You are on page 1of 6

Copyright 2008 ZERO TO THREE. All rights reserved.

For permission to reprint, go to www.zerotothree.org/reprints

Infants Take Self-Regulation


Into Their Own Hands
CLAIRE VALLOTTON
Harvard Graduate School of Education

Ten-month-old Celia watched her papa make a fire in the woodstove in their living room. Sitting Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky was one
several feet behind him, she watched him crinkle the paper and stack the logs into the stove. The long,
of the greatest contributors to our theories and
bright red lighter lay next to him on the floor. She crawled toward it. understanding of the relationships between
social interactions, language, and thought. He
From the time she was 6 months old, Celia’s mama and papa had been teaching her that the acknowledged that the basis of human behav-
woodstove in the living room was “hot.” They used the word repeatedly and paired the word with ior lies in the reflexes and impulses demon-
a gesture: extending one open palm toward, but not touching, the hot object. Celia had begun using strated by very young children in reaction to
this sign when she was 8 months old and used it to describe many different hot objects, including the events, desires, or feelings, and he proposed
lighter, showing that she understood the concept “hot.” that development involves learning to bring
our higher mental processes to bear on these
Papa watched out of the corner of his eye as Celia reached toward the lighter, then shook her reflexive reactions (Vygotsky, 1934/1986).
head and said “No,” and then retracted her hand without touching it. She repeated this series of Further, Vygotsky believed that symbols—
actions again and again: reaching toward the lighter, saying “No” and shaking her head, then most commonly, but not always, words—are
retracting her hand. Though very tempted by the bright, fascinating object, Celia successfully the mental tools we use for monitoring and
prohibited her own inclination by using symbols—both language and gesture—to tell herself not manipulating our own behavior.
to touch the hot object. In his studies, Vygotsky described the
development of self-regulatory self-talk
beginning around 3 years old (Wertsch, 1979).
n this story, recounted to me by Celia’s father, Celia inhibited an action she Through participation in regulatory inter-

I wanted to do but that she associated with a danger prohibited by her par-
ents, demonstrating her growing ability to regulate her own behavior

For older children and adults, language


serves as a mental tool set for self-regulation:
It allows us to reflect on, monitor, and modify
our own behavior. Recall your own self-
(Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1985). Can preverbal
children also use infant signs as a form of
self-talk for the purpose of self-regulation?
actions, children internalize the regulatory
speech of their caregivers. At first, a caregiver
in compliance with her parents’ expectations. Although she was just begin- (a parent, other adult, or more advanced peer)
ning to use her first words, Celia also demonstrated the relationship
between language and self-regulation. Further, her use of gestures points
to the possibility that both gestures and words may be used by young
children as mental tools for self-regulation.
Abstract
Language serves as a mental tool
set for self-regulation, allowing us
to reflect on and modify our own
behavior. Children as young as 3 years
talk out loud to themselves to regulate
regulatory response to frustration; it most their behavior, using self-regulatory
Language as a Psychological Tool
likely includes talking to yourself, possibly self-talk. Can preverbal children use
even out loud. Children as young as 3 years talk
Set for Self-Regulation infant signs as self-talk for the purpose
out loud to themselves to regulate their behav-
ior in challenging situations, using private
speech as self-regulatory self-talk (Winsler,
De León, & Wallace, 2003). By 4 ⁄ 1
2 years, chil-
L earning to inhibit a first impulse—
whether it is to reach toward a familiar
resting place for an object no longer
there, use aggressive behavior to get what we
want, or give up on a difficult task—allows us
of self-regulation? The author shares
observations from a child development
center using the Baby Signs® Program.
Infants and toddlers use signs to
dren are aware of their own use of private request comfort in regulatory inter-
speech as a coping tool (Manfra & Winsler, to solve complex problems, accomplish our actions with caregivers, and they use
2006). But the abilities to communicate and goals, and get along with others. Though self- signs when alone to modify their own
to mentally represent concepts—the two regulation involves a complex set of skills span- behavior in emotionally challenging
main functions of language—both begin prior ning all developmental domains, self-inhibition situations. Infant signs provide infants
to the onset of spoken language. Preverbal is a building block in the foundation of this with the cognitive tools to participate
children can even use infant signs to sym- critical ability. How does this ability to change actively in their own regulation.
bolically represent referents in their absence the course of our own behavior develop?

Se pte mbe r 2008 Z e ro to Three 2 9


part of the psychological tool set for self-
regulation for preverbal children. Two
studies have looked at the self-regulatory
function of conventional gestures in young
children. One study highlights the use of the
head shake as a self-prohibition gesture (Pea,
1980), and the other describes one child’s use
of pointing and showing gestures to focus her
own attention and complete a challenging
physical task (Rodriguez & Palacios, 2007).
Roy Pea (1980) documented toddlers’ use
of the head shake as a self-prohibitive “no”
gesture—similar to Celia’s head-shaking
in my opening story—in his studies on the
development of negation in early language.
Pea called this use of the head shake self-
prohibition negation, which he described as “a
form of egocentric symbol use in which the
Photo: Rebecca Vallotton

child approaches a previously forbidden


object or begins to do something which
has been prohibited in the past and then
expresses a negative” (p. 164). True to the
nature of young children’s adamant curiosity,
these self-prohibitions are not always suc-
Eight-month-old Celia signs “Hot” from a safe distance. cessful in helping the child avoid the prohib-
ited object (Pea, 1980). In self-prohibition,
speaks to the child to keep him focused on caregiver directed toward him, and he has the child is acting out two roles, both her
the task at hand. If he gets distracted, the internalized this speech through a process of own role as action-initiator and the role
caregiver draws his attention back to the task increasingly active participation in regulatory that is usually played by her caregiver,
(e.g., “Let’s finish this puzzle first before interactions. the action-constrainer (Pea, 1980). Pea
you play”). If he gets frustrated, the care- reflected that “The awareness of this contrast
giver provides reassurance (e.g., “Yes, this is a Gestures as Early Symbols Used [between roles] is most striking when the
hard one, but you can do it if you try again”). for Self-Regulation child actually stops the action as if the parent

T
When he gets stuck and doesn’t know how here are two broad functions of had been the one to say ‘no’ rather than the
to proceed, the caregiver coaches him language: communication and rep- self” (p. 182).
(e.g., “The piece didn’t fit there; where will resentation. However, both commu- Looking at self-focusing rather than self-
you try it next?”). In a sense, the caregiver nication and representation begin to develop prohibition, Rodriguez and Palacios (2007)
is acting as an external executive function, prior to the onset of spoken language. Infants examined a single child’s use of gestures to
monitoring and manipulating the child’s begin intentional communication as early help herself solve a problem to complete a
inhibition, attention, and strategies. Then as 6 months of age (Wagner, 2006), and by particularly challenging task. They observed
the child begins to participate in and take 10 months most typically developing infants Nerea and her parents in a stacking rings puz-
over this role for himself. He begins by have a repertoire of communicative behav- zle task when Nerea was 12, 15, and 18 months
talking out loud to himself, using words and iors, including vocalizations and gestures such old. At 18 months, Nerea used private ges-
phrases parallel to the ones his caregiver used as pointing and showing (Crais, Douglas, & tures (gestures not directed to either parent)
(e.g., “Puzzle first, no play,” “Try again,” “Not Campbell, 2004; Wagner, 2006). By 12 months to think externally about the problem at hand.
there; where next?”). More and more he they are even intentional about using gestures In trying to place a ring around a post, Nerea
serves as his own regulator, though his speech to influence others’ mental states (Tomasello, repeatedly showed the ring to herself, turn-
is still out loud, still external. He says to him- Carpenter, & Liszkowski, 2007). ing it over and over, looking for the right posi-
self the same words his caregiver used, hears Most scientists studying infant gesture tion. Further, without looking to the adults in
those words, and responds to them. As his look primarily at its communicative function. the room, she also pointed repeatedly at the
cognitive skills become more advanced, the Yet according to Susan Goldin-Meadow top of the post where she knew she wanted to
young puzzle-solver will no longer say the (2005) and her colleagues, gestures used place the ring. Her mother had done this same
regulatory phrases out loud, but will mouth along with speech often represent unspoken pointing behavior minutes earlier to guide
them quietly to himself as he goes about his content of thought. Further, in a series of Nerea’s actions. Eventually she solved the
task, often independently of the caregiver. studies on gesture use by preverbal children puzzle, placing the ring around the post for
In the final stage of this process, the child and their parents, Linda Acredolo and Susan the first time without physical help from an
maintains focus on the problem without Goodwyn (1985, 1988) showed that preverbal adult. When Nerea tried to solve the problem
external speech. The caregiver’s regulatory infants are capable of using truly symbolic herself, she regulated her own behavior by
speech has been internalized as the child’s gestures; that is, the infants’ gestures repre- using the same gestures her parents had used
conscious dialogue of executive function, sent their referents in the absence of the to help her with the task.
the psychological tool set the child will use referents (Werner & Kaplan, 1963). Both the self-prohibitive “no” gesture
to manipulate his own thoughts, feelings, Because gestures can be used as both com- and the use of the self-reflexive pointing and
and behavior. Thus the child’s self-regulatory munication and representation by infants, showing gestures for self-focusing reveal
self-talk is a reflection of the speech his we may wonder whether gestures could be that several of Vygotsky’s hypotheses about

3 0 Z e ro to Three Se pte mbe r 2008


self-regulatory self-talk in older children are detail. In the stories that follow, I distinguish
also true of self-regulatory self-gesture in between these two data collection methods
younger children. They reveal that even very by noting in parentheses at the end of each
young children can “think out loud”—in this story (V) for video or (A) for anecdotal note.
case using gesture—to direct their own In each story, names have been changed, but
behaviors prior to the internalization of age and gender remain accurate.
executive thought processes. Further, they
show that self-regulatory gestures emerge Caregivers Use Signs to Help Regulate
in the same way that self-regulatory speech Infant Behavior and Emotion
does: by internalizing the way caregivers used The first step to internalizing self-regulatory
the same gestures during previous regulatory speech is hearing and responding to a care-
interactions. giver’s regulatory speech. In the CCFS class-
The studies by Pea (1980) and by Rodriguez rooms, caregivers use both words and infant

Photo courtesy of BabySigns, Inc.


and Palacios (2007) examined young children’s signs when they talk with infants about
use of conventional gestures—shaking head, their own and infants’ behaviors, believing
pointing, showing—used by most infants, as that infant signs may be easier for infants to
well as by adults. But babies who use infant understand because they are more concretely
signs are capable of using gestures to symboli- linked to the concepts they represent.
cally represent and communicate a wide range
of their thoughts and feelings (Acredolo & MELISSA RESPONDS TO A REQUEST TO
Goodwyn, 1985, 1988, 1992; Vallotton, 2008). BE GENTLE
Could infants also use infant signs as an early Melissa (11 months) was sitting in my lap near The “Gentle” sign.
form of self-regulatory self-talk? I sought to the gate of the snack room, waiting while her
answer the following questions on the self- caregiver set up her snack. Ruby (another
regulatory function of infant signs: infant) was standing in front of Melissa, In this story Tony attended to his caregiver’s
holding onto the gate and watching her own words and signs as she tried to comfort him.
• Does caregivers’ use of infant signs help caregiver. Melissa was looking at the back of He seemed to be comforted by knowing his
children regulate their emotions or Ruby’s head, which was about an arm’s length mother would return later.
behavior? away. Melissa reached out her hand to touch
• Do children use signs to participate Ruby’s hair. I said, “You are looking at Ruby’s Using Signs to Request Comfort
actively with their caregivers in regulat- hair and are wanting to touch it. We need to From Caregivers
ing their emotions and behavior? be gentle.” I stroked my arm gently to sign In the CCFS classrooms, children and
• Can preverbal children use infant signs “gentle.” She touched Ruby’s hair with one caregivers co-establish comforting routines,
in service of self-regulation to change index finger, very lightly. I said, “Yes, thank you including songs, games, looking at the fish
the course of their own behavior? for being gentle.” (A) tank, reading a favorite book, or looking out
the window. Caregivers use signs in these
The Children and Their Signs Here a caregiver used a sign as part of a regu- routines, then children can initiate or modify

T he children I describe below were


enrolled in the Infant and Toddler
Program at the University of Califor-
nia, Davis Center for Child and Family Studies
(CCFS). The head teachers and caregivers
latory interaction. After her caregiver talked
and signed about being gentle, Melissa mod-
ified her behavior from a full hand touch to a
light, one-finger touch.
the routines when they are upset and need
comfort.

ANDREW LEARNS TO INITIATE A


COMFORTING SONG
systematically used infant signs in everyday TONY IS COMFORTED BY THE IDEA THAT Background: Paper stars hang above the
interactions with the children in what was MOM WILL BE BACK LATER diapering table. Caregivers often blow on
the original application of the Baby Signs® Tony (13 ⁄ 1
2 months) sat next to me as his mom the stars to make them move while they are
Program, modeled after the work of Acredolo walked out the classroom door. When she was changing a baby’s diapers. They often sing
and Goodwyn (1985, 1988). Caregivers used gone, he turned and looked at me with wide songs, including “Twinkle, Twinkle, Little
70 different signs between them, and children eyes, stood up, and ran toward the door. He Star,” to comfort or amuse the children while
learned a subset of these. The infants and pressed his hands against the door, opening and changing them.
toddlers learned the signs as they do language, closing his fists as he watched out the window
picking them up in interactions with adults. in the door. I crawled over and sat beside him. November 11. I put Andrew (11 months)
They were never explicitly taught or forced to He whimpered a little bit, with the same wide- down on the diaper table, and I said, “While
use signs. eyed look. I said, “I know, I saw your mommy I am changing your diaper, we can sing some
The signing stories that I describe below leave. It’s hard sometimes when Mommy leaves, songs.” While I was unbuttoning his pants,
were observed in two ways. Some were cap- huh? She’ll be back later,” and I showed him Andrew stared up at the stars for a few seconds.
tured on videotape, collected, and transcribed the gesture for “later.” He stared into my eyes I said, “It looks like you are looking at the
for the purposes of research (for complete the entire time I talked, except when I did the stars. . . . Twinkle, twinkle, little star. . . .”
information on methodology see Vallotton, gesture, at which point he looked down at my While I sang, I signed “star.” The second time
2008). Others were reported as part of stu- hands. When I finished talking, Tony smiled around, Andrew signed “star,” bringing his
dent caregivers’ weekly assignments to write at me, turned away from the door, and ran hand up in the air and slowly opening and
what the CCFS called “anecdotal notes.” As into the classroom toward the toy shelves. He closing it. After the end of the song, Andrew
part of their training, caregivers were taught stopped after a few steps and looked back at me, clapped. (A)
to observe and record (in an electronic data- and I began crawling behind him to where he November 18. I laid Andrew down on his
base) child behavior objectively and in careful was going. (A) back at the diapering table, and he immediately

Se pte mbe r 2008 Z e ro to Three 3 1


begin to take more and more active roles in that hurt when Shayna hit you.” Looking at
emotion- and behavior-regulating interactions the child who took his toy, Gerry signed “all
with caregivers. In the CCFS classrooms, care- done.” Christy replied, “We can wait until
givers are encouraged to let children find their Shayna is all done,” signing “wait, all done.”
own solutions to distress and conflict, provid- Gerry responded by signing “wait” and “all
ing incremental emotional support, just as a done.”
sensitive caregiver would provide incremen- A minute later, Gerry gestured “wait”
tal physical support for a child learning to walk again. Christy responded, “You can play with
or climb. something else while you wait until Shayna is
all done,” signing “play, wait, all done.” Gerry
Illustration courtesy of BabySigns, Inc.

CLARA MANAGES SEPARATION BY watched Christy, then signed, “wait, all done,”
THINKING ABOUT REUNION TIME then nodded “yes.” Christy responded, “Let’s
Background: Popsicle Time (“Pops Time”) play with something else,” signing “play.”
is a daily routine at the CCFS, the time when Gerry smiled and pointed to another child
parents return for the children; children, across the room. Together Gerry and his
caregivers, and parents have popsicles caregiver walked across the room to play with
together, sing songs, and say good-bye. The the other child. (V)
stimulating popsicles provide a memory
aid for the children, something concrete to In this story, Gerry learned a sign to help with
remember and look forward to as the time conflict resolution in the future, the “stop”
The “Stop” sign. when parents return. The phrase “Pops Time” sign. (This sign is very popular in the toddler
and the accompanying sign (closed fist tap- classroom.) Gerry also actively participated
ping chin) become part of most separation in this regulatory interaction by using signs
raised his hands in the air and signed “stars.” routines and a reference used by caregivers to indicate that he was waiting to get the toy
I said, “That’s right. We usually sing about the when children are missing their parents. back, and then that he was all done waiting
stars and those are some stars up there.” I then and was ready to do something else. Together,
gently pushed on the stars. I started to sing, “Bye-bye, bye-bye,” said Clara (25 months), Gerry and his caregiver peacefully managed
“Twinkle, twinkle,. . .” and Andrew gestured waving her hand as she watched her mother a conflict with another child by using words
“star” throughout the song. (A) walk toward the gate. She turned away and and signs to reflect on and monitor Gerry’s
buried her face in my leg. I picked her up and internal states, as well as to decide on and
In the first anecdote, Andrew’s caregiver we watched her mom leave. Clara’s mouth was modify a course of action.
established a comforting routine during sealed tight. Her mom turned around and signed
diapering in which Andrew participated. In “Pops Time.” Clara watched and pointed to Using Signs in the Service of
the second anecdote, just one week later, the gate in front of her. “Mama’s going out the Self-Regulation
Andrew initiated this routine himself. gate,” I said, “but she will be back at Pops Time.” Once infants have responded to caregivers’
When her mom was out of sight, Clara put her regulatory signs and speech, and used signs to
HELENE REQUESTS A DIFFERENT SONG hand to her chin and gestured “Pops Time.” actively participate in regulatory interactions
Following snack, I carried Helene (12 months) She focused her eyes in the direction where her with caregivers, the next step in internalizing
into the nap room. As I sat down in the rocking mother had walked and waved again. (A) this self-regulatory self-talk is to use it inde-
chair with Helene in my lap, I started to sing pendently of the caregiver as a means to
“Twinkle, Twinkle, Little Star.” Helene gestured During this separation event, Clara waited regulate their own emotions and behavior.
“monkey.” So I sang “Monkeys Jumping on the until her mother had left, then used a sign for
Bed,” using the gestures with one hand, repeat- the specific time when Mom would return and MEGHAN WAITS FOR SNACK
edly until her eyelids began to droop. I continued waved good-bye again to her absent mother. Background: In the infant classroom, the
rocking her and let her fall into a deeper sleep Clara was not actually gesturing to her mother, snack area is separated from the rest of the
before attempting to set her down on the mat. but using the signs self-reflexively (to herself), classroom by a short gate, about knee-height
Each time I knelt to lay her down, Helene would possibly to comfort herself with idea of her for an adult and shoulder-height for an infant.
awaken and gesture “monkey.” I sang the mother’s return. It was typical of several infants to express
“Monkeys” song each time until her eyelids their anxiousness for snack time by rattling
again drooped. After the third time I was finally GERRY PEACEFULLY RESOLVES A the gate within the small margin that it could
able to place her down asleep on her mat. (A) PEER CONFLICT move, while their caregivers set up the snack
Gerry, a 21-month-old boy, was playing inside tables.
Helene’s caregiver began a routine they typ- the classroom. Shayna (another toddler) hit
ically shared. Helene used a sign to request a Gerry, and Gerry dropped the toy he was play- Meghan (12 months) and Shellie (11 months)
modification of that routine, then to initiate ing with, then Shayna took it. Gerry’s caregiver, were rattling the gate as they watched their
it as many times as she needed to fall asleep. Christy, talked to Gerry about what happened, caregivers set up for snack. Meghan’s caregiver
Signs provided both Andrew and Helene a using signs along with words. “You can tell turned to the infants and said, “We’re setting
way to make specific requests for comfort Shayna to stop when she hurts you,” Christy up for snack. It’ll be ready in a few minutes. I
during what could be emotionally challenging said, showing Gerry the sign for “stop.” Gerry need you to wait for snack while I set up.” She
events. responded by nodding “yes” and signing “stop.” signed “snack” and “wait” while she spoke.
Christy signed “stop” again, and again Gerry Meghan then stopped rattling the gate and
Using Signs in Participatory Regulation nodded “yes.” signed “snack, wait, snack, wait” repeatedly
As infants begin to internalize the regulatory Christy reflected on what happened, using while she watched her caregiver finish setting
speech and gestures of their caregivers, they the sign for “hurt” and saying, “It looks like the tables. (V)

3 2 Z e ro to Three Se pte mbe r 2008


Here Meghan changed her behavior from symbols—in the form of gestures—to com-
rattling the gate to signing about her internal municate about their internal states with
state. She signed throughout her wait, seem- caregivers and also to “talk” to themselves.
ingly to herself, whether her caregiver was This raises the question: Do signs reveal
watching or not. Was she using the signs to capacities of the infant that were always
think out loud, telling herself to wait, or sim- there, or do signs help infants develop
ply expressing that she was waiting? Either capacities they would not otherwise have?
way, Meghan used the signs in service of self- If language is truly a mental tool for
regulation while she was anxiously awaiting thought, then signs may provide babies with
her snack. the means to monitor and modify their own

Illustration courtesy of BabySigns, Inc.


emotions and behavior, abilities they would
KATE REMINDS HERSELF TO BE GENTLE not otherwise have until they were talking.
Background: Kate, one of the older infants in The concepts children represented in the
the classroom, was the youngest child of six. sign stories I have described—internal states
Both of her parents were often stressed, and like “hurt” or “waiting,” adjectives like “gen-
Kate was treated roughly by older siblings tle,” time concepts like “later” or “Popsicle
at home. She had become aggressive with Time”—were all somewhat abstract and dif-
younger infants in the classroom, repeatedly ficult concepts. Having a sign as an external
targeting one particular child, David. Her representation for these concepts actu-
caregivers worked with her patiently while ally gave the infants a way to understand and
protecting the other children. They frequently think with the concepts. The “Sad” sign.
used the sign for “gentle” (right hand stroking
left arm softly) when talking with her about
her behavior. “I need you to be gentle with the
other children. And other people need to be Learn More both hearing and deaf children, as well as special
gentle with you.” It had been several weeks needs children, based on the work of Joseph
since Kate, now 15 months old, had attacked Baby Signs Garcia, which emphasized how to teach adults to
another child when the child development www.babysigns.com sign with infants.
specialist observed the following interaction. This site offers the “story” of how Baby Signs
came to be, as well as curriculum and other Publications
Kate was kneeling on the floor by the toy shelves products for using the Baby Signs infant signing Baby Signs: How to Talk With Your Baby Before
when she looked across the classroom and saw curriculum. Your Baby Can Talk (Rev. ed.) by L. Acredolo,
David. She stared at David intensely, with that S. Goodwyn, & D. Adams (2002)
look in her eye she gets just before she attacks International Infant Sign Researchers New York, NY: McGraw-Hill
him. Then she started crawling quickly across http://groups.google.com/group/IISR This book provides both a history of the Baby
the classroom toward him. As she crawled, This Google Groups Web page offers contact Signs® story—from case study to experimental
her left arm jutted out in front of her and she with other researchers study infant signing, as well research—as well as a how-to guide to using infant
stopped, looked at her arm, and stroked it with as a bibliography of publications and presentations signs. It is written for a lay audience and is full of
her right arm, doing the “gentle” sign. She then about infant signing. engaging photos and stories from both a research
altered her path, crawling around David to and parent or practitioner perspective.
another section of the classroom. (A) Research on Infant Signing
Baby Signs Impact of Symbolic Gesturing on Early Language
In this episode Kate clearly changed the www.babysigns.com/index.cfm/fuseaction/ Development by S. W. Goodwyn, L. P. Acredolo, &
course of her own behavior. She stopped her institute.research/research.cfm C. A. Brown (June 2000) Journal of Nonverbal
own dominant response, not by telling herself This site offers a summary of the original Behavior, 24, 81–103. Special Issue: Gesture and
“no,” but by reminding herself of a different research on symbolic gestures by Linda Acredolo Development.
way to behave, using the same sign her care- and Susan Goodwyn, on which the book Baby This article describes the original experimen-
givers had used so often with her. Signs and the Baby Signs® program are based. tal research on the effects of encouraging parents
This site goes on to describe subsequent research to use symbolic gestures with their preverbal chil-
Discussion on the use of Baby Signs® by parents and child dren. Results show that contrary to popular fears,

A s these stories illustrate, infant


signs provide preverbal children with
a way to communicate with caregiv-
ers about what will comfort or reassure them,
a way to coconstruct and actively participate
care centers.

Signing Smart
www.wideeyedlearning.com/research.html
encouraging symbolic gestures actually enhances
language development.

Signs of Emotion: What Can Preverbal Children


This Web site summarizes the findings of a “Say” About Internal States? by C. D. Vallotton
in regulatory interactions, and a way to “talk” national study by Michelle Anthony and Reyna (2008) Infant Mental Health Journal, 29, 234–258
to themselves in the service of self-regulation. Lindert on children using the Signing Smart This article describes a study on infants’ use
Adults have internal dialogues of self-talk program. of signs to express emotion and feeling concepts.
that we use for self-monitoring, self-planning, The results show that infants and toddlers are
and self-regulation. Until now it was assumed Sign 2 Me capable of using signs to describe their own
that young children did not begin to develop www.sign2me.com/research.php and others’ emotions, and reveal sophisticated
this internal dialogue until they were well This site provides references for research understanding of the social–emotional world in
into using their first language. However, articles and a summary of research on signing with preverbal children.
infants’ use of signs makes visible their use of

Se pte mbe r 2008 Z e ro to Three 3 3


Infant signs are a form of label that Time,” “wait” for “snack,” be “gentle”). This
makes difficult concepts accessible to parallel use of signs implies two things about Claire Vallotton, PhD, is a postdoctoral
preverbal children. They work not only children’s social cognition. First, preverbal fellow at the Harvard University Graduate School
because children are capable of performing children have a sense of shared meaning for of Education. Her research is supported by a
the signs before they can speak the words, the content of the signs. Second, they have Clinical Research Fellowship from the National
but also because signs are physical, more a certain degree of consciousness about the Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
concrete, and thus may be easier to under- socially constructed appropriateness of their ment. She studies the relation between symbol
stand than words (Werner & Kaplan, own behaviors. skills—including gestures and words—and social
1963). In conclusion, infant signs allow children development in very young children in the con-
Supporting Vygotksy’s (1934/1986) to take self-regulation into their own hands. text of early child–caregiver relationships. She
hypothesis about the internalization of self- They provide children a way to participate was honored with the 2006 New Investigator
regulatory self-talk, the children’s use of signs with caregivers in regulatory interactions and Award from the World Association of Infant Men-
in both participatory regulation and self- to begin to develop the internal dialogue that tal Health for her work showing that infants can
regulation was very similar to the ways that will become their own conscious and self- use gestures (infant signs) to communicate about
caregivers used them (e.g., later at “Pops regulatory self-talk. A emotions.

References Manfra, L., & Winsler, A. (2006). Preschool chil- Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and Language (Rev.
dren’s awareness of private speech. International ed.). A. Kozulin, Ed. Cambridge, MA: The MIT
Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (1985). Symbolic Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 537–549. Press. (Original work published 1934)
gesturing in language development: A case Pea, R. (1980). The development of negation in Wagner, M. (2006). First steps to communication: A
study. Human Development, 28, 40–49. early child language. In D. R. Olson (Ed.), The pragmatic analysis. Tübingen, Germany: Narr
Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic social foundations of language and thought: Essays Francke Attempto Verlag.
gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, in honor of Jerome Bruner (pp. 156–186). New Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation.
59, 450–466. York: Norton & Company. Oxford, England: Wiley.
Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (1992). Baby signs: Rodriguez, C., & Palacios, P. (2007). Do private ges- Wertsch, J. V. (1979). From social interaction to
How to talk with your baby before your baby can tures have a self-regulatory function? A case study. higher psychological processes: A clarification
talk. Chicago: Contemporary Books. Infant Behavior and Development, 30, 180–194. and application of Vygotsky’s theory. Human
Crais, E., Douglas, D. D., & Campbell, C. C. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Development, 22, 1–22.
(2004). The intersection of the development Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant Winsler, A., De León, J. R., & Wallace, B. A. (2003).
of gestures and intentionality. Journal of Speech, pointing. Child Development, 78, 705–722. Private speech in preschool children: Develop-
Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 678–694. Vallotton, C. D. (2008). Signs of emotion: What mental stability and change, across-task consis-
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). The two faces of ges- can preverbal children “say” about internal tency, and relations with classroom behaviour.
ture: Language and thought. Gesture, 5, 241–257. states? Infant Mental Health Journal, 29, 234–258. Journal of Child Language, 30, 583–608.

3 4 Z e ro to Three Se pte mbe r 2008

You might also like