You are on page 1of 22

Psychology Science, Volume 46, 2004 (3), p.

302 - 323

Identification of Gifted and Talented Students


KURT A. HELLER
1

Abstract
After a brief introduction with four main questions related to identifying gifted and talented students, this article centres on the following topics: (1) multidimensional conceptions of giftedness as preconditions of suitable identification procedures, (2) functions and benefits vs. dangers of identification measures, (3) methodological problems and (4) practical recommendations for the identification of various groups of gifted and talented students. Key words: Identification, Giftedness, Munich Model of Giftedness

Prof. Dr. Kurt A. Heller, Department of Psychology, University of Munich, Leopoldstr. 13, 80802 Mnchen, Germany; E-mail: heller@edupsy.uni-muenchen.de

Identification basic issues

303

The necessity for the identification (and education) of gifted and talented students is often the subject of controversial discussions. There are several questions which provoke varying opinions from experts and lay persons alike. The following sections deal with four main topics of gifted identification and its related aspects: 1. 2. What is to be identified? Questions concerning the relevant diagnostic variables arise, that are additional to conceptual problems. Why or for what purposes is the identification attempted? In the literature, a substantial differentiation between talent searches (for special gifted programs or educational measures) and single case diagnostics (e.g. in the school counseling context and/or for intervention purposes) has been made. In both cases, benefits and dangers of identification procedures must be weighed. How can gifted and talented students be identified? This question is directed at sources of diagnostic information and measurement instruments as well as at information processing and decision strategies concerning being gifted or not, the fit between individual (learning) needs and social (instructional) conditions of gifted education, etc. Identification and program evaluation aspects also are included in this section. When, or more precisely at what point in time or developmental stage, should gifted children and talented youth be identified? Should single attempts or continuous diagnostic approaches be used? Voluntary or obligatory participation in talent searches (inside vs. outside of school)? These and other questions must be answered, especially with regard to the second question mentioned above.

3.

4.

Finally, practical recommendations for the identification of various groups of gifted and talented students will be discussed. For greater detail see Heller (1987, 1989, 1991/2000), Hany (1993), Mnks & Heller (1994), Feldhusen & Jarwan (2000), Perleth, Schatz & Mnks (2000), Sternberg & Subotnik (2000), Trost (2000), Sternberg & Grigorenko (2002), among others.

Multidimensional Conceptions of Giftedness and Talent as Preconditions of Suitable Identifying Procedures


Our knowledge regarding giftedness and talent both terms are used synonymously according to the recent literature (cf. Heller, Mnks, Sternberg & Subotnik, 2000/2002; Sternberg & Davidson, 2004) is supplied by different research paradigms. Approaches that are particularly relevant to the identification of gifted or highly gifted students are based on the psychometric vs. the expert-novice paradigm. Whereas the psychometric (so-called statusdiagnostic vs. dynamic or process-oriented) models are focused on the individual potential which should be identified for diagnostic or prognostic purposes, the expert-novice paradigm focuses more or less on personality (motivational and learning) and social-cultural conditions in which giftedness (intelligence) plays only a slight role. However, recent attempts have been made to combine both research paradigms in order to optimize the amount of insight into what we call giftedness or talent (cf. Perleth & Ziegler, 1997; Ziegler & Perleth, 1997;

304 K. A. Heller

Figure 1: The Munich Model of Giftedness (MMG) as an example of multidimensional, typological conceptions 2 (according to Heller et al. 1992, 2001)

Legend: Talent factors (predictors) intelligence (language, mathematical, technical abilities, etc.) creativity (language, mathematical, technical, artistic, etc.) social competence musicality artistic abilities psycho-motor skills practical intelligence Environmental conditions (moderators) home environmental stimulation (creative environment) educational style parental educational level demands on performance made at home social reactions to success and failure number of siblings and sibling position family climate quality of instruction school climate critical life events differentiated learning and instruction

Identification basic issues

(Noncognitive) Personality characteristics (moderators) achievement motivation hope for success vs. fear of failure control expectations thirst for knowledge ability to deal well with stress (coping with stress) self-concept (general, scholastic, of talent, etc.)

Performance areas (criteria variables) mathematics, computer science, etc. natural sciences technology, handicraft, trade, etc. languages music (musical-artistic area) social activities, leadership, etc. athletics/sports

305

306

K. A. Heller

Ziegler & Heller, 2000; Perleth, 2001; Heller, Perleth & Lim, 2004). Other synthetic approaches stem from Sternberg (2000, 2003). For practical diagnostic purposes, psychometric (multidimensional) giftedness models still remain to be indispensable (see Lubinski 2004). As an example, the Munich Model of Giftedness (MMG) is presented in Figure 1. Here giftedness is conceptualized as a multifactorized ability construct within a network of non-cognitive (e.g. motivation, interests, selfconcept, control expectations) and social moderators which are related to the giftedness factors (predictors) and the exceptional performance areas (criterion variables). For diagnostic or prognostic purposes, the differentiation between the three kinds of variables predictors, moderators (or catalysts sensu Gagn, 2000), criteria is of particular interest. Furthermore, the MMG represents a typological model of giftedness or talent. In Figure 1, seven types or forms of gifts/talents are listed in the left row. Although these forms of giftedness are the most cited in the current literature, they do not represent all kinds of giftedness or talent. For a meta-theoretical overview see Ziegler & Heller (2000). In the last decade, the MMG has been validated in several national longitudinal and cross-cultural studies (cf. Heller, 2001, 2002; Perleth, 2001; Perleth & Heller, 1994; Heller & Perleth, 2004, 2005; Heller, Perleth & Lim, 2004).

Functions and Benefits vs. Dangers of Identification Measures


The identification depends not only on the theoretical basis of the definition of giftedness/talent, but also on the purpose of this definition. Two main functions of identification could be differentiated: the talent search and the single case analysis in the diagnosis of giftedness. The talent search focuses on the fit between the individual prerequisites for special gifted programs or educational measures and curriculum or instructional demands such as task difficulties and complexity of the learning subjects, available learning time, etc. The talent search is legitimized through the right of every individual to receive optimal nurturance of his/her talent development and the social demand on each individual to make an appropriate contribution to the society. Hence, a comprehensive and differentiated approach is an indispensable component for talent search. Single case analysis in the diagnosis of giftedness, as the basis for (school) counseling and educational treatments, serves the purpose of providing information about prevention or intervention in individual behavior and performance problems, social conflicts, education and social problems in as far as giftedness can directly or indirectly be made responsible for them. Corresponding assumptions are to be confirmed diagnostically or repudiated before the planning and realization of rationally founded educational-psychological decisions, counseling or intervention measures take place. It has been adequately proved that a continual lack of challenge (due to giftedness not having been recognized), pressure to conformity (e.g. based on the fear of negative labeling effects), insecurity of adults in connection with their dealing with exceptionally gifted children and youth, and feelings of threat and envy could lead to behavior problems and conflicts between gifted individuals and their social environment. It is frequently possible that ignorance of gifted individuals is more to blame than evil intentions. If expert estimations are correct that 20-30 % or more of the highly gifted individuals are not recognized as gifted, then it is easy to judge which omissions at least in

Identification basic issues

307

relation to an individually appropriate nurturance of development are caused by doing without identification of giftedness. This fear is especially valid for members of so-called high-risk groups mentioned below. Even when one considers the methods of critical analyses (e.g. Czeschlik & Rost, 1988; Rost, 1993, 2000) in research literature available on psychosocial adaptation problems mentioned, it is impossible to overlook the numerous counseling situations that have to do with the development of giftedness and corresponding socialization problems (cf. Colangelo, 1997; Colangelo & Assouline, 2000; Feger, 1987; Feger & Prado, 1986, 1998; Freeman, 2000; Heller, 2005; Mnks, 1987; Silverman, 1993, 1997; Stapf, 2003; Webb, Meckstroth & Tolan, 2002). With regard to the postulate of equal opportunity and the (justified) demand for individualized education, several problems concerning identification exist. Highly gifted students cannot in contrast to some beliefs always be easily identified. However, the number of unrecognized gifted students cannot be justified in our political or education systems. If one assumes the good will and intentions of all those involved in identifying gifted and talented students, the following circumstances should be considered to make identification difficult: perceptual distortions due to false assumptions and prejudices, observational errors (etc.) or even the lack of knowledge about how giftedness appears and the developmental conditions; recognition of high risk groups, e.g. highly gifted students with behavior problems, gifted handicapped children and adolescents, highly gifted girls (especially in math and natural sciences), gifted underachievers (gifted students with low school performances; see Butler-Por, 1993; Peters, Grager-Loidl & Supplee, 2000; Ziegler, Dresel & Schober, 2000; Ziegler & Stger, 2003), economically disadvantaged and minority gifted learners or gifted immigrant children and youth (see VanTasselBaskas contribution to this special issue); unfavorable family and school socialization settings for the concerned students so that an identification attempt focused on achievement or product criteria has to fail, i.e. that the chances are extremely limited for recognizing special talents in any one area.

This list could go on forever. It is becoming clear that it is more important to realize the weight of these arguments rather than to have a very complete list of these many proven facts. This does not mean that special rights are called for, just equal rights for everyone including the gifted and talented persons. What about the suspected dangers and disadvantages in connection with the identification of giftedness and talent? In this area there are many more uncontrolled results and unconfirmed hypotheses than there are confirmed recognitions. For example, the labeling problem is continually mentioned. Empirical studies (e.g. Robinson, 1986, 1993; Robinson & Clinkenbeard, 1998) have only reported the feared negative effects in a small number of cases which could have to do with identification. Interestingly enough, according to questionnaire-findings, the greatest reservations about identification attempts are not observed on the part of the gifted or their parents, but rather much more from psychologists and counselors and in part of non-gifted siblings. Most of the classmates also reacted as the parents,

308

K. A. Heller

positively to the label gifted. Although the opinions from German and American teachers questioned were different from one another, both tended to be positive. The following dangers are usually associated with labeling problems: social isolation, development of egocentric attitudes and behaviors, endangering or disturbing the personality development and self-concept through extreme achievement pressures or too much responsibility, etc. Certainly, these dangers must be kept in mind during the identification process and if necessary, be accompanied by counseling measures. If one is aware of the consequences of making no identification attempts, then almost everything speaks for the first alternative: for the exertion of identifying the gifted and talented children and youth as completely as possible so that assistance in their education and upbringing is available at the same time (cf. Heller, Reimann & Senfter, 2005). Even if not all gifted students will need such help, it is nevertheless irresponsible to leave the estimated half of the especially gifted including their parents and teachers alone with their personal and social problems. The range of counseling and problem cases reaches from asynchrony between acceleration of the intelligence development and immature (appropriate to the chronological age) feelings, behavior problems due to permanent lack of challenge, social isolation because of lacking contact possibilities with gifted peers and problems specific to gifted girls (especially in math and the sciences) due to role expectations and educational attitudes, indifferent or even rejecting behaviors by parents or siblings and teachers up to psychiatric risks, e.g. anorexia nervosa (cf. Baldwin, Vialle & Clarke, 2000; Detzner & Schmidt, 1986; Heller & Ziegler, 1996; Kaufmann & Castellanos, 2000; Terrassier, 1985; Yewchuk & Lupart, 2000). A directed talent nurturance is in many cases hardly possible without identification, which provides definite fundamental diagnostic information about the concerned problem. All in all, the weightier arguments are clearly on the side of diagnosing/identifying giftedness and talent during childhood and adolescence. However, prophylactic measures for the prevention of undesirable or even harmful side-effects should be considered as well as different cognitive and social/emotional needs of the gifted students (cf. Adams-Byers, Whitsell & Moon, 2004). See also Schofield & Hotulainen in this special issue.

Methodological Problems of Identification


Questions of methodology concerning identification include various aspects, such as problems with the definition of relevant indicators, sources of diagnostic information or measurement problems. Furthermore, diagnostic decision strategies with respect to specific sources of error, effectivity and economy of the selection of talented students for appropriate gifted programs, the decision to use so-called static diagnostic approaches vs. dynamic or process diagnostic approaches, etc. are also included. According to the currently more favored multidimensional concepts of giftedness and talent, the following behavioral characteristics are considered to be indicators of a special talent in childhood and adolescence: cognitive aptitudes like intellectual precocity, quick comprehension and high-speed of learning, being quick to pick up concepts, often ahead of the usual time (needed by the age-mates), distinct curiosity, a large vocabulary for ones age, creative (original) ideas and methods to solve complex problems, the individual challenging tasks or questions, eminent cognitive abilities to think convergently (as indicators of intelligence) and

Identification basic issues

309

divergently (as indicators of creativity sensu Guilford), sensitivity for problems, spontaneous inclination toward challenging and difficult tasks and thought problems, distinctive metacognitive competencies, etc. With respect to the MMG (see Figure 1 above), these cognitive variables serve as predictors. In the procedure of assessing the influence of non-cognitive personality factors, as well as socio-cultural conditions of the individuals environment, the following items should be considered as moderators: intrinsic achievement motivation and striving for success, willingness to take risks or persistence and striving for perfection, preference for independent learning style, coping with stress, test anxiety or control expectations, along with socio-emotional climate in the family and at school, educating and instructional styles, reactions of peers, siblings, parents and teachers to success and failure of gifted students, quality of stimulation and expectation pressure of the social environment, etc. In addition, according to the type of giftedness, performance variables related to more or less specific areas like mathematics, natural sciences, technology or computer science, languages, music, arts, etc. are named criteria in the diagnosis-prognosis paradigm. Sources of diagnostic information include life, questionnaire, and test data (according to Cattell, 1965). Hence, diagnostic variables and their operationalization, i.e. the measurement instruments, behavioral observations and observational techniques respectively (l-data), diagnostic interviews and questionnaires including self-, parent- and teacher-nominations or checklists (q-data) as well as standardized tests (t-data). If necessary, the named instruments can be supplemented by biographical analyses or something similar. For the identification of highly gifted youth within the talent search and single case diagnosis, one usually needs not only an adequately differentiated set of appropriate instruments, but also has to consider the scale niveau and the test quality characteristics, such as objectivity, reliability, and validity. Ceiling effects have to be dealt with when one employs normed tests. This means that the concerned test does not differentiate adequately in the extreme upper region of the scale. Therefore, special test scales for the identification of highly gifted especially the ablest students are needed; see, for example, the Triarchic Abilities Test (Level H) by Sternberg (1993) or the Munich High Ability Test Battery (MHBT) by Heller & Perleth (2005). A further methodological problem is the breadth-fidelity dilemma encountered in personnel decisions (cf. Cronbach & Gleser, 1965). To cope with this dilemma, the identification procedure involves several steps: First, a general screening takes place. This means that a less exact, but wider, range of factors and instruments is included, e.g. checklists often in combination with rating scales. In the following Tables 1 and 2, examples of such checklists with respect to intellectual giftedness and creative talent from the MHBT are represented. For further checklists, assessing such aspects as social competence, musicality, and psycho-motor skills, please refer to the MHBT by Heller & Perleth (2005). With their aid, teachers or educators nominate a certain number of children or youth who fit the listed talent characteristics. In the next step, more precise tests are employed for the determination of the individual talent dimensions. Finally, individual and social moderator variables are collected which are relevant to the training gifted program or special educational measures; see the sequential strategy model in Figure 2 below. The final selection is thus more accurate than the screening which helps to reduce the danger of not recognizing talents. Such selection decisions generally include risks. The risk of type I or alpha error consists of a person being identified as (highly) gifted when he or she is, in fact, not (highly) gifted.

310

K. A. Heller

Table 1: Teachers Checklist: Intellectual Giftedness ID or Name 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30

Ratings 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3

To assess this type of giftedness, you may refer to the following dimensions (they need not be all present; it is sufficient if the student excels in some of them): Logical / analytical thinking Abstract thinking Mathematical thinking Scientific / technical thinking Language skills (rich vocabulary, fluency of expression, talent for foreign languages) Learning ability (quick understanding, retentive memory, accurate reproduction, active learning) Powers of deduction, combination etc. Broad knowledge Consolidated special knowledge in one or more domains.

Please consider now which of the students in your class apply here, and cross the appropriate code next to their class numbers (ID)!

Scoring system: 1 = top 10 % 2 = top 20 % 3 = below top 20 %

Table 2: Teachers Checklist: Creative Giftedness

Identification basic issues

311

ID or Name 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30

Ratings 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3

To assess this type of giftedness, you may refer to the following dimensions (they need not be all present; it is sufficient if the student excels in some of them): Curiosity, quest for knowledge Imagination, ability to think in alternatives Creative and inventive thinking Originality, search for extraordinary problem/task solutions Flexible thinking, spiritual agility, ability to consider a problem from various points of view Self-sufficiency, independence of thinking and opinion Interest-oriented, independent solving of problems Multiplicity of interests Stability of interests

Please consider now which of the students in your class apply here, and cross the appropriate code next to their class numbers (ID)!

Scoring system: 1 = top 10 % 2 = top 20 % 3 = below top 20 %

312

K. A. Heller

Figure 2: A sequential strategy model for the identification of exceptionally gifted students at school level according to Heller (2000, p. 252) Legend: (1) = Nomination of the 10-20 % class leaders with respect to the different dimensions of giftedness, e.g. through teacher checklists; see Table 1 and/or Table 2 above. (2) = (Field-specific) tests of giftedness for the selected 10 % or 20 % and/or differentiated teacher ratings (cf. checklists). (3) = (If necessary), selection interviews for further selection. (4) = Assignment to various nurturing programs, e.g. curriculum compacting, pull-outprogram, enrichment courses, special classes or special schools for highly gifted and talented students in math and/or sciences, etc.

The risk of type II or beta error is manifested when a person who is (highly) gifted is not identified as such. The first type of error can be reduced by increasing, the second by decreasing values, e.g. IQ- or T-cut-offs (threshold model). Unfortunately, it is not possible to reduce the risk of both types of errors at the same time. Institutional (e.g. capacity of the gifted program) interests usually dictate that the reduction of the alpha errors risk takes place. However, it is in the individuals best interests to minimize the risk of the beta error. Hence, this strategy ought to be favored in the identification (talent search), whereby one can employ valid instruments and the described successive decision procedure to further reduce beta errors. Regarding this topic further, see the articles by Ziegler & Stger and Pyryt in this special issue. The quality of the selection strategy mentioned above is related to its effectivity and economy or efficiency. The effectivity can be defined here as percentage of the (truly) gifted

Identification basic issues

313

already determined in the screening phase (exhausting quota according to Pegnato & Birch, 1959; see Pyryt in this special issue). It is the more important of the mentioned criteria in identifying as many of the gifted as possible. The efficiency (economy) is determined by the percentage of the actual gifted in the screening quota. This criterion is thus a measure for the effort necessary for the total identification process. When trying to find all (highly) gifted persons, it is best to set one's priorities on the first criterion (effectivity). Special questions arising in the identification of gifted children and adolescents from socalled high-risk groups as well as further methodological problems are discussed by Feger (1987), Hany (1987, 1993, 2001), Feldhusen & Jarwan (2000), Kanevsky (2000), Perleth et al. (2000), Sternberg & Subotnik (2000), Perleth (2001a/b), Heller (2002, 2005). While traditional psychometric (trait-oriented or so-called status test) diagnostics are indispensable in the identification of worthy or needy gifted children and youth, in the processoriented or dynamic testing approaches one hopes for important discoveries about the type of learning and thought processes used by (highly) gifted students. Corresponding models primarily aim at proving surmised qualitative differences between gifted and non-gifted groups, especially with regard to information processing during the solving of difficult, complex problems. In contrast to the restrictive problem-solving situation of many skill-based tests (in the psychometric paradigm) which is seen as disadvantageous, open and less structured tasks are attempted. Such tasks, especially when they are reproduced in the experimental design, should allow not only for product analyses (as is customary in the psychometric tradition), but should also make process and causal analyses possible. Undoubtedly, this is a desiderata regarding for example the measurement of creative production (see Urbans article in this special issue). Beyond this, one hopes for insights into those learning and thought processes that are responsible for the development of expertise, beneficial vs. inhibiting conditions in the development and socialization of gifted and talented students, and also information about provisions necessary for the furtherance of development of its efficiency (cf. Facaoaru & Bittner, 1987; Klix, 1983; Waldmann & Weinert, 1990; Weinert & Waldmann, 1985). However, one should not overlook a limitation of this new test diagnostic procedure: its validation, which is not yet sufficient in most cases. It is possible to research without empirical proof of validity, though not in the practice of gifted identification. This is the most important methodological postulate (Jger, 1987). When should (highly) gifted children be identified? This question includes two aspects which are expressed in the following alternatives: 1) Identification at the preschool or elementary school age, i.e. identification as early as possible? 2) Sporadic or continual identification? Another question closely tied to these is 3) whether the identification should proceed in a voluntary or obligatory fashion (through the education system). Considerations which are directed at optimizing individual socialization and education processes speak for the earliest possible identification (Lewis & Louis, 1991; Urban, 1992; Robinson, 1993; Feger & Prado, 1998; Stapf, 2003). This affects not only cognitive, but also motivational and social-emotional areas of the personality development. Correspondingly, Lehwald (1986), who emphasizes early identification, stated the following: The most important thing here is a certain decision, so that the childs optimal nurturance and development can take place. The nurturance of giftedness in a larger sense without adequate diagnostic information about the status and prognosis of personality development is not possible. Process-diagnostically determined biographical data opens the way for determining the individual developmental course of gifted children and deciding upon concrete educational measures for

314

K. A. Heller

the individual case. Early prognoses aid not only the child psychologist but are also indispensible to the early childhood educator (1986, p. 161) translated by the author. In recent years, neuro-biologists also plead for early identification (e.g. Singer, 1999). Other educational and developmental psychologists (e.g. Weinert, 1992) are more skeptical and stress that all preschool children and students at primary (and secondary) school level must be optimally nurtured, i.e. provided with a rich, stimulating learning environment in the family and school settings, also of course the (highly) gifted. In my opinion, this postulate is true, but does it exclude the arguments for early identification? For an overview see Perleth et al. (2000). In the discussion about early identification, methodological problems must also be considered. Difficulties in early identification stem from the lack of reliable and practical (for parents as well as teachers) criteria for the identification of highly gifted children. Also, inadequate knowledge about the problem-solving processes and their development, as they are specific to the gifted, is often an obstacle. The improvement of this background knowledge is an essential prerequisite for the construction of better diagnostic instruments, or more complete intelligence diagnosis on the experimental design (cf. Klix, 1983). The process diagnostic approaches discussed above are attempts in this direction, as are sequential decision strategies as well. Above and beyond this, such diagnostic instruments need to measure not only interindividual differences, but also intraindividual progress regarding formal problem-solving and learning competencies. Objections which are directed at the limited reliability of intelligence tests in preschool or early school groups are less sound. According to Casey & Quisenberry (1982) also see Robinson & Robinson (1992), Perleth et al. (2000) or Stapf (2003) the results in the upper areas of intelligence tests are already relatively reliable in preschool ages so that an early identification of giftedness could provide important information for individual nurturance of the gifted (if ceiling effects can be avoided). More serious are the reservations of many educators and preschool pedagogues about the realization chances of early diagnostic measures. Sometimes, however, the unprejudiced observers suspicions that are raised, those ideological motives and not factually grounded arguments, are guiding the discussion. How else can one explain that parents of gifted children often present quite different problems, e.g. danger of lack of challenge, helplessness, regarding possibilities for appropriate stimulation and challenge? Yet this points out even more that for directed aid or prophylactic measures the available knowledge about positive and negative developmental conditions of gifted children and adolescents is often inadequate. Ideological mind-sets will hardly change this situation, to say nothing of dealing with the helplessness. The most necessary thing at this point is developmental and educational-psychological research, which would also benefit the gifted identification possibilities. There is certainly no danger in the foreseeable future of making a final selection at the preschool or elementary school age. The question of when identification attempts should begin loses its force when one decides on continual diagnosis/prognosis which accompanies personality development. This would be better for the diagnosis of chronological developmental advances and for identifying individual uniqueness. At the same time, this makes the nurturance of giftedness and talent or more inclusively personality in the sense of formative evaluation possible. One important educational goal in the nurturance of giftedness is the development of a realistic self-concept. In this way, misdiagnoses of giftedness can be recognized early so that a continuous adjustment to the individuals needs of practical support measures is possible. It can

Identification basic issues

315

be a disadvantage to have to go to so much effort; this must be kept in reasonable relation to the gain in information for the nurturance of the gifted children. Finally, each individuals right to free choice, including that of the gifted person, must be respected. The principle of voluntary participation, i.e. the individual decision about taking advantage of identification measures available to the gifted, cannot be allowed to deteriorate into an individual incapacitation. There seems to be, at least in Western democracies, general consensus on this point. A limitation of this right can only be derived from the educational responsibility for individually optimal socialization chances true for all young people. In this light, the question of the individual's right to free choice is not a problem specific to the gifted children and youth.

Summarizing Conclusions
There are a number of problems intertwined with the identification of giftedness and talent, e.g. questions concerning the conceptualization of high ability or giftedness constructs, methodological problems of identification like measurement and procedural questions, decision paradigms, validation and last but not least evaluation problems. In order to solve them, not only is differential and diagnostic psychology called upon, but one also expects important contributions from the fields of developmental and educational psychology, social and clinical psychology, and (empirical) education sciences. Without claiming to be a complete list, the necessity of an interdisciplinary cooperative problem-oriented effort should be emphasized. From this we can expect decisive progress to be made in the near future. The following research tasks seem to be the most pressing: 1. The elaboration and increased precision of differential diagnostic instruments for determining various forms of giftedness and talent. This should include both psychometric (skill-/trait-based) and cognitive psychological approaches (from experimental diagnostics). The argument of whether the support of giftedness should be more related to general cognitive competencies and general thought processes or to specific skills and abilities (knowledge competencies) naturally influences the operationalism of the giftedness/talent constructs. If one concurs with the investment theory by Cattell (1971), according to which Cattells (1963) crystallized intelligence (in this instance, knowledge acquisition) benefits from the Cattells fluid intelligence (in this instance, general thought potential) or is partially dependent on it, the solution of this problem is only to be found in the inclusion of both approaches; for greater detail, see Heller & Hany (1996). With regard to optimal identification results, one should give preference to process diagnostic over so-called status diagnostic methods. Naturally, one is confronted with the previously discussed unsolved problem of actually obtaining processes analyses and not only product analysis results. The process-oriented giftedness diagnosis is in an analogous and just as often dissatisfying situation as learning or dynamic test diagnostics (cf. Guthke, 1992; Kanevsky, 2000; Sternberg & Grigorenko, 2002). Developmental diagnostics are no less deficient (cf. Stapf, 2003). Since gifted individuals exhibit various characteristics both in the developmental process and in the achievement behavior, these must all be considered in the identifi-

2.

316

K. A. Heller

3.

cation of gifted children and youth. Multivariate classificatory approaches to the determination of specific configurations of giftedness and talent (using test profiles, etc.) are supplemented by idiographic methods, e.g. biographical analyses (cf. Bloom, 1985). A systematic determination of counseling needs specific to the gifted as a basis for prevention and intervention is just desirable as the development and testing of appropriate continuing education measures for counselors and schoolpsychologists (for greater detail see Heller, 2005). The greatest challenge probably lies in the establishment of interaction diagnostics and its validation (cf. Mnks, 1992). In connection with this, additional evaluation problems and indirectly conceptualizing problems are virulent. It would appear that the topic of giftedness or talent is being rediscovered as a research task (cf. Ziegler & Heller, 2000; Sternberg, 2000, 2003).

Practical Recommendations
Despite conceptual and methodological problems that are as of yet unsolved, a few recommendations for the better practice of identification may be formulated here. Not only is the utility aspect to be included in the following discussion, but also the possible disadvantages and consequences of doing without diagnostic aids. 1. In single-case diagnoses as well as talent searches, a step-by-step procedure is most effective. This best meets (highly) gifted individual needs. A sequential decision strategy reduces the danger of incorrect identification outcomes by minimizing the bandwidth fidelity dilemma. Since the most recent theories of giftedness and talent have almost always evolved from complex or hierarchical constructs, multidimensional measurement methods and classificatory approaches (to data analysis) are to be recommended over traditional one-dimensional (IQ cut-off scores) methods. For that purpose, elaborated test profiles of (various groups) gifted and talented students are needed (as provided e.g. by the MHBT by Heller & Perleth, 2005). Another example is the Revolving Door Identification Model (RDIM) by Renzulli (1984). To develop a talent pool, Renzulli offers a six step model for identifying and nurturing gifted students. The gifted students can then be supported individually, based on needs and wants, in voluntary work or study groups (also see MacRae & Lupart, 1991). In a similar way, Ziegler & Stger developed the five step model ENTER for the identification, which is presented below in this special issue. Besides these status-diagnostic (psychometric) approaches following the learn-test or dynamic test paradigm, one proceeds later in a more process-diagnostic manner. Analogously, one would attempt a step-by-step confirmation of the identification results in the singlecase evaluation, whereby the uniqueness of the individual must be the center of the identification process. Therefore, detailed biographical analysis should always be included if possible. Despite their measurement inadequacies, one would not want to do without informal diagnostic instruments such as parent and teacher nominations or checklists (see Nebers article in this special issue), observational techniques or diagnostic interviews. In individual cases, self-nominations and contests can play a further part

Identification basic issues

317

2.

3.

with older adolescents, although the self-selection limits many diagnostic procedures (see Gagn, 1989; Gagn, Bgin & Talbot, 1993). Combined with formal methods, e.g. cognitive ability tests, they can provide important supplementary diagnostic information (cf. Campbell, Wagner & Walberg, 2000). One must be careful during the selection of ability and generally of achievement normed tests that they do not produce ceiling effects, i.e. failing to sufficiently differentiate adequately in the upper ranges of the concerned scales. Multifactorized tests are usually more appropriate for the identification of (highly) gifted and talented students than (normed) tests of general intelligence, etc. If one contrasts the advantages of gifted identification with the possible disadvantages, then the advantages clearly outweigh the disadvantages. Neither the feared labeling effects (Perleth et al., 2000; Endepohls-Ulpe, 2004; Robinson, 1986, 1993) nor unusual personality or social conflicts from gifted identification measures have been proven. The fact that such undesireable effects can occasionally occur should lead to their being dealt with by accompanying counseling measures, and not by doing without (useful) diagnostic information (also see Lehwald, 1986, 1987). One also finds the results of identification omissions on the education and upbringing or personality development of the gifted children and youth in the literature, which are felt to be much more serious. Gifted identification is frequently an essential element of individual development chances. Feger (1987), Mnks (1987, 1992), Borland & Wright (2000) or Stapf (2003) suspect that many gifted children are presently not being recognized. Primarily these are the so-called high-risk groups mentioned above; for greater detail see Van Tassel-Baska et al. in this special issue. Thus, in many instances, a continual identification or diagnostic process which begins at an early age is essential as a prophylactic measure. The success of such identification attempts depends on general conditions and educational provisions. The preparedness of parents and teachers, school counselors and psychologists, to deal with the tasks of identifying and nurturing the gifted without fear or prejudice, is a main concern. This challenge can be everything except easy. The identification or diagnosis of giftedness and talent fulfills not only an important function with regard to an optimal personality development of gifted and talented youth in the social (family and school) settings. It also serves as a prevention and intervention measure in crisis situations.

References
1. Adams-Byers, J., Whitsell, A.S. & Moon, S.M. (2004). Gifted Students Perceptions of the Academic and Social/Emotional Effects of Homogeneous and Heterogeneous Grouping. Gifted Child Quarterly, 48, 7-29. Baldwin, A.Y., Vialle, W. & Clarke, C. (2000). Global Professionalism and Perceptions of Teachers of the Gifted. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 565-572). Oxford: Pergamon. Bloom, B.S. (Ed.). (1985). Developing Talent in Young People. New York: Ballantine Books.

2.

3.

318

K. A. Heller

4.

5.

6.

7.

8. 9. 10. 11.

12.

13. 14.

15.

16.

17.

18.

19. 20.

Borland, J.H. & Wright, L. (2000). Identifying and Educating Poor and Under-Represented Gifted Students. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 587-594). Oxford: Pergamon. Butler-Por, N. (1993). Underachieving Gifted Students. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks & A.H. Passow (Eds.), International Handbook of Research and Development of Giftedness and Talent (pp. 649-668). Oxford: Pergamon. Campbell, J.R., Wagner, H. & Walberg, H.J. (2000). Academic Competitions and Programs Designed to Challenge the Exceptionally Talented. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 523-535). Oxford: Pergamon. Casey, J.P. & Quisenberry, N.L. (1982). Hochbegabung in der frhen Kindheit Ein Forschungsberblick [Giftedness in Early Childhood A Review] (pp. 73-91). Heidelberg: Schindele. Cattell, R.B. (1965). The Scientific Analysis of Personality. Chicago: Penguin. Cattell, R.B. (1963). Theory of Fluid and Crystallized Intelligence: A Critical Experiment. Educational Psychology, 54, 1-22. Cattell, R.B. (1971). Abilities: Their Structure, Growth, and Action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Colangelo, N. (1997). Counseling Gifted Students: Issues and Practices. In N. Colangelo & G.A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of Gifted Education (2nd ed., pp. 353-365). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Colangelo, N. & Assouline, S.G. (2000). Counseling Gifted Students. In K. A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 595-607). Oxford: Pergamon. Czeschlik, T. & Rost, D.H. (1988). Hochbegabte und ihre Peers [The Gifted and Their Peers). Zeitschrift fr Pdagogische Psychologie, 2, 1-23. Detzner, M. & Schmidt, M.H. (1986). Are Highly Gifted Children and Adolescents Especially Susceptible to Anorexia Nervosa? In K.A. Heller & J.F. Feldhusen (Eds.), Identifying and Nurturing the Gifted. An International Perspective (pp. 149-162). Toronto: Huber Publ. Endepohls-Ulpe, M. (2004). Wie stellen Grundschullehrkrfte sich hochbegabte Schler/ innen vor? Der Einfluss persnlicher Erfahrung in der Unterrichtung Hochbegabter [Primary School Teachers Images of a Gifted Pupil Effects of Personal Experience in Teaching Gifted Children]. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 51, 126-135. Facaoaru, C. & Bittner, R. (1987). Kognitionspsychologische Anstze der Hochbegabungsdiagnostik [Cognitive Psychological Approaches to the Diagnosis of Giftedness]. Zeitschrift fr Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 8, 193-205. Feger, B. (1987). Spezialprobleme bei der Identifikation Hochbegabter aus Risikogruppen [Special Problems in the Identification of Gifted from High-Risk Groups]. Zeitschrift fr Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 8, 227-233. Feger, B. & Prado, T. (1986). The First Information and Counseling Center for the Gifted in West Germany. In K.A. Heller & J.F. Feldhusen (Eds.), Identifying and Nurturing the Gifted. An International Perspective (pp. 139-148). Toronto: Huber Publ. Feger, B. & Prado, T.M. (1998). Hochbegabung [High Ability]. Darmstadt: Primus. Feldhusen, J.F. & Jarwan, F.A. (2000). Identification of Gifted and Talented Youth for Educational Programs. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.),

Identification basic issues

319

21.

22. 23.

24. 25.

26.

27.

28.

29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35.

36.

37.

International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 271-282). Oxford: Pergamon. Freeman, J. (2000). Families: The Essential Context for Gifts and Talents. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 573-585). Oxford: Pergamon. Gagn, F. (1989). Peer Nominations as a Psychometric Instrument: Many Questions Asked But Few Answered. Gifted Child Quarterly, 33, 53-58. Gagn, F. (2000). Understanding the Complex Choreography of Talent Development Through DMGT-Based Analysis. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 67-79). Oxford: Pergamon. Gagn, F., Bgin, J. & Talbot, L. (1993). How Well Do Peers Agree Among Themselves When Nominating the Gifted or Talented? Gifted Child Quarterly, 37, 39-45. Guthke, J. (1992). Lerntests auch fr Hochbegabte? [Learning Tests also for Highly Gifted?] In E.A. Hany & H. Nickel (Hrsg.), Begabung und Hochbegabung [Giftedness and High Ability] (pp. 125-141). Bern: Huber. Hany, E.A. (1987). Psychometrische Probleme bei der Identifikation Hochbegabter [Psychometric Problems in the Identification of the Gifted]. Zeitschrift fr Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 8, 173-191. Hany, E.A. (1993). Methodological Problems and Issues Concerning Identification. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks & A.H. Passow (Eds.), International Handbook of Research and Development of Giftedness and Talent (pp. 209-232). Oxford: Pergamon. Hany, E.A. (2001). Identifikation von Hochbegabten im Schulalter [Identification of Highly Gifted in School-Age]. In K.A. Heller (Hrsg.), Hochbegabung im Kindes- und Jugendalter [High Ability in Childhood and Youth] (2nd ed., pp. 41-169). Gttingen: Hogrefe. Heller, K.A. (Ed.). (1987). Hochbegabungsdiagnostik [Identification of the Gifted]. Bern: Huber. Heller, K.A. (1989). Perspectives on the Diagnosis of Giftedness. German Journal of Psychology, 13, 140-159. 2 Heller, K.A. (Ed.). (1991, 2000). Begabungsdiagnostik in der Schul- und Erziehungsberatung [Identification of Gifted Students]. Bern: Huber. Heller, K.A. (Ed.). (2001). Hochbegabung im Kindes- und Jugendalter [High Ability in Childhood and Youth] (2nd ed.). Gttingen: Hogrefe. Heller, K.A. (Ed.). (2002). Begabtenfrderung im Gymnasium [Gifted Education in the German Gymnasium]. Opladen: Leske + Budrich. Heller, K.A. (2005). Education and Counseling of the Gifted and Talented in Germany. International Journal for the Advancement of Counselling, 27, in press. Heller, K.A. & Hany, E.A. (1996). Psychologische Modelle der Hochbegabtenfrderung [Psychological Models of Gifted Education]. In F.E. Weinert (Ed.), Psychologie des Lernens und der Instruktion, Bd. 2 der Pdagogischen Psychologie (Enzyklopdie der Psychologie) (pp. 477-513). Gttingen: Hogrefe. Heller, K.A. & Ziegler, A. (1996). Gender Differences in Mathematics and the Sciences: Can Attributional Retraining Improve the Performance of Gifted Females? Gifted Child Quarterly, 40, 200-210. Heller, K.A. & Perleth, Ch. (2004). Adapting Conceptual Models for Cross-Cultural Applications. In J.R. Campbell, K. Tirri, P. Ruohotie & H. Walberg (Eds.), Cross-Cultural Research: Basic Issues, Dilemmas, and Strategies (pp. 81-101). Tampere, FL: Hame Polytechnic (University of Tampere, Finland).

320

K. A. Heller

38. Heller, K.A. & Perleth, Ch. (2005). Mnchner Hochbegabungs-Testbatterie (MHBT) [Munich High Ability Test Battery]. Gttingen: Hogrefe (Belz Test). 39. Heller, K.A., Perleth, Ch. & Lim, T.K. (2005). The Munich Model of Giftedness Designed to Identify and Promote Gifted Students. In R.J. Sternberg & J.E. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of Giftedness (2nd ed., pp. 172-197). New York: Cambridge University Press. 40. Heller, K.A., Reimann, R. & Senfter, A. (2005). Hochbegabung im Grundschulalter: Erkennen und Frdern [High ability in the Elementary Age]. Mnster : LIT. 41. Heller, K.A., Mnks, F.J., Sternberg, R.J. & Subotnik, R.F. (Eds.). (2000). International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed.; revised 2nd ed. 2002). Oxford: Pergamon. 42. Jger, A.O. (1986). Validitt von Intelligenztests [Validity of Intelligence Tests]. Diagnostica, 32, 272-289. 43. Kanevsky, L. (2000). Dynamic Assessment of Gifted Students. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 283-295). Oxford: Pergamon. 44. Kaufmann, F.A. & Castellanos, F.X. (2000). Attention Deficit /Hyperactivity Disorder in Gifted Students. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 621-632). Oxford: Pergamon. 45. Klix, F. (1983). Begabungsforschung ein neuer Weg in der kognitiven Intelligenzdiagnostik? [Ability Research A New Method of Cognitive Intelligence Measurement?] Zeitschrift fr Psychologie, 191, 360-386. 46. Lehwald, G. (1986). Frhdiagnostik als Voraussetzung fr eine entwicklungsgerechte Frderung begabter Kinder [Early Identification as a Prerequisite for Developmentally Appropriate Enrichment for Gifted Children]. In U. Schaarschmidt, M. Berg & K.D. Hnsgen (Eds.), Diagnostik geistiger Leistungen (pp. 160-167). Berlin: Volk und Wissen. 47. Lehwald, G. (1987). Theoretisch-methodologische Positionen zur Diagnostik von Begabungen im Kleinkind- und Vorschulalter [Theoretical-Methodological Standpoints on the Diagnosis of Giftedness in Toddlers and Pre-School Children]. In U. Schaarschmidt, M. Berg & K.D. Hnsgen (Eds.), Neue Trends in der Psychodiagnostik. Berlin: Volk und Wissen. 48. Lewis, M. & Louis, B. (1991). Young Gifted Children. In N. Colangelo & G.A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of Gifted Education (pp. 365-381). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. 49. Lubinski, D. (2004) Introduction to the Special Section on Cognitive Abilities: 100 Years After Spearmanss (1904) Genenral Intelligence, Objecitively Determined and Measured. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 112-129. 50. MacRae, L.D. & Lupart, J.L. (1991). Issues in Identifying Gifted Students: How Renzullis Model Stacks Up. Roeper Review, 14, 53-58. 51. Mnks, F.J. (1987). Einzelfallanalyse in der Hochbegabungsdiagnostik [Single Case Analysis in the Diagnosis of Giftedness]. Zeitschrift fr Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 8, 207-216. 52. Mnks, F.J. (1992). Ein interaktionales Modell der Hochbegabung [An Interaction Model of High Ability]. In E.A. Hany & H. Nickel (Eds.), Begabung und Hochbegabung (pp. 1722). Bern: Huber. 53. Mnks, F.J. & Heller, K.A. (1994). Identification and Programming of the Gifted and Talented. In T. Husn & T.N. Postlethwaite (Eds.), The International Encyclopedia of Education, Vol. 5 (2nd ed., pp. 2725-2732). Oxford: Pergamon. 54. Pegnato, C.W. & Birch, J.W. (1959). Locating Gifted Children in Junior High Schools A Comparison of Methods. Exceptional Children, 25, 300- 304.

Identification basic issues

321

55. Perleth, Ch. (2001a). Follow-up-Untersuchungen zur Mnchner Hochbegabungsstudie [Follow-Ups of the Munich Longitudinal Study of Giftedness]. In K.A. Heller (Ed.), Hochbegabung im Kindes- und Jugendalter (2nd ed., pp. 357-446). Gttingen: Hogrefe. 56. Perleth, Ch. (2001b). Zur Methodik der Mnchner Hochbegabungsstudie [Method of the Munich Giftedness Study]. In K.A. Heller (Ed.), Hochbegabung im Kindes- und Jugendalter (2nd ed., pp. 447-477). Gttingen: Hogrefe. 57. Perleth, Ch. & Heller, K.A. (1994). The Munich Longitudinal Study of Giftedness. In R.F. Subotnik & K.D. Arnold (Eds.), Beyond Terman: Contemporary Longitudinal Studies of Giftedness and Talent (pp. 77-114). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 58. Perleth, Ch. & Ziegler, A. (1997). berlegungen zur Begabungsdiagnose und Begabtenfrderung in der Berufsaus- und -weiterbildung [Considerations about Identification in the Occupational Education System. In U. Kittler & H. Metz-Gckel (Eds.), Pdagogische Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht (pp. 11-112). Essen: Blaue Eule. 59. Perleth, Ch., Schatz, T. & Mnks, F.J. (2000). Early Identification of High Ability. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 297-316). Oxford: Pergamon. 60. Peters, W.A.M., Grager-Loidl, H. & Supplee, P. (2000). Underachievement in Gifted Children and Adolescents: Theory and Practice. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 609620). Oxford: Pergamon. 61. Robinson, A. (1986). The Identification and Labeling of Gifted Children. What Does Research Tell Us? In K.A. Heller & J.F. Feldhusen (Eds.), Identifying and Nurturing the Gifted. An International Perspective (pp. 103-109). Toronto: Huber Publ. 62. Robinson, A. & Clinkenbeard, P.R. (1998). Giftedness: An exceptionality examined. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 117-139. 63. Robinson, N.M. (1993). Identifying and Nurturing Gifted, Very Young Children. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks & A.H. Passow (Eds.), International Handbook of Research and Development of Giftedness and Talent (pp. 507-524). Oxford: Pergamon. 64. Robinson, N.M. & Robinson, H. (1992). The Use of Standardized Tests with Young Gifted Children. In P.S. Klein & A.J. Tannenbaum (Eds.), To Be Young and Gifted (pp. 141-170). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 65. Rost, D.H. (Hrsg.). (1993). Lebensumweltanalyse hochbegabter Kinder: das Marburger Hochbegabtenprojekt [Analysis of the Environmental Conditions of Gifted Children: the Marburg Project]. Gttingen: Hogrefe. 66. Rost, D.H. (Ed.). (2000). Hochbegabte und hochleistende Jugendliche [Gifted Achievers in Youth]. Mnster: Waxmann. 67. Silverman, L.K. (1993). Counseling Needs and Programs for the Gifted. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks & A.H. Passow (Eds.), International Handbook of Research and Development of Giftedness and Talent (pp. 631-647). Oxford: Pergamon. 68. Silverman, L.K. (1997). Family Counseling with the Gifted. In N. Colangelo & G.A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of Gifted Education (2nd ed., pp. 382-397). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. 69. Singer, W. (1999). In der Bildung gilt: Je frher, desto besser [In Education is valid: The Sooner The Better]. Psychologie Heute, Dec. 1999, 60-65. 70. Stapf, A. (2003). Hochbegabte Kinder [Highly Gifted Children]. Mnchen: C.H. Beck. 71. Sternberg, R.J. (1993). Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test (Level H). Unpublished Test. 72. Sternberg, R.J. (2000). Giftedness as Developing Expertise. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 55-66). Oxford: Pergamon.

322

K. A. Heller

73. Sternberg, R.J. (2003). WICS as a Model of Giftedness. High Ability Studies, 14, 109-137. 74. Sternberg, R.J. & Subotnik, R.F. (2000). A Multidimensional Framework for Synthesizing Disparate Issues in Identification, Selecting, and Serving Gifted Children. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 831-838). Oxford: Pergamon. 75. Sternberg, R.J. & Grigorenko, E.L. (2002). Dynamic Testing. New York: Cambridge University Press. 76. Sternberg, R.J. & Davidson, J.E. (Eds.). (2004). Conceptions of Giftedness (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. 77. Terrassier, J.-Ch. (1985). Dyssynchrony: Uneven Development. In J. Freeman (Ed.), The Psychology of Gifted Children (pp. 265-274). Chichester: John Wiley. 78. Trost, G. (2000). Prediction of Excellence in School, Higher Education, and Work. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 317-327). Oxford: Pergamon. 79. Webb, J.T., Meckstroth, E.A. & Tolan, S.S. (2002). Guiding the Gifted Child (3rd ed.). Ohio: Psychology Publ. 80. Urban, K.K. (1992). Begabungsfrderung im Vorschulalter [Gifted Education in Preschool Age]. In E.A. Hany & H. Nickel (Eds.), Begabung und Hochbegabung (pp. 159-169). Bern: Huber. 81. Waldmann, M.R. & Weinert, F.E. (1990). Intelligenz und Denken [Intelligence and Thinking]. Gttingen: Hogrefe. 82. Weinert, F.E. (1992). Wird man zum Hochbegabten geboren, entwickelt man sich dahin, oder wird man dazu gemacht? [Born as Highly Gifted, Developed, or Socialized?]. In E.A. Hany & H. Nickel (Eds.), Begabung und Hochbegabung (pp. 197-203). Bern: Huber. 83. Weinert, F.E. & Waldmann, M.R. (1985). Das Denken Hochbegabter Intellektuelle Fhigkeiten und kognitive Prozesse [Thought Processes of the Gifted Intellectual Capabilities and Cognitive Processes]. Zeitschrift fr Pdagogik, 31, 789-804. 84. Yewchuk, C. & Lupart, J. (2000). Inclusive Education for Gifted Students with Disabilities. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 659-670). Oxford: Pergamon. 85. Ziegler, A. & Perleth, Ch. (1997). Schafft es Sisyphos, den Stein den Berg hinaufzurollen? Eine kritische Bestandsaufnahme der Diagnose- und Frdermglichkeiten von Begabten in der beruflichen Bildung vor dem Hintergrund des Mnchner Begabungs-Prozess-Modells [Will Sisyphus Be Able to Roll the Stone Up the Mountain? A Critical Examination of the Status of Diagnosis and Promotion of the Gifted in Occupational Education Set Against the Munich Talent Model]. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 44, 152-163. 86. Ziegler, A, & Heller, A. (2000). Conceptions of Giftedness from a Meta-Theoretical Perspective. In K.A. Heller, F.J. Mnks, R.J. Sternberg & R.F. Subotnik (Eds.), International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed., pp. 3-21). Oxford: Pergamon. 87. Ziegler, A. & Stger, H. (2003). Identification of Underachievement: An Empirical Study on the Agreement Among Various Diagnostic Sources. Gifted and Talented International, 18, 87-94.

Identification basic issues

323

88. Ziegler, A., Dresel, M. & Schober, B. (2000). Underachievementdiagnose: Ein Modell zur Diagnose partieller Lernbeeintrchtigungen [Underachievement Analysis: A Model for the Diagnosis of Partial Learning Handicapped Students]. In K.A. Heller (Ed.), Begabungsdiagnostik in der Schul- und Erziehungsberatung (2nd ed., pp. 259-278). Bern: Huber.

You might also like