that differ less.
the similarity between training and test colors as indicat- In a preliminary discrimination experiment, in which
ed by color distances (Table 1). Choice frequencies after either Tr1 or Te4 was rewarded and the other was not,
training to Tr4 and Tr5 also differed significantly (Tr4: both colors were found to be discriminated to a level of
F(3, 40)=20.949, P<0.0001; Tr5: F(3, 24)=21.200, P<0.0001). 75% correct choices. This level lies between perfect dis-
The choice frequencies obtained after training to Tr3, crimination (100%) and random choice (50%) and can
Tr4, and Tr5 were not significantly different from each be used as a measure of discrimination threshold be-
other (F(6,66)=0.471, P=0.827). They could therefore be tween the colors. The discrimination threshold would
pooled and plotted as a function of the dominant wave- thus lie at 1.4 units in the COC diagram. All other color
length of the test color (Fig. 5a). This function can be pairs except Tr1-Te3 have a greater color distance and
compared to data from other authors who tested sponta- are expected to be well discriminated. It can therefore be
neous choice rates of naive animals to colored stimuli of concluded that a high choice frequency between a train-
different dominant wavelengths (Lunau 1990: bumble ing color and a test color with a smaller color distance
bees, Fig. 5b; Giurfa et al. 1995: honeybees, Fig. 5c) and should not be the result of imperfect discrimination but
data about the learning acquisition of experienced ani- rather that of a generalization process. The high choice
mals as measured by the number of choices to the re- rate for Te3 after training to Tr1, however, could be the
warding color after a single learning trial (Menzel 1967: result of both colors being below the discrimination
honeybees, Fig. 5d). All graphs show high choice rates threshold.
for stimuli of dominant wavelengths between 400 and Te4 was also chosen most frequently after training to
420 nm, but low choice rates between 430 and 490 nm. Tr2, which has a color distance beyond the discrimina-
tion threshold. However, from the color distance be-
tween Tr2-Te3 as compared to Tr2-Te4 it may be expect-
Discussion ed that Te3 would be chosen even more frequently than
Te4, which was not observed. In this case it was not pos-
Influence of the training color on the choice rates sible to exclude the possibility that generalization from
for test colors Tr2 to Te4 was stronger because of physical properties
other than the color distance. In terms of the dominant
Bumble bees chose differentially among the four test wavelength Tr2 is more similar to Te4 than to Te3
colors in all experiments. The training color had a signif- (Fig. 3). It is also possible that with increasing color dis-
icant effect on the choice frequencies of new, unknown tance among the colors considered, the metric of the col-
colors. If the training color was very similar to the test or space does not sufficiently correspond with perceptual
colors (Tr1), the choice frequencies were ranked accord- color distances.
ing to their color distance from the training color. Such
an influence of training to a particular stimulus on choice
decisions towards other, similar, stimuli is usually re- Innate preferences for test colors
ferred to as generalization (Pearce 1997). It is important
to note in this context that stimuli are defined by their If the color distance between the training and the test
perceptual quality and not by their physical properties. colors was increased further, the influence of color dis-
Thus, even if stimuli differ in physical properties such as tance on choice frequencies disappeared. However, even
their reflectance spectra, they may not differ in how they if there was no influence of a dissimilar training color on
are perceived, so that a high choice rate for a test color the choice frequencies of the four test colors, they were
may by the result of imperfect discrimination between not chosen equally frequently. After training to Tr3, Tr4,
training and test color. Likewise, stimuli differing only and Tr5, which had a greater color distance from the test
marginally in their physical properties may well be dis- colors, Te1 was always chosen more frequently than any
criminated. Colors have, therefore, to be expressed in a other, and Te2 ranked second. In these cases, generaliza-
way that captures in a quantitative way the perceptual tion cannot explain the pattern of choices observed.
value of colors. This requirement is met by calculating Moreover, the same pattern was obtained after training
color loci from spectral reflection functions using the with different colors. If the differential choice rate for
COC model of color vision (Backhaus 1991). This mod- the test colors under these conditions is independent of
el also provides an estimate of perceptual distances be- the learning process at the training color, and does not
42

reflect generalization, then it must reflect innate color Both findings were interpreted as indicating a differen-
choice behavior. tial preparedness to learn these colors. Naive bumble
Which properties of innate choice behavior may con- bees showed a higher choice rate for stimuli of a domi-
trol the choice rates observed for the test colors? In the nant wavelength between 400–410 nm than to those be-
experiment of Lunau (1990) naive bumble bees preferred tween 450 and 490 nm (Lunau 1990; Fig. 5b). Unfortu-
colors of higher spectral purity. However, differences in nately, Lunau did not analyze whether this difference
spectral purity cannot account for the different choice was significant. He tested for a linear correlation be-
rates in the experiments presented here. Had the animals tween wavelength and choice rate and did not find a sig-
used this physical property of the stimuli to guide their nificant result. However, a second maximum of choice
choices, then the predicted ranking of the test colors rate was present below 510 nm, which may have
would be Te4>Te1>Te2>Te3, which was not observed. obscured the existence of a correlation in his analysis.
Furthermore, differences in the long-wavelength receptor It can be concluded that both naive honeybees and
contrast to the background (predicted ranking: Te3> naive bumble bees show a particular preference for
Te4>Te2>Te1) and in color contrast to the background wavelengths between 400 and 420 nm, and that this pref-
(predicted ranking: Te1>Te2>Te4>Te3), or in brightness erence is maintained even after extensive training to
contrast (predicted ranking: Te1>Te2>Te3>Te4; with other colors. However, experience with colors can inter-
strong differences in brightness contrast between Te3 fere with this innate preference as long as the trained
and Te4) also cannot explain the observed ranking of color is similar to the variety of colors that the bee has to
color choice behavior. Long-wavelength receptor con- choose among. The conclusion that the pattern of prefer-
trast and brightness contrast did also not affect the ences that a naive animal evinces in its first foraging
choice behavior of naive animals (Lunau 1990; Giurfa et bout is maintained, and not erased by cumulative experi-
al. 1995). Long-wavelength receptor contrast was shown ence with other colors, is important. The pattern of pref-
to be important in the honeybee for the detection of erences remains inhibited, and can only be expressed
small objects, but is not used for the recognition of ob- again under particular circumstances.
jects subtending an angle of more than 15° (Giurfa et al. Taking the results presented together with those cited
1996, 1997). The experimental design chosen here ex- above it is obvious that it is not only the very first flower
cluded a choice for test stimuli from a long distance so choices in the life of a bee that are governed by innate
that long wavelength receptor contrast is expected to be color preferences. Bees rather appear to refer to these
of little effect here. Choice frequency was correlated preferences in situations in which their preferred flower
with color contrast against the background in the experi- types are not available and they have to switch to new
ment of Lunau (1990) and Lunau et al. (1996), but not in species. This foraging behavior has implications for the
that of Giurfa et al. (1995). Color contrast with the back- pollination of plant species that are visited mainly due to
ground and spectral purity are correlated to each other. their floral signals rather than due to their reward. How
Therefore it is not possible to relate any one of the physi- much floral signals influence the visitation rate to a plant
cal properties of the colored stimuli to the response rat- species should depend on the abundance of the plant and
ing in the experiments by Lunau and co-workers. the amount of reward it offers. Abundant and highly re-
warding plants can train their pollinators, which learn to
identify the plant by its floral signals. In contrast, rare
Correspondence of innate color preferences to those and dispersed plants have little opportunity to train their
of naive animals individual pollinators, but will rather receive a large
number of flower visits from individuals that have no
The data are in good agreement with observations of previous experience on these species. Their flower
Menzel (1967), Lunau (1990), and Giurfa et al. (1995) if choice behavior may be more influenced by their experi-
it is assumed that the choice rate is determined by color ences on other species or by innate preferences. A study
hue. The dominant wavelength of a color is taken as a of flower colors in plant communities revealed that, in-
physical correlate of hue, as is usual for human vision deed, rare plants depend to a higher degree on their floral
(Wyszecki and Stiles 1982). The bumble bees in the ex- signals than abundant plants (Gumbert et al. 1999). Rare
periment presented here preferred Te1 (406 nm) over plants showed a higher tendency to either diverge or
Te2 (411 nm), and these two over Te3 (438 nm) and Te4 converge in their color signals to other plants than com-
(480 nm). These findings correspond well with the re- mon plants.
sults obtained from experiments performed with naive Plants that do not provide a reward for their pollina-
honeybees (Giurfa et al. 1995): colors between 409 and tors are expected to depend particularly on their floral
414 nm were chosen more frequently than colors be- signals. A number of cases have been described, in
tween 434 and 483 nm (Fig. 5c). The acquisition rate of which plants are pollinated by bees that search for food
honeybees trained with monochromatic lights shows a but do not receive a reward when visiting the flowers
similar dependence on the wavelength (Menzel 1967, (Dafni 1984). Such cases have to be separated from
Fig. 5d). After a single training trial the relative choice those where the plant offers no nectar but may still be re-
rate for stimuli of 413 nm was higher than that for longer warding in terms of pollen. Similarly to rare plants, non-
wavelengths followed by 428 nm, 444 nm, and 494 nm. rewarding species are also unable to train their pollina-
43

tors and may follow a strategy of either mimicking other Gumbert A, Kunze K, Chittka L (1999) Floral colour diversity in
rewarding flowers or addressing innate color evaluation plant communities, bee colour space and a null model. Proc R
Soc Lond B 266:1711–1716
systems. In the latter case, they would also obtain a high- Heinrich B (1975) Energetics of pollination. Annu Rev Ecol Syst
er visitation rate if the pollinator has visited very differ- 6:139–170
ent rewarding flowers before. It would be interesting to Heinrich B (1976) The foraging specializations of individual bum-
investigate whether rare and non-rewarding flowers ad- blebees. Ecol Monogr 46:105–128
Heinrich B, Mudge PR, Deringis PG (1977) Laboratory analysis
dress innate color preferences more strongly when they of flower constancy in foraging bumblebees: Bombus tern-
flower in a habitat devoid of rewarding flowers with arius and B. terricola. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:247–265
similar colors. Honig WK, Urcuioli P (1978) Reizgeneralisierung und Reizkon-
trolle. In: Anonymous (ed) Die Psychologie des 20. Jahrhun-
Acknowledgements I want to thank the following people for derts. Kindler, Zürich, pp 250–305
numerous helpful comments and improvements of earlier Land MF (1989) Variations in the structure and design of com-
drafts: R. Brandt, M. Giurfa, U. Greggers, N. Hempel de Ibarra, P. pound eyes. In: Stavenga DG, Hardie RC (eds) Facets of
Kretzschmar, J. Kunze, and R. Menzel and two anonymous refer- vision. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 90–111
ees. This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemein- Lunau K (1990) Colour saturation triggers innate reactions to
schaft. The experiments conducted for this publication comply flower signals: flower dummy experiments with bumblebees.
with current German law. J Comp Physiol A 166:827–834
Lunau K, Maier EJ (1995) Innate colour preferences of flower vis-
itors. J Comp Physiol A 177:1–19
Lunau K, Wacht S, Chittka L (1996) Colour choices of naive bum-
References ble bees and their implications for colour perception. J Comp
Physiol A 178:477–489
Backhaus W (1991) Color opponent coding in the visual system of Menzel R (1967) Untersuchungen zum Erlernen von Spektralfar-
the honeybee. Vision Res 31:1381–1397 ben durch die Honigbiene (Apis mellifica). Z Vergl Physiol
Brandt R, Vorobyev M (1997) Metric analysis of threshold spec- 56:22–62
tral sensitivity in the honeybee. Vision Res 37:425–439 Menzel R (1985) Learning in honeybees in an ecological and be-
Chittka L, Thomson JD, Waser NM (1999) Flower constancy, in- havioral context. In: Hölldobler B, Lindauer M (eds) Experi-
sect psychology, and plant evolution. Naturwissenschaften 86: mental behavioral ecology. Fischer, Stuttgart, pp 55–74
361–377 Menzel R, Backhaus W (1991) Colour vision in insects. In:
Dafni A (1984) Mimicry and deception in pollination Annu Rev Gouras P (ed) Vision and visual dysfunktion. The perception
Ecol Syst 15:259–278 of colour. Macmillan, London, pp 262–288
Fægri K, Van der Pijl L (1980) The principles of pollination ecolo- Menzel R, Shmida A (1993) The ecology of flower colours and
gy, 3rd edn. Pergamon, Oxford the natural colour vision of insect pollinators: the israeli flora
Frisch K von (1914) Der Farbensinn und Formensinn der Biene. as a study case. Biol Rev 68:81–120
Zool Jb Physiol 37:1–238 Müller H (1881) Alpenblumen. Engelmann, Leipzig
Giurfa M (1991) Colour generalization and choice behaviour of Pankiew P (1967) Study of honeybee preference and behaviour on
the honeybee Apis mellifera ligustica. J Insect Physiol 37: normal and standardless alfaalfa flowers. J Apicult Res 6:
41–44 105–112
Giurfa M, Núñez J, Chittka L, Menzel R (1995) Colour preferenc- Pearce JM (1997) Animal learning and cognition. Psychology
es of flower-naive honeybees. J Comp Physiol A 177:247–259 Press, Hove
Giurfa M, Vorobyev M, Kevan P, Menzel R (1996) Detection of Peitsch D, Fietz A, Hertel H, Souza J de, Ventura DF, Menzel R
coloured stimuli by honeybees: minimum visual angles and re- (1992) The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects
ceptor specific contrasts. J Comp Physiol A 178:699–709 and their receptor-based colour vision. J Comp Physiol A
Giurfa M, Vorobyev M, Brandt R, Posner B, Menzel R (1997) 170:23–40
Discrimination of colored stimuli by honeybees: alternative Waser NM (1986) Flower constancy: definition, cause, and mea-
use of achromatic and chromatic signals. J Comp Physiol A surement. Am Nat 127:593–603
180:235–243 Wyszecki G, Stiles WS (1982) Color science: concepts and
Greggers U, Mauelshagen J (1997) Matching behavior of honey- methods, quantitative data and formulae. Wiley, New York
bees in a multiple-choice situation: the differential effect of
environmental stimuli on the choice process. Anim Learn
Behav 25:458–472

You might also like