You are on page 1of 10

Marine Biology (2004) 145: 435–444

DOI 10.1007/s00227-004-1339-1

R ES E AR C H A RT I C L E

K. J. Benoit-Bird

Prey caloric value and predator energy needs: foraging predictions


for wild spinner dolphins

Received: 17 April 2003 / Accepted: 16 February 2004 / Published online: 25 March 2004
Ó Springer-Verlag 2004

Abstract Spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) feed on Finally, the results suggest that spinner dolphins are
individual small (2–10 cm long) prey that undergo diel time and therefore efficiency limited rather than being
vertical migrations, presumably making them inaccessi- limited by the total amount of available prey. This may
ble to dolphins during the day. To examine how time, explain the diel migration exhibited by spinner dolphins
prey behavior, prey distribution, and energy needs that allows them to follow the movements of their prey
constrain dolphin foraging, a calorimeter was used to and presumably maximizes their foraging time.
measure the caloric content of prey items. These data
were combined with information on prey distribution in
the field and the energetic needs of dolphins to construct
basic bioenergetic models predicting the total prey con- Introduction
sumption and mean feeding rates of wild dolphins as
well as potential prey preferences. The mean caloric Bioenergetics models of foraging have provided useful
density of mesopelagic animals from Hawaii was high predictions about the behavior and population dynamics
(2,837 cal/g wet weight for shrimps, squids, and myc- of many predators. For example, models incorporating
tophid fishes). Their total caloric content, however, was prey energy values and predator energetic needs have
low because of their small size. Energy value of prey and been used to predict dive times in blue whales (Acevedo-
energetic needs of spinner dolphins were used to exam- Gutierrez et al. 2002), total hunting time for wild dogs
ine the effect of time and energy constraints on dolphin (Gorman et al. 1998), and growth rates in flounder
foraging. The results predict that spinner dolphins need (Burke and Rice 2002). Models incorporating foraging
to consume an estimated minimum of 1.25 large prey energetics in pelagic predators have also been useful in
items per minute to meet their maintenance energy examining prey preferences and understanding the
needs. If the additional energy costs of foraging are impact of the predator on the ecosystem (Bunce 2001).
considered, the estimated necessary foraging rate is These kinds of predictions are particularly useful when
predicted to increase only slightly when large prey are studying populations that are difficult to observe
consumed. If smaller prey are consumed, the total en- directly, such as pelagic marine animals. These models
ergy demand may be twice the basic maintenance value. provide testable hypotheses for data that are often more
Prey density and size are predicted to be important in easily measured than the parameters of the model and
determining if dolphins can forage successfully, meeting can also provide explanations for observed behaviors.
their energetic needs. The prey size predictions compare Spinner dolphins, Stenella longirostris, a subtropical
well with results from previous gut content studies and pelagic species, consume small, mesopelagic prey, pre-
from stomach contents of a recently stranded spinner sumably one individual at a time (Norris and Dohl
dolphin that had enough prey in its stomach to meet its 1980). Analyses of stomach contents of spinner dolphins
estimated basic maintenance energy needs for a day. revealed that they are primarily nocturnal feeders; their
stomachs are full when caught early in the day and
Communicated by P.W. Sammarco, Chauvin
empty when caught at other times of the day (Norris and
Dohl 1980). Stomachs of spinners caught off the coast of
K. J. Benoit-Bird West Africa (Cadenat and Doutre 1959) and in the
Hawaii Institute of Marine Biology, eastern tropical Pacific (Perrin et al. 1973) contained
P.O. Box 1106, Kailua, HI 96734, USA
E-mail: benoit@hawaii.edu
mostly myctophid fish. Fitch and Brownell (1968) found
Tel.: +1-808-2475063 that two species of myctophid fishes, Benthosema pana-
Fax: +1-808-2475831 mense and Lampanyctus parvicauda, accounted for more
436

than 50% of the spinner’s diet in the eastern tropical pattern observed in spinner dolphins. This approach is
Pacific but animals from this area also had large num- conceptually similar to the bioenergetics model of
bers of individuals from other mesopelagic fish families Stockwell and Johnson (1999), which incorporated for-
including Gonostomatidae, Melamphaeidae, and Breg- aging constraints to predict diel migration observed in
macerotidae (Fitch and Brownell 1968; Perrin et al. fish. Simple models were also used to predict potential
1973). Mesopelagic squid species, mostly from the prey preferences, both by size and by taxonomic iden-
family Enoploteuthidae, also accounted for a large tity. While limited information is available for estimat-
proportion (nearly 30%) of the prey consumed by ing the parameters of the model, the relative predictions
spinner dolphins in the eastern tropical Pacific (Perrin of the model are robust to changes in these parameters
et al. 1973). and provide information on the importance of prey size
Stomach contents of spinner dolphins caught or for potential foraging success. Model predictions were
stranded in Hawaiian waters are similar to those caught preliminarily compared with results from the stomach
in other locations, containing primarily small (2–10 cm) contents of a single stranded animal as well as previously
mesopelagic fish and squid. Norris et al. (1994) found published gut contents studies.
that more than 50% by number of the contents of
Hawaiian spinner dolphin stomachs were myctophid
fish. Hawaiian spinner dolphins also consumed large Materials and methods
proportions of mesopelagic squid; Abralia trigonura and
A. astrosticta were the most abundant species (Norris Prey value
and Dohl 1980; Clarke and Young 1998). One difference Animals from the mesopelagic boundary community were captured
in the diet of Hawaiian spinner dolphins from that of alive using an Isaacs–Kidd midwater trawl. Their backscattering
animals from other locations is the relatively large strength (Benoit-Bird and Au 2001), wet weight, dry weight, dis-
contribution of the sergestid crustacean Sergia lucens to placement volume, and caloric content (Benoit-Bird and Au 2002)
were measured. Calorie measurements were made using a bomb
their diet, as well as a small contribution by pasiphaeid calorimeter.
crustaceans (Norris and Dohl 1980; Norris et al. 1994).
Nearly all of the species identified from the stomachs
of Hawaiian spinner dolphins are components of the Total caloric needs
mesopelagic boundary community (Reid et al. 1991).
The mesopelagic boundary community is a land-asso- No food intake values are available for spinner dolphins, Stenella
longirostris, either in the wild or in captivity. To estimate the total
ciated community that has a composition of fish, energy required for maintenance of a spinner dolphin, the kiloca-
shrimp, and squid that is distinct from the micronek- lories per kilogram of body weight required to maintain a captive
tonic community found in waters beyond the archipel- striped dolphin, Stenella coeruleoalba (Kastelein et al. 2002a), was
ago’s slopes (Reid 1994). Many of the species found in applied to the full range of spinner dolphin weights and compared
the boundary community migrate diurnally, reaching with the average maintenance energy values for other delphinids in
captivity.
depths of 400–700 m during the day and ranging from
the surface to 400 m at night (Reid 1994). The boundary
community also migrates horizontally, moving nearly Stomach contents of a stranded spinner dolphin
2 km toward shore until midnight, when it achieves its
maximum density in waters approximately 1 km from As a preliminary test of the viability of the model proposed, the
stomach contents of a stranded animal were examined and com-
the shoreline. After midnight, the community then mi- pared, along with published reports of spinner dolphin gut con-
grates away from shore as it descends (Benoit-Bird et al. tents, with the model predictions. A sub-adult female spinner
2001). Rather than foraging offshore for the entire night, dolphin, weighing approximately 45 kg, stranded early in the
spinner dolphins track the horizontal migration of their morning of 7 July 2002 on Magic Island, Ala Moana Park on
the south shore of Oahu. Despite attempts to save and rehabilitate
prey (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). It is hypothesized that the animal, it died. The animal appeared well nourished and no
this tracking of their prey allows spinner dolphins to cause of death was apparent after necropsy. The stomach of the
maximize their foraging time while foraging on the prey animal was dissected within 24 h of death, and the identifiable prey
at its highest densities (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). parts, including fish otoliths, squid lenses and beaks, and whole
To examine the effect of time and energy constraints animals, were frozen, measured, and identified to the lowest taxo-
nomic level possible.
on spinner dolphins, data on the energy content of To determine a lower limit to the caloric content of the prey
spinner dolphin prey and estimates of the foraging needs consumed by this animal, the caloric content was estimated from
of spinner dolphins are presented. Prey energy density the wet weight of the stomach contents. Unfortunately, no catalog
values are crucial inputs to bioenergetic consumption exists for the otoliths of the myctophids or the beaks of the squids
found in the Hawaiian boundary layer so the caloric content of
models but were not available for mesopelagic micro- individual prey items could not be calculated by relating the size
nekton in general, or for the specific mesopelagic prey of of the hard parts to the length of the prey items. The distribution of
spinner dolphins in Hawaii. The effects of various for- prey caloric content per gram of wet weight was unimodal and not
aging costs on energy needs and foraging rates, as well as significantly different from a normal distribution. Because there
were no significant differences between groups, the mean calories
the relationship between available prey and spinner per gram of wet weight for all groups was multiplied by the total
dolphin energy needs, were examined to determine what weight of the stomach contents to estimate the total caloric value of
factors may be important in driving the diel migration the consumed prey.
437

The data available on animals from the Hawaiian boundary fit (R2=0.76) after removing the single outlier, a nearly
community is limited, making it impossible to estimate the length 10-cm-long myctophid. The relationship between the
of prey from the hard parts in the stomach. Two methods were
used to estimate the average length of prey items eaten. First, the wet weight of individual animals and their energy con-
total calorie–length relationship for squid and myctophids was tent in calories is shown in Fig. 1b (calories=2,496*wet
applied to the estimated average caloric content of each prey item. weight+55; R2=0.95). The outlier did not significantly
Second, we applied the wet weight–length relationship for squid affect this relationship. The distribution of prey caloric
and myctophids separately to the average wet weight of each prey
item. The total length of any whole animals was directly measured.
content per gram of wet weight (Fig. 2) was unimodal
and approximated a normal distribution. There were
also no significant differences in the caloric content per
Models and parameters
gram of animal wet weight between shrimp, myctophids,
Simple models based on the energetic needs of dolphins and the and squid (ANOVA; df=2, P>0.10). The caloric den-
energy content of prey were used to predict the number of prey that sity of animals from the mesopelagic boundary com-
an individual spinner dolphin needs to consume. Information on munity is high (mean 2,837 cal/g wet weight) relative to
prey behavior was used to convert these estimates to average rates food commonly fed to marine mammals in captivity
of prey consumption. The consequences of foraging costs, derived
from the literature, on these estimates were explored. The effects of (Table 1). Only anchovies and sardines, both high in oil
prey size on rate estimates were also examined. The effects of prey content like mesopelagic animals, come close to the
size on foraging estimates allowed prediction of prey size prefer- energy density of the food consumed by spinner
ence. Possible preferences for prey type were investigated based on dolphins in the wild (Kizevetter 1971/1973; Honda et al.
the acoustic scattering of prey and the swimming ability of prey
obtained from the literature. Finally, information on the distribu- 1992).
tion of prey in the field was combined with the foraging models to
understand the limitations faced by spinner dolphins in the wild.
Total caloric needs
Results The maintenance requirement for a striped dolphin,
54 kcal/kg of dolphin per day (Kastelein et al. 2002a),
Prey value
compares well with the average maintenance energy
values of 50 kcal/kg of dolphin per day reported for
The total caloric content of individual prey items from
other delphinids in captivity (J. Pawloski and M. Breese,
the boundary community measured with a bomb calo-
personal communication). The estimated kilocalories,
rimeter increases with increasing animal length (Fig. 1a).
assuming a 54 kcal/kg maintenance need, required for
The relationship can be described by a power function
maintenance each day by spinner dolphins of various
for myctophid fishes, and a linear function approxi-
sizes are shown in Table 2. These represent the best
mately describes the relationship for shrimp and squid
estimates available for the average maintenance energy
where E is energy content in calories, and L is total
needs for a spinner dolphin. Differences in these values
length in centimeters for all three groups.
affect the results of the model by shifting curves but do
EFish ¼ 8.8  L2:64 ; R2 ¼ 0.87; n ¼ 58 not change the relative relationships, so for simplicity,
these fixed values are utilized throughout.
EShrimp ¼ 228  L  78.2, R2 ¼ 0.87; n ¼ 14

ESquid ¼ 214  L  379.6; R2 ¼ 0.80; n ¼ 8 Stomach contents of a stranded spinner dolphin

A single linear relationship can be fitted through The entire contents of the stomach weighed 834 g (wet)
all three groups pooled for simplification of analysis and had a displacement volume of 3.7 l. The identified
(ETotal=388*L)1,139) while retaining a relatively strong contents included 2 intact shrimp (both Sergia fulgens),
Fig. 1 a Prey total caloric
content, measured with a bomb
calorimeter, of mesopelagic
animals from the Hawaiian
mesopelagic boundary layer
as a function of animal length.
b Total caloric value of all three
taxonomic groups of
mesopelagic prey from the
Hawaiian boundary layer was
strongly correlated with wet
weight
438

estimate of the average squid total length of 11.4 cm and


an average myctophid total length of 7.8 cm. Applying
the wet weight–length relationship for squid and myc-
tophids separately to the average wet weight of each
prey item yielded an estimate for the average squid total
length of 8.4 cm and an average myctophid total length
of 7.7 cm. The total length of the 117 whole squids ex-
tracted from the stomach ranged from 2.4 to 9.6 cm,
with a mean of 7.7 cm (Fig. 3).

Models and parameters

The steps in the derivation of the models and their


parameters build successively; each component of the
model is presented under a separate heading along with
justification for parameter choices, a more detailed
Fig. 2 Distribution of calories per gram of wet weight for all
mesopelagic boundary community animals measured. No signifi- description of the methods used to generate the results,
cant differences were observed between myctophid fishes, shrimps, and the important findings.
and squids

1,012 fish otoliths (96% were identified as myctophids: Foraging costs


88% Benthosema, 8% Diaphus), 117 whole squids [all
identified as mesopelagic boundary community species Maintenance energy requirements obtained from captive
(Reid et al. 1991)], 720 squid eye lenses, and 102 squid studies include only costs associated with feeding and
beaks. These prey bodies and parts were interpreted to routine activities, and not those associated with forag-
represent at least 985 individual prey. Because spinner ing. To understand the importance of food to spinner
dolphins captured in the afternoon typically have com- dolphin behavior, the effects of foraging costs on energy
pletely empty stomachs (Perrin et al. 1973, 1999), it is needs and food intake rates need to be explored. The
likely that all 985 of the prey items represented in the energetic costs experienced by foraging spinner dolphins
stomach contents were eaten the night before the animal come from swimming, diving, acoustic searching, and
stranded. ‘herding’ of prey and finally from behaviors associated
The estimated caloric content of the stomach con- with the capture of the prey. Spinner dolphins foraging
tents was about 2,400 kcal, making the average caloric on the Hawaiian mesopelagic boundary community
content of each prey item about 2,400 cal. Because of move at least 8 km back and forth over the slope of the
the degree of digestion and consideration of only the island each night while following the horizontal migra-
stomach, not the entire gut, this should be regarded as a tion of their prey (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). Near the
minimum estimate of the calories consumed by this beginning and end of the prey’s migration, spinner
animal. The maintenance energy needs of a spinner dolphins probably dive to depths of at least 150 m. Near
dolphin this size, without considering possible growth the apex of the migration, their diving depths are re-
costs, were estimated at 2,440 kcal per day (Table 2). duced to an average of 25 m (Benoit-Bird and Au
The size of prey consumed by this dolphin was of 2003a). Hawaiian spinner dolphins feed cooperatively,
interest for preliminarily examining the results from the in pairs, within relatively large groups (20 or more ani-
models presented. The total calorie–length relationship mals). They appear to actively concentrate their prey by
for squid and myctophids was applied to the estimated swimming in a circle about 25 m in diameter around the
average caloric content of each prey item, giving an outside edges of the prey (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a).

Table 1 Mean caloric densities


(calories per gram of wet Mean caloric density Source
weight) for prey items
commonly fed to marine Boundary community 2,837 This article
mammals compared with prey Anchovy 2,808 Kizevetter 1971/1973
of Hawaiian spinner dolphins Sardines 2,300 Honda et al. 1992
and other midwater Capelin 1,266–1,863 Martensson et al. 1996
micronektonic animals Herring 1,480–1,831 Kastelein et al. 2002b; Martensson et al. 1996
Other midwater vertical 1,800 Brooks 1977
migrators
Mackeral 1,700 Cox et al. 1996
Cod 1,003)1,340 Smith et al. 1997; Martensson et al. 1996
Smelt 1,200 M. Breese, personal communication
Squid 852 Cox et al. 1996
439

Table 2 Estimated kilocalories required by spinner dolphins each


day for maintenance

Dolphin weight (kg) kcal day)1

Stranded animal 45 2,430


Small adult female 55 2,970
Average adult 65 3,520
Large adult male 75 4,050

Fig. 4 The number of prey items necessary per day as a function of


mean prey length eaten for an average-size spinner dolphin to meet
its basic energy requirements at different fixed search costs per prey
item

costs from 0.00 to 0.15 kcal per prey item was investi-
gated. This represents an increase of 0–8% over the
Fig. 3 Distribution of squid total length for animals extracted
whole from the stomach of a stranded Hawaiian spinner dolphin animal’s maintenance energy during active foraging.
Increases in metabolic rate of 10% for efficient swim-
ming (Yazdi et al. 1999) and 20% or more for diving
The foraging costs to spinner dolphins of finding have been observed in dolphins (Williams et al. 1999).
prey, diving to the layer, and ‘herding’ are associated Because dolphins may not be swimming and diving for
with prey patches and are relatively isolated from indi- the entire foraging time, lower costs were used in the
vidual prey, so they will be referred to collectively as calculations. However, values up to a 50% increase over
‘search’ costs. The energetic costs associated with prey maintenance energy do not affect the conclusions of the
search (swimming, acoustic searching, diving, and analysis. The effect of adding fixed costs on the number
‘herding’) are not likely to be affected by the size of of prey that must be consumed by an average-size
individual prey. The costs of swimming (see Yazdi et al. spinner dolphin to meet its maintenance energy costs,
1999) over the island’s slopes to find prey would not be calculated using the estimated net gain of energy for
dependent on prey size. Acoustic prey searching ability individual prey of each size, is shown in Fig. 4. In all
at a large scale is not affected by individual prey size, but cases, the number of prey necessary to meet the animal’s
by the distribution of the prey patch or entire scattering energy needs decreases with increasing prey size. When
layer because of volume reverberation (Urick 1983). no search cost is assessed and prey average 2.5 cm in
Costs associated with diving to the prey layer (see Wil- length, a total of nearly 15,000 prey items must be eaten.
liams et al. 1999) are probably independent of prey size When a dolphin consumes large prey, averaging 10 cm
because the depth of prey is not strongly correlated with in length (the largest prey available within the boundary
individual animal size in the upper 150 m (Reid 1994). layer), about 825 prey items must be consumed. If search
Finally, the conserved nature of the observed coopera- costs remain the same regardless of prey size, the costs
tive ‘herding’ behavior of prey by spinner dolphins will have a greater impact on an animal foraging on
(Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a) suggests relatively constant small prey. When a meager 0.15 kcal/prey cost is added
effort is used to herd each prey patch. These search costs (less than the increase in metabolic costs of swimming
are amortized over all prey in each patch successfully for the entire foraging bout), the estimated number of
captured in a foraging bout, and consequently, within a prey necessary to meet a dolphin’s predicted energy
foraging bout they would be the same for each item, needs goes up to nearly 40,000 for a 2.5-cm prey item
regardless of the item’s caloric content. (Figure 4). This is a more than threefold increase over
Little information is available with which to estimate the estimate with no search cost. For 10-cm prey, the
search costs for spinner dolphins directly; however, their estimated increase in the number of prey that must be
effects on spinner dolphin total energetic needs and consumed when a 0.15 kcal/prey cost is added only
foraging rates can be investigated. A range of search represents a few percent over the number that must be
440

consumed when no cost is assessed. The results can also Other costs
be looked at as the estimated total calories that must be
consumed by an individual dolphin as a function of prey Relatively little information is available on the energetic
size at different search costs (Fig. 5a). This suggests that costs of lactation or pregnancy in cetaceans. Captive
animals should focus foraging effort on prey items about bottlenose dolphins did not change the number of cal-
6 cm in total length or longer to minimize the number of ories consumed during gestation but increased their
prey and consequently the total number of calories they caloric consumption by 52–97% during lactation (Kas-
need to consume. Changing the maintenance energy of telein et al. 2002b). Other mammals that feed their off-
the individual or changing the search costs to higher spring for a similar amount of time, including hamsters
values has no effect on this prediction. and humans, also increase their energy consumption by
Search costs are probably independent of prey size, more than 50% during lactation (Day et al. 2002; Du-
but prey capture costs probably increase with prey size four et al. 2002). Estimated increases in the average-size
because prey mobility also increases. If prey capture female spinner dolphin’s food intake by various per-
costs increase proportionately with prey size as observed centages to represent the energetic costs of lactation over
in fish (Sambilay 1990) and squid (Yatsu et al. 1999), the maintenance energy are shown in Fig. 6. The data in this
simplest case, then the prey capture cost can be ex- figure can also be used to estimate changes in food in-
pressed as a percentage of the post-search value of the take due to other causes such as growth, health, and
prey (net prey value=gross prey caloric content)0.0– environmental changes, among others, as these change
0.15 kcal fixed search cost per prey)capture cost ex- the maintenance energy requirements. For example,
pressed as a percentage caloric content of each prey; growth in a subadult captive dusky dolphin resulted in
Fig. 5b). The values selected are within the same range approximately a 10% increase in maintenance energy
as the percentage increase in metabolic rates associated over the 50 kcal/kg base value (Kastelein et al. 2000),
with burst swimming over efficient swimming (5–20%; resulting in a modest increase in estimated total energy
Yazdi et al. 1999). This added prey capture cost de- that must be consumed by this growing animal.
creases the slope of the total calorie curve between small
and large prey items. Changing the prey capture cost to How fast must a dolphin eat?
a higher value shifts the curve up and further decreases
the difference in the estimated total calories that must be Spinner dolphins have a limited effective foraging depth,
consumed between small and large prey. However, even estimated at between 200 and 250 m (Fitch and
at the lowest fixed search cost (0.05 kcal/prey), it is not
possible to negate the differences between small and
large prey in the estimated total calories that need to be
consumed by the dolphin. Regardless of the prey capture
cost, dolphins are still predicted to preferentially con-
sume larger prey.

Fig. 6 Estimated total caloric needs for the average-size female


spinner dolphin at different non-foraging costs. The costs of
foraging on 7.5-cm-long prey are included at both a fixed, per prey
item level, and a fixed plus percentage foraging cost. The dotted
Fig. 5 a Total calories necessary per day for an average-size region shows the range of costs of lactation observed in captive
spinner dolphin to meet its basic energy requirements as a function bottlenose dolphins by Kastelein et al. (2002b). This graph can also
of prey size at different fixed per-prey-item search costs. b Total be used to examine the effects of other costs such as growth or
calories that must be consumed by an average-size spinner dolphin stress. Growth, for example, added a 10% cost to maintenance
to meet its basic energy requirements with both a fixed search cost needs for a captive dolphin of another species (Kastelein et al.
per prey item and a prey capture cost that increases with prey size 2000)
441

Brownell 1968; Dolar et al. 2003) although they may be


able to dive to depths of up to 400 m (Dolar et al. 2003).
This means that the boundary community is inaccessible
to spinner dolphins during the day due to its depth at
between 400 and 700 m at that time. Based on the ver-
tical migration patterns observed in the boundary layer
by Reid (1994) and Benoit-Bird et al. (2001), the
boundary layer is probably accessible to spinner dol-
phins for approximately 11 h of each day. An average-
size adult spinner dolphin eating only the largest prey
available (10 cm in length) would need to average an
estimated 1.25 prey items per minute of its probable
foraging time for maintenance energy. A dolphin con-
suming 2.5 cm prey is predicted to average 22 prey/min
over the 11 h the boundary community is available to
spinner dolphins if no foraging costs are considered.
Adding both prey search and capture costs does not
substantially alter the predicted rate of a dolphin con- Fig. 7 The relationship between total length of individual prey
suming large prey, increasing it to only an estimated taxonomic groups and their dorsal-aspect acoustic backscattering
maximum of 1.4 prey/min. If the length of prey averaged cross-section at 200 kHz (Redrawn from Benoit-Bird and Au 2001)
7.5 cm, the model estimates that between 3 and 4 prey/
min would need to be consumed, depending on search on a spinner dolphin’s ability to meet its energetic needs.
and capture costs. If prey were on average only 2.5 cm While all groups of prey exhibit an increase in back-
long, the estimated number of prey consumed per min- scattering strength with increasing length, the differences
ute would range between 22 and 46, depending on total in backscatter from shrimp and squid over the range of
foraging costs. available prey sizes is small (Benoit-Bird and Au 2001).
Because backscattering strength is also affected by ori-
entation of the animal (Benoit-Bird and Au 2001) and its
Possible prey preferences behavior (Arnaya et al. 1989a, 1989b), these small dif-
ferences probably make it difficult for a dolphin to
No strong differences in the amount of energy available estimate accurately the size of a shrimp or squid using
within the different major taxonomic groups of meso- echolocation. Myctophid fishes show a much stronger
pelagic prey around Hawaii are apparent, so no length–backscattering strength relationship, making it
advantage of prey groups is clearly evident (no proxi- easier to assess the prey’s size with echolocation before it
mate analyses were conducted so no conclusions can be is consumed. If echolocation is important in prey
made about other potential differences in food quality). detection and selection in spinner dolphins, myctophids
However, there are probably differences in the costs of should be preferred over shrimp or squid of equal size.
foraging upon the different prey groups, particularly in Once prey is detected and selected, there are probably
prey detection and handling. Assuming spinner dolphins differences in the ability of spinner dolphins to capture
use echolocation to find their prey, the backscattering prey of the different groups. Large differences between
strength of prey items determines the ability of cetaceans groups exist in the swimming rates of individuals of
to detect them. Shrimp and squid have a much smaller similar size. Sustained swimming speeds in a species of
backscattering cross-section than myctophids, meaning shrimp in the same genus, of the same size and body type,
that they are weaker echolocation targets (Fig. 7). Al- as the most abundant Hawaiian boundary community
though spinner dolphins use a very different sonar signal shrimp, reach 0.06 m/s, with burst speeds of 0.2 m/s
than the one used by Benoit-Bird and Au (2001) to (Cowles 2001). Similar burst speeds have been observed
measure scattering from spinner dolphin prey, their in krill, similar in size and body plan to mesopelagic
relative scattering strengths probably remain similar. boundary shrimp, when moving forward; however, re-
The difference in scattering strength between fish, verse thrusts can reach 1.1 m/s and can cover 0.6 m (Kils
shrimp, and squid has been observed at individual fre- 1979). Hawaiian boundary community myctophids reach
quencies between 30 and 220 kHz (MacLennan and much higher sustained speeds of 0.75 m/s (Reid 1994).
Simmonds 1992), covering the entire frequency range of Burst swimming speeds of fish with the size and fin type
spinner dolphin echolocation signals (Schotten et al. of myctophids are estimated at around 3 m/s (Sambilay
2004). Because of these differences in scattering strength, 1990). Burst speeds of squid (corrected for body length)
individual shrimp and squid would be detectable to an are even higher at 8 m/s, with cruising speeds of 0.8 m/s
echolocating dolphin over less range than myctophid (Yatsu et al. 1999). Based on swimming speed estimates,
fishes. There are also important differences in the ability shrimp are probably the easiest common prey for spinner
of sonar to determine prey size between the prey groups, dolphins to capture, with myctophids intermediate, and
significant because of the hypothesized effect of prey size squid the most difficult.
442

The distribution of mesopelagic boundary commu- Because of the constancy of these differences, differences
nity species and their relative abundance is currently in the population sizes that could be supported by these
unknown. However, the combination of ease of detec- prey layers are predicted. If total available prey energy is
tion and size determination of myctophids with echo- more important in determining spinner dolphin foraging
location and their moderate swimming abilities success, the population density of spinner dolphins off
compared with the other prey choices suggest that they Hawaii’s leeward coast is predicted to be roughly double
should be preferentially consumed over the other two that of the leeward Oahu population. If prey density is
prey groups. Shrimp and squid would both be more more important for foraging success, the density of
difficult to detect; however the order of magnitude spinner dolphins off the leeward Oahu coast should be
slower burst speeds of shrimp suggest that, given equal higher. Information on the population densities of
sizes, they should be preferentially consumed over squid. spinner dolphins is not available, but differences in
Information on the distribution of these groups will help population sizes between locations may provide insights
to refine these predictions based on diving costs of dol- into the foraging mechanisms used by spinner dolphins.
phins and relative availability of the prey groups.
Preliminary model assessment
Total prey availability Stomach contents from individual dolphins probably
vary widely, particularly when considering only stranded
The density of mesopelagic boundary community ani- animals. The estimate of the caloric value of prey in the
mals around the Hawaiian Islands varies as a function of stomach of this animal cannot be considered a perfect
time, distance from the shoreline, and location. The mean measure of the energy consumed, particularly as the
density of animals found nearshore during the night is contents of the remainder of the digestive track were not
between 5 and 25 animals/m3. Maximum densities can saved, but it does represent the minimum this animal
reach up to 1,800 animals/m3 (Benoit-Bird et al. 2001). consumed on this night. The stomach contents of this
Areas with spinner dolphins have the highest densities animal show that it is possible for a wild dolphin to meet
observed (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). Utilizing the the minimum energy needs proposed, consume the large
acoustic scattering–caloric content relationships estab- number of prey predicted by the model in the time it had
lished by Benoit-Bird and Au (2002) for mesopelagic to forage, and to fit the volume of this prey inside its
boundary community animals, the minimum available stomach.
energy within the layer was estimated. To simplify esti-
mates, only the myctophid relationship was utilized,
giving the most conservatively low estimate of energy
possible. The mean caloric density of the mesopelagic Discussion
boundary community is estimated at 83 kcal/m3 with a
maximum of nearly 9,000 kcal/m3. The average caloric The energy available within the mesopelagic boundary
density of areas in which spinner dolphins were found community far exceeds the energy necessary to support
ranged between 112 and 186 kcal/m3, with each single, the spinner dolphin population off the leeward coasts of
discrete patch of prey containing spinner dolphins having Oahu and Hawaii where spinner dolphins are most
between 1.65*106 kcal and 5.95*106 kcal (Benoit-Bird commonly sighted, and it is very densely distributed
and Au 2003a). These patches in which spinner dolphins (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003b). Each discrete patch in
were foraging have enough energy to support the esti- which dolphins were found but in which no other
mated maintenance energy needs of between 510 and boundary community predators were detected had en-
1,830 average-size spinner dolphins if they could exploit ough prey to support an order of magnitude or two
all the prey within the patch. The maximum number of more dolphins than were detected in each patch. This
dolphins observed in a single patch at any one time was suggests that spinner dolphin foraging is limited by time
26, much lower than could be supported by the patch. and consequently efficiency, rather than by the avail-
Large, consistent differences in the numerical density ability of food. The diel migration behavior of the
and available energy of prey were observed between mesopelagic boundary community probably eliminates
coastlines (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003b). The density of the possibility of daytime foraging by spinner dolphins
animals in the nearshore layer off the leeward coast of and may alter the costs of foraging throughout the night
Oahu during the night averaged 1.6 times the density as prey depth and prey density change over the migra-
observed off the leeward coast of Hawaii near tion.
Kealakekua Bay (Benoit-Bird et al. 2001). However, the Analysis of the effects of including even small incre-
total available energy per kilometer surveyed off Hawaii mental foraging costs in the model reveals that foraging
averaged twice that off the leeward coast of Oahu by spinner dolphins on small prey probably increases
because of differences in the vertical extent of the layer. energetic requirements dramatically. Changes in the ex-
While prey off Hawaii were found in discrete, relatively act parameters used in the model change the absolute
small patches (tens of meters across), the prey off Oahu numbers predicted, but not the shape of the curve or the
were found in extensive layers, covering kilometers. relative predictions. Because of the curve’s shape, pre-
443

dictions from these simple foraging cost models, The total available energy in the mesopelagic
regardless of the costs used, suggest that spinner dol- boundary community is substantial and exceeds the
phins should selectively forage on prey longer than needs of the population of spinner dolphins by orders of
about 6 cm. These predictions match well with the magnitude. Even an individual prey patch can support
lengths of prey observed in stomach contents. The esti- many times more dolphins than are actively foraging
mated total length of myctophids (based on partially within it. This huge excess of energy suggests that den-
digested remains) was 7.7–7.8 cm. Dolar et al. (2003) sity of prey rather than the total available biomass
found five species of myctophids with average total probably determines the density, and consequently the
lengths (estimated from standard lengths) of 6.3– population size, of spinner dolphins. Changes in density
13.1 cm. The mean measured length of whole squid from of prey probably have strong effects on the foraging
the single spinner dolphin’s stomach examined in this efficiency (i.e., prey capture rate) of spinner dolphins.
article was 7.7 cm. The estimated total length of the This is evidenced by the strong positive correlation be-
other squid (based on partially digested remains) was tween spinner dolphins and areas of high prey density
8.4–11.4 cm. The mean total length of squid (converted both vertically and horizontally (Benoit-Bird and Au
from mantle lengths) found in spinner dolphin stomachs 2003a). It appears that spinner dolphins actively increase
in Hawaii by Norris and Dohl (1980) was about 7.8 cm the density of their prey through group foraging
and by Dolar et al. (2003) was 7.6 cm. The distribution behavior that decreases the size of prey patches while
of squid lengths measured in this study shows an inverse increasing their aggregation (Benoit-Bird and Au
relationship to the size distribution of most boundary 2003a). For this behavior to be adaptive, this investment
community animals; animals in the boundary commu- of time by spinner dolphins to create prey areas with
nity have a mean length of about 5 cm with a distribu- favorable characteristics must be outweighed by
tion skewed toward smaller rather than larger animals improvements in foraging efficiency within those prey
(Reid 1994). This suggests that spinner dolphins are areas.
selectively foraging on larger prey, rather than simply The estimates of foraging costs and needs presented
selecting animal sizes at random from the population. here are based on limited information. While absolute
While the sample size for shrimp is small, both this study predictions about spinner dolphin foraging cannot be
and Norris and Dohl (1980) found that shrimp in made, the results point the way for future research
Hawaiian spinner dolphin stomachs were 4.8–5.2 cm in testing both the expected results and the important
total length, somewhat smaller than predicted by the assumptions. The effects of exploring a range of values
estimated foraging costs. Dolar et al. (2003) found in these calculations provide insight into the constraints
similarly small shrimp in the stomach contents of spin- under which spinner dolphins forage. A mammal with
ner dolphins in the eastern tropical Pacific. Perhaps the relatively high energetic needs, foraging on small mobile
slow swimming speed of shrimp compared with other prey individually, but with a limited time to forage, must
groups reduces the costs associated with foraging upon forage efficiently, both in terms of capture rate and
shrimp. Differences between taxonomic groups in the selectivity. The importance of food to spinner dolphins
size distribution of the prey may also be a factor. and the effects of these constraints on the patterns ob-
No matter what size prey spinner dolphins consume served in their nocturnal behavior are apparent. Spinner
from the size distribution available, the relatively small dolphins must be efficient at exploiting their food
size of all available prey probably means that to meet resources to meet their energetic needs.
their energetic needs, spinner dolphins need to forage
quickly. If spinner dolphins use the entire time the Acknowledgements Marlee Breese, Paul Nachtigall, and Robert
mesopelagic layer is estimated to be within their foraging Braun of the Hawaiian Islands Stranding Response Group pro-
range (Fitch and Brownell 1968), they would need to eat vided information on the stranded animal and extracted and saved
its stomach under Dr. Braun’s Letter of Authorization from the
at an average rate of 1.25 10-cm-long prey/min for National Marine Fisheries Service. Iris Fischer, Michiel Schotten,
maintenance. The foraging rates estimated for mainte- Marc Lammers, Whitlow Au, and Ariel Rivera-Vicente assisted in
nance increase dramatically if smaller prey are consumed, the dissection of the stomach. Timothy Tricas generously per-
up to 47 2.5-cm-long prey/min if foraging costs (both mitted me to utilize his laboratory and Ariel Rivera-Vicente made
search and capture) are considered. If spinner dolphins sure I had everything I needed. Gayla Ivey and Ken Longenecker
assisted with the classification of fish otoliths. Whitlow Au, Paul
consistently consume prey with the average lengths ob- Nachtigall, and George Losey provided helpful comments on
served in this and the Norris and Dohl studies (1980), that earlier drafts of this manuscript. Support for this work was pro-
is, just under 8 cm in total length, then a dolphin would vided by a Leonida Family Scholarship, the ARCS Foundation,
need to average 3 to 4 prey/min for the entire 11-h po- and the Hawaii Institute of Marine Biology’s Marine Mammal
Research Program. This work was also funded by a grant from
tential foraging time. While it is unlikely that this rate is the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration, Project
constant over time, particularly since the depth of the R/FM-7, which is sponsored by the University of Hawaii Sea
boundary layer changes by 100 m or more, it does indi- Grant College Program, SOEST, under Institutional Grant No.
cate that spinner dolphins are time limited. This limita- NA16RG2254 from NOAA Office of Sea Grant, Department of
Commerce. The views expressed herein are those of the author
tion lends support to the idea that spinner dolphins need and do not necessarily reflect the views of NOAA or any of its
to follow both inshore/upward and offshore/downward subagencies. UNIHI-SEAGRANT-JC-03–01 This is HIMB
migrations of their prey (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). contribution 1172.
444

Kastelein RA, Elst CA van der, Tennant HK, Wiepkema PR


References (2000) Food consumption and growth of a female dusky dol-
phin (Lagenorhynchus obscurus). Zoo Biol 19:131–142
Acevedo-Gutierrez A, Croll DA, Tershy BR (2002) High feeding Kastelein RA, Au WWL, de Haan D (2002a) Underwater audio-
costs limit dive time in the largest whales. J Exp Biol 205:1747– gram of a striped dolphin (Stenella coeruleoalba) measured with
1753 narrow-band frequency-modulated signals. J Acoust Soc Am
Arnaya IN, Sano N, Iida K (1989a) Studies on acoustic target 112:334–344
strength of squid II. Effect of behaviour on averaged dorsal Kastelein RA, Vaughan N, Walton S, Wiepkema PR (2002b) Food
aspect target strength. Bull Fac Fish Hokkaido Univ 40:83–99 intake and body measurements of Atlantic bottlenose dolphins
Arnaya IN, Sano N, Iida K (1989b) Studies on acoustic target (Tursiops truncatus) in captivity. Mar Environ Res 53:199–218
strength of squid III. Measurement of the mean target strength Kils U (1979) Swimming speed and escape capacity of Antarctic
of small live squid. Bull Fac Fish Hokkaido Univ 40:110 krill, Euphausia superba. Rep Mar Res 27:264–266
Benoit-Bird KJ, Au WWL (2001) Target strength measurements of Kizevetter IV (1973) Chemistry and technology of Pacific fish. The
animals from the Hawaiian mesopelagic boundary community. Israel Program for Scientific Translations (trans). U.S.
J Acoust Soc Am 110:812–819 Department of Commerce, National Technical Information
Benoit-Bird KJ, Au WWL (2002) Energy: converting from acoustic Service, Springfield, Va., ‘IT72–50019 (Original work published
to biological resource units. J Acoust Soc Am 111:2070–2075 1971)
Benoit-Bird KJ, Au WWL (2003a) Prey dynamics affect foraging MacLennan DN, Simmonds EJ (1992) Fisheries acoustics. Chap-
by a pelagic predator (Stenella longirostris) over a range of man and Hall, New York
spatial and temporal scales. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 53:364–373 Martensson PE, Lager Gotaas AR, Nordoy ES, Blix AS (1996)
Benoit-Bird KJ, Au WWL (2003b) Spatial dynamics of a nearshore Seasonal changes in energy density of prey of northeast Atlantic
micronekton sound-scattering layer. ICES J Mar Sci 60:899– seals and whales. Mar Mamm Sci 12:635–640
913 Norris KS, Dohl TP (1980) Behavior of the Hawaiian spinner
Benoit-Bird KJ, Au WWL, Brainard RE, Lammers MO (2001) dolphin, Stenella longirostris. Fish Bull 77:821–849
Diel horizontal migration of the Hawaiian mesopelagic Norris KS, Würsig B, Wells RS, Würsig M (1994) The Hawaiian
boundary community observed acoustically. Mar Ecol Prog Ser spinner dolphin. University of California Press, Berkeley, Calif.
217:1–14 Perrin WF, Warner RR, Fiscus CH, Holts DB (1973) Stomach
Brooks AH (1977) A study of the swimbladders of selected meso- contents of porpoise, Stenella spp., and yellowfin tuna, Thunnus
pelagic fish species. In: Andersen NR, Zahuranec BJ (eds) albacares, in mixed-species aggregations. Fish Bull 71:1077–
Oceanic sound scattering prediction. Plenum Press, New York, 1092
pp 565–590 Perrin WF, Dolar MLL, Robineau D (1999) Spinner dolphins
Bunce A (2001) Prey consumption of Australasian gannets (Morus (Stenella longirostris) of the western Pacific and southeast Asia:
serrator) breeding in Port Phillip Bay, southeast Australia, and Pelagic and shallow-water forms. Mar Mamm Sci 15:1029–1053
potential overlap with commercial fisheries. ICES J Mar Sci Reid SB (1994) Spatial structure of the mesopelagic fish community
58:904–915 in the Hawaiian boundary region. Ph.D. dissertation, Depart-
Burke BJ, Rice JA (2002) A linked foraging and bioenergetics ment of Oceanography, University of Hawaii, Honolulu
model for southern flounder. Trans Am Fish Soc 131:120–131 Reid SB, Hirota J, Young RE, Hallacher LE (1991) Mesopelagic-
Cadenat J, Doutre M (1959) Notes sur les delphinides Ouest-Af- boundary community in Hawaii: micronekton at the interface
ricains, 5. Sur un Prodelphinus a long bec capture au large des between neritic and oceanic ecosystems. Mar Biol 109:427–440
cotes du Senegal: Prodelphinus longirostris. Bull Inst Fondam Sambilay VC Jr (1990) Interrelationships between swimming speed,
Afr Noire 21:787–792 caudal fin aspect ratio and body length of fishes. Fishbyte
Clarke M, Young RE (1998) Description and analysis of cepha- 83:16–20
lopod beaks from stomachs of six species of odontocete ceta- Schotten M, Au WWL, Lammers MO, Aubauer R (2004) Echo-
ceans stranded on Hawaiian shores. J Mar Biol Assoc UK location recordings and localizations of wild spinner dolphins
78:623–641 (Stenella longirostris) and pantropical spotted dolphins (Ste-
Cowles DL (2001) Swimming speed and metabolic rate during nella attenuata) using a four hydrophone array. In: Thomas JA,
routine swimming and simulated diel vertical migration of Moss C, Vater M (eds) Echolocation in bats and dolphins.
Sergestes similis in the laboratory. Pac Sci 55:215–226 University of Chicago Press, Chicago (in press)
Cox M, Gaglione E, Prowten P, Noonan M (1996) Food prefer- Smith RL, Paul JM, Gillispie J (1997) Energy content of Arctic cod
ences communicated via symbol discrimination by a California and saffron cod in the northeastern Chukchi Sea. In: Reynolds
sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Aquat Mamm 22:3–10 JB (ed) Fish ecology in Arctic North America. American
Day DE, Mintz EM, Bartness TJ (2002) Diet choice exaggerates Fisheries Society, Symposium 19. American Fisheries Society,
food hoarding, intake and pup survival across reproduction. Bethesda, Md., pp 319–325
Physiol Behav 75:143–157 Stockwell JD, Johnson BM (1999) Field evaluation of a bioener-
Dolar MLL, Walker WA, Kooyman GL, Perrin WF (2003) getics-based foraging model for kokanee (Oncorhynchus nerka).
Comparative feeding ecology of spinner dolphins (Stenella Can J Fish Aquat Sci 56:140–151
longirostris) and Fraser’s dolphins (Lagenodelphis hosei) in the Urick RJ (1983) Principles of underwater sound. McGraw Hill,
Sulu Sea. Mar Mamm Sci 19:1–19 New York
Dufour DL, Reina JC, Spurr GB (2002) Energy intake and Williams TM, Haun JE, Friedl W (1999) The diving physiology of
expenditure of free-living, lactating Colombian women in an bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) I. Balancing the de-
urban setting. Eur J Clin Nutr 56:205–213 mands of exercise for energy conservation at depth. J Exp Biol
Fitch JE, Brownell RL Jr (1968) Fish otoliths and their importance 202:2739–2748
in interpreting feeding habits. J Fish Res Bd Can 25:2561–2574 Yatsu A, Yamanaka K, Yamashiro C (1999) Tracking experiments
Gorman ML, Mills MG, Raath JP, Speakman JR (1998) High of the jumbo flying squid, Dosidicus gigas, with an ultrasonic
hunting costs make African wild dogs vulnerable to klepto- telemetry system in the eastern Pacific Ocean. Bull Natl Res Inst
parasitism by hyaenas. Nature 391:479–481 Far Seas Fish 36:55–60
Honda S, Wada T, Matsubara T (1992) Relationship between food Yazdi P, Kilian A, Culik BM (1999) Energy expenditure of
consumption and growth of Japanese sardine Sardinops mela- swimming bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Mar Biol
nostictus in the laboratory. Bull Fac Fish Hokkaido Univ 134:601–607
56:1–6

You might also like