You are on page 1of 9

Language Learning and

Perfectionism: Anxious and


Non-Anxious Language Learners’
Reactions to Their Own Oral
Performance
TAMMY GREGERSEN ELAINE K. HORWITZ
Universidad de Atacama Foreign Language Education
Copayapu 485 SZB 528
Copiapó University of Texas at Austin
Chile Austin, TX 78712
Email: Tgregersen@educatio.uda.cl Email: Horwitz@mail.utexas.edu

This interview study sought to clarify the relationship between foreign language anxiety and
perfectionism. The comments of anxious and non-anxious language learners were audiore-
corded as they watched themselves interact in a videotaped oral interview. By examining the
reactions of the language learners to their actual oral performance and analyzing the audio-
tapes for instances of perfectionism, evidence was gathered suggesting that anxious and
non-anxious learners differ in their personal performance standards, procrastination, fear of
evaluation, and concern over errors. Because the results of this study indicated a link between
language anxiety and perfectionism, the article ends with a discussion of procedures that have
been used to overcome perfectionism and that may also be helpful to anxious foreign
language learners.

LANGUAGE TEACHERS AND RESEARCHERS fully or to understand what another person says
have been interested in the phenomenon of for- can easily lead to frustration and apprehension
eign language anxiety for a number of years. Hor- given that the apprehensive communicator is
witz, Horwitz, and Cope (1986) argued that for- aware that complete communication is not possi-
eign language anxiety is a specific syndrome that ble and may be troubled by this prospect.
may be related to three well-known anxieties asso- In the case of foreign or second language
ciated with first language use and everyday life. learning, fear of negative evaluation is likely to be
These are: communication apprehension, fear of manifested in a student’s overconcern with aca-
negative evaluation, and test anxiety. Communica- demic and personal evaluations of his or her per-
tion apprehension refers to an individual’s dis- formance and competence in the target language
comfort in talking in front of others. In the foreign (MacIntyre & Gardner, 1991). Although it is axi-
language context, Horwitz, Horwitz, and Cope omatic that language learning cannot occur with-
(1986) contended that the mismatch between for- out errors, errors can be the source of anxiety in
eign language students’ mature thoughts and some individuals because they draw attention to
their immature foreign or second language profi- the difficulty of making positive social impres-
ciency results in self-consciousness and anxiety in sions when speaking a new language (MacIntyre
some individuals. The inability to express oneself & Gardner, 1989). People who are highly con-
cerned about the impressions that others form of
The Modern Language Journal, 86, iv, (2002) them tend to behave in ways that minimize the
0026-7902/02/562–570 $1.50/0 possibility of unfavorable evaluations. Like com-
©2002 The Modern Language Journal
munication-anxious individuals, people who fear
Tammy Gregersen and Elaine K. Horwitz 563
negative evaluation rarely initiate conversation [2] motivation more from fear of failure than from
and interact minimally. Language students who pursuit of success;
experience this anxiety tend to sit passively in the [3] measurement of one’s own worth entirely in
terms of productivity and accomplishment;
classroom, withdraw from activities that could in-
[4] all-or-nothing evaluations that label anything
crease their language skills, and may even avoid other than perfection as failure;
class entirely (Ely, 1986; Gregersen, 1999/2000; [5] difficulty in taking credit or pleasure, even when
Horwitz, Horwitz, & Cope, 1986). success is achieved, because such achievement is
The construct of test anxiety also seems rele- merely what is expected;
vant to a discussion of the anxious foreign or [6] procrastination in getting started on work that
second language learner. Some learners may in- will be judged; and
appropriately view foreign or second language [7] long delays in completing assignments, or repeat-
edly starting over on assignments, because the work
production as a test situation rather than as an
must be perfect from the beginning and continue to
opportunity for communication (Horwitz, 1986).
be perfect as one goes along. (p. 1)
Although the constructs of communication ap-
prehension, fear of negative evaluation, and test Other symptoms commonly observed in per-
anxiety have proven useful in understanding the fectionist students include an unwillingness to
nature of foreign language anxiety, relatively little volunteer to respond to questions unless they are
is known about the relationships between and certain of the correct answer, overly emotional
among foreign language anxiety and other per- and “catastrophic” reactions to minor failures,
sonality characteristics. Understanding these rela- and low productivity due to procrastination or
tionships would help teachers better understand excessive “start overs.”
the experience of uncomfortable language learn- Although perfectionism is intuitively recog-
ers and point to better ways to help these students. nized by many teachers and helping profession-
This article will, therefore, examine the relation- als, psychologists have tended to consider it in
ship between foreign language anxiety and a logi- association with other psychological traits. Pacht
cally related personality construct, perfectionism. (1984) argued that perfectionism plays a promi-
Communication apprehension, fear of negative nent role in several forms of psychopathology,
evaluation, and test anxiety evoke an image of a and several theorists believe that it is a less exag-
language learner who is overly concerned with the gerated form of obsessive-compulsive disorder
“appearance” of his or her communication at- (OCD) (Mallinger, 1984; McFall & Wollersheim,
tempts. Like anxious foreign language learners, 1979; Pittman, 1987a, 1987b; Salzman, 1968).1
perfectionists set excessively high standards for Recently, however, several researchers have raised
performance accompanied by overly critical self- the possibility that perfectionism, like anxiety,
evaluations (Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, can itself be an important source of poor school
1990). With respect to language learning, perfec- performance. In his book, Teaching Problem Stu-
tionist students would not be satisfied with merely dents, Brophy (1996) argued:
communicating in their target language—they Perfectionists show unsatisfactory achievement pro-
would want to speak flawlessly, with no grammati- gress because they are more concerned about avoid-
cal or pronunciation errors, and as easily as a na- ing mistakes than about learning. They are inhibited
tive speaker. Rather than demonstrating less-than- about classroom participation and counterproduc-
perfect language skills and exposing themselves to tively compulsive in their work habits. (p. 112)
the possible negative reactions of others, perfec- Readers familiar with the literature on foreign
tionist language learners would likely prefer to re- language anxiety are likely to notice a number of
main silent, waiting until they were certain of how parallels from it to these descriptions of perfec-
to express their thoughts. Such impossibly high tionism. It is important to note that as for anxious
performance standards create the ideal condi- language learners, the success of perfectionists is
tions for the development of language anxiety. often impeded because they spend their energy
Based on Pacht’s (1984) conceptualization, avoiding mistakes rather than focusing on learn-
Brophy (1999) catalogued a number of symp- ing. Not only do they refrain from classroom par-
toms of perfectionism in students that seem to be ticipation, but they also indulge in compulsive
counterproductive to learning of any kind and behaviors that negatively influence their work
especially so for language learning: habits (Brophy, 1999). Brophy’s descriptions of
perfectionists evoke several items on the Foreign
[1] performance standards that are impossibly high Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (FLCAS; Hor-
and unnecessarily rigid; witz et al., 1986), an instrument used to identify
564 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
language-anxious students. Anxious foreign lan- classes at the Universidad de Atacama in Chile and
guage learners agree with statements such as, who were participating in a quantitative study ex-
“Even if I am well prepared for language class, I ploring the relationships between perfectionism
feel anxious about it.” “The more I study for a and foreign language anxiety. This sample con-
language test, the more confused I get.” “I get sisted of the 4 most anxious and the 4 least anxious
nervous when I don’t understand every word my participants in the larger study according to their
language teacher says.” “I always feel that the scores on the FLCAS. Table 1 displays the mean
other students speak the foreign language better FLCAS scores for the 78 participants in the quanti-
than I do.” Conversely, anxious foreign language tative study and the scores for the 8 individuals par-
students disagree with statements such as, “I don’t ticipating in the study reported here.
feel pressure to prepare very well for my language The 8 participants were second-year students in
class.” And “I don’t worry about making mistakes the English Education program at the Universi-
in language class”(Horwitz et al., pp. 129–130) dad de Atacama. They had completed at least 6
Brophy’s (1999) suggestion that perfectionist years of secondary English language studies be-
students set overly high performance standards fore entering the university the previous year, and
that are accompanied by a fear of failure is re- they were preparing to become high school En-
flected in MacIntyre and Gardner’s anxiety glish teachers. Of the 8 participants, 7 were fe-
model (1991): male and 1 was male (high-anxious student No.
3). This gender ratio of 1:8 was consistent with
Thus, foreign language anxiety is based on negative the general population of students in the English
expectations that lead to worry and emotionality.
Education program at the Universidad de Ata-
This leads to cognitive interference from self-deroga-
cama. In addition, 7 of the participants were of
tory cognition that produces performance deficits.
Poor performance and negative emotional reactions typical university age and 1 (low-anxious student
reinforce the expectations of anxiety and failure, fur- No. 3) was a returning adult student. After com-
ther anxiety being a reaction to this perceived threat. pleting the FLCAS, the 8 students were asked to
(p. 110) participate in the interview phase of the study,
and all agreed.
Although the preceding analogy between for-
eign language-anxious and perfectionist students
is appealing, no study has examined the connec- Procedures
tion between these two traits. This interview The interview study reported here consisted of
study, therefore, sought to clarify the interaction two phases. In the first phase, participants were
of language anxiety and perfectionism in a group videotaped in a one-on-one oral interview de-
of language learners. Specifically, we attempted signed to elicit a sample of their conversational
to identify instances of perfectionism in anxious English ability. The oral interviews, conducted by
language learners and confirm that such reac- the first author, lasted about 5 minutes during
tions are less prevalent in non-anxious learners. which the participants were asked to respond in
Thus, in order to examine the relationships be- English to common conversational prompts:
tween perfectionism and language anxiety, the
comments of anxious and non-anxious language
learners were audiorecorded as the students
watched themselves participate in a videotaped TABLE 1
oral interview, and the audiotapes were examined FLCAS Scores
for instances of perfectionism. By having the par-
Mean FLCAS Score 58.28
ticipants review their videotaped interviews, this Standard Deviation 14.81
study also examined the reactions of language
learners to their actual oral performance, a sec- High-Anxious Student No. 1 34
ond area that has never before been explored. High-Anxious Student No. 2 37
High-Anxious Student No. 3 41
High-Anxious Student No. 4 41
METHODOLOGY
Low-Anxious Student No. 1 101
Participants Low-Anxious Student No. 2 100
Low-Anxious Student No. 3 97
For this interview study, the researchers selected Low-Anxious Student No. 4 92
8 students from a larger group of 78 students who Note. Low scores represent high anxiety; high scores
were enrolled in second-year English language represent low anxiety.
Tammy Gregersen and Elaine K. Horwitz 565
1. Where are you from? needed prompting at the beginning, all of the
2. Tell me about your family. participants readily offered their reactions to the
3. How do you celebrate Independence Day? tapes. These sessions lasted about 10 minutes.
4. Where do you go and what do you do on The students’ reactions to their conversations
vacation? were audiorecorded and transcribed. Each tran-
5. How do you normally spend your weekends? scription was then analyzed independently by
three raters fluent in Spanish and familiar with
The videotaped conversations were used in the the literature on perfectionism who catalogued
second part of the study to elicit the students’ indications of perfectionist or nonperfectionist
possible feelings of perfectionism and anxiety as tendencies as defined by Brophy (1999). Specifi-
well as any other emotional reactions. (An effort cally, the raters were asked to look for student
was made to put participants at ease during both commentary and reactions reflecting personal
phases of the study.) For rating purposes, perfec- performance standards, procrastination, emo-
tionism was operationalized as comments reflect- tional responses to evaluation, and error-con-
ing high personal performance standards and sciousness. The raters were requested to excerpt
procrastination, fear of evaluation, and error- from the complete texts any quotations that they
consciousness. After all 8 students had completed perceived as corresponding to perfectionist or
the conversation task (a period of about 1 week), nonperfectionist tendencies. The transcripts
they were invited to review their videos with the were presented in random order, so that the rat-
first author, and again, all agreed. In this part of ers, although aware that this was an anxiety study,
the study, the participants watched their vide- would be blind to the anxiety status of the partici-
otaped interviews and were asked to reflect on pants. Only those quotations that appeared on
their own performances. The students were given two or more raters’ lists are included in the ensu-
the option of making comments while the video ing discussion. Table 2 categorizes and summa-
was playing, or of commenting at the end of the rizes the number of perfectionist comments iden-
video, and, with only one exception (low-anxious tified for each of the 8 participants. As can be
student No. 3), the interviewees opted to com- seen in the table, the number of perfectionist
ment after viewing the entire interview. (We note comments offered by the more anxious partici-
that several interviewees groaned or made other pants ranged from 7 to 11, whereas the low-anx-
noises while watching the videotape.) This phase ious students offered only one or two such com-
of the study was conducted in the students’ native ments. In addition, of the three comment
Spanish. When the videotape ended, the inter- categories, it is interesting to note that the large
viewer prompted the students with such ques- majority of comments were judged to be of the
tions as “What do you think of your perfor- personal performance standards and procrastina-
mance?” “Did you like it?” Thus, in order to elicit tion type.
potentially perfectionist and uncomfortable reac- In order to triangulate these results, the three
tions from them, the interviewer gave the partici- raters were also asked to put the transcripts in
pants a somewhat evaluative orientation to their rank order from the most perfectionist (1) to the
performance. Although some of the students least perfectionist (8). Table 3 compares the perfec-

TABLE 2
Frequency of Perfectionist Comments

Personal
Performance Fear of
Standards and Negative Concern
Students Procrastination Evaluation Over Errors Total
High-Anxious Student No. 1 5 2 2 9
High-Anxious Student No. 2 2 4 1 7
High-Anxious Student No. 3 6 3 2 11
High-Anxious Student No. 4 7 2 2 11

Low-Anxious Student No. 1 0 0 1 1


Low-Anxious Student No. 2 0 0 1 1
Low-Anxious Student No. 3 2 0 0 2
Low-Anxious Student No. 4 1 0 0 1
566 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
tionism rank order with the rank order of anxiety emplified how procrastination and low productiv-
taken from the FLCAS scores. Raters agreed on ity result from unrealistic personal standards,
the rankings 92% of the time. “Because I am very slow in doing things, I begin
to get nervous about it, and I start to look at all
the details. And then I begin to waste a lot of
STUDENT REACTIONS TO THEIR ORAL time.” Thus, not only did these anxious learners
PERFORMANCE report avoidance and procrastination in their lan-
Personal Performance Standards and Procrastination guage learning, but they actually seemed to avoid
talking about their performance on the tape.
Unusually high personal standards and pro- The impossibly high and unnecessarily rigid
crastination are hallmarks of perfectionism (Bro- performance standards described by Brophy
phy, 1999). Perfectionist students often demon- (1999) are exemplified in the following excerpt
strate long delays in completing assignments or from high-anxious student No. 2 who com-
repeatedly restart them because they believe that mented directly on her tape. (Even though this
their work must be perfect from beginning to student had high language proficiency, she was
end. Thus, low productivity is strongly associated also highly anxious.) She complained, “I have
with perfectionism, and for that reason, the two some problems with verbs, and I still have to
issues will be discussed together here. improve some things concerning vocabulary. I
The anxious students in this study offered a believe that if I work harder I could meet my
number of comments that are consistent with the speaking goals. But what happens is that I often
high-standard–low-productivity association de- forget what some things mean.” She repeated
scribed by Brophy. Strikingly, unlike the less-anx- several times, “I believe that if I study a little more
ious students whose comments will follow, the . . . I believe that if I study a little more . . . ” Thus,
anxious participants in this study were clearly re- even though her language proficiency level was
luctant to comment directly on their oral per- particularly high for a second-year student and
formance and instead quickly turned the discus- clearly high for these 8 participants, she was not
sions of their videotapes to more general personally satisfied with her performance.
discussions of how they often put off assignments The non-anxious language students, however,
and other language-related tasks. For example, were happy to discuss their performances on the
high-anxious student No. 1 ignored her perfor- videotapes and readily described personal stan-
mance on the tape almost entirely and began to dards that were more realistic for their levels of
discuss when she should take a particular class, language ability. The non-anxious participants
saying, “If I am not going to do it [the class] well, recognized that their language production was
it would be better for me to wait until next year imperfect but did not demand the same level of
and do it better.” Later, with reference to a paper accuracy that their language anxious counter-
she was not satisfied with, she commented, “Why parts did. In fact, they often seemed proud of
should I turn in something bad if I could have their performance and aware of their own re-
done it well? It’s better not to do it then. I always laxed state. Low-anxious student No. 3 (the re-
do this, and then I get frustrated, and I end up turning adult student) whose oral performance
staying that way.” High-anxious student No. 4 ex- was particularly poor stood out in contrast to the

TABLE 3
Rank Orders for Anxiety and Perfectionism

Anxiety Perfectionism (Based


Student (Based on FLCAS Scores) on Mean Rater Rankings)
High-Anxious Student No. 1 1 1
High-Anxious Student No. 2 2 3
High-Anxious Student No. 3 3 2
High-Anxious Student No. 4 3 4

Low-Anxious Student No. 1 5 5


Low-Anxious Student No. 2 6 6
Low-Anxious Student No. 3 7 8
Low-Anxious Student No. 4 8 7
Tammy Gregersen and Elaine K. Horwitz 567
high-proficiency but anxious student described nounce perfectly. For example, I have a classmate
above. This non-anxious student commented, “I who is very calm when he speaks. He gets mixed up
said less than I would have liked to. It [my En- sometimes, but he untangles himself quickly. But not
glish] was not very fluent yet, nevertheless, I me. I get mixed up and then I get even more mixed
up. I get into even deeper trouble.
didn’t feel at all inhibited. Although I don’t like
cameras, it’s fun to see myself now on video.” In addition to worrying about how others per-
Low-anxious student No. 4 stated, “I started get- ceive him, this participant recognized that other
ting involved in the topic. Sure, I had some gaps learners may also have difficulty speaking English
when I tried to think of the right word in English but still remain calm.
and respond, but in the end, I felt relaxed.” This High-anxious student No. 2, explaining the
sentiment was also reflected in the comments of anxiety she felt in large groups, commented, “I
a third non-anxious student, “I wanted to say believe that everyone gets nervous when they
more . . . It was like I had difficulty in finding the have to confront a group and you have to show
words, and this slowed me down, but it wasn’t what you know. This makes me nervous. In reality,
stressful.” It is interesting that this participant a big group is what makes me really nervous.”
recognized limitations in her language produc- Whereas all of the anxious participants com-
tion but did not find it stressful when she had mented about perceived evaluation by peers or
difficulty expressing herself. Rather, she viewed conversational partners when speaking English,
her difficulty in finding words as an impediment not a single non-anxious participant did so. It
to her speed of production. appears that perceived evaluation by others is a
These comments suggest that although the feeling that clearly distinguishes anxious and
non-anxious students recognized weaknesses in non-anxious foreign language learners, and the
their language skills, they set realistic personal possibility of looking foolish is an area of great
standards and seemed pleased when considering concern to the anxious language learner.
their own performance in English. Unlike the
anxious students, not only did they comment
cheerfully on their performance but they did not
Concern over Errors
report either procrastination or work avoidance.
There was not a single reference to either pro- The perfectionist tendency to avoid and over-
crastination or avoidance of English tasks in the react to errors appeared prominently in the com-
transcripts of the non-anxious learners. The non- ments of the anxious participants. The 4 anxious
anxious learners also seemed to value being re- students not only noticed errors but lamented
laxed and gave themselves credit for not getting them. Anxious participants offered comments
anxious. In fact, unlike the anxious students who such as, “I made so many mistakes talking, gram-
recognized that becoming anxious interfered matically, everything . . . I made a lot of mistakes.
with their performance, these learners seemed to They make me nervous” (from the male partici-
view their lack of anxiety as a kind of success. pant); “I have problems with verbs, and I have yet
to improve my vocabulary”; and a simple, “Oh, I
Fear of Evaluation made so many mistakes!”
By contrast, only 2 of the 4 non-anxious inter-
Consistent with conceptualizations of both lan- viewees commented at all about their errors and,
guage anxiety and perfectionism, the high-anx- in stark contrast to their language-anxious coun-
ious participants in this study tended to fear the terparts, did not seem bothered by them. Low-
evaluation of their peers and the subsequent pos-
anxious student No. 1 commented, “I had some
sibility of appearing foolish. All 4 language-anx-
grammatical errors, but small ones. I was fine. I
ious interviewees in this study commented nega-
am quite fluent and spontaneous. It wasn’t diffi-
tively about their errors and compared
cult to express myself.” A second participant, low-
themselves negatively with their peers. High-anx-
anxious student No. 2, offered:
ious student No. 3 (the male) worried:
I am bothered a little [about my errors] because I get It [the interview] seems very good to me. I was very
nervous, and I think that the other person thinks that calm and didn’t feel any pressure. I made some mis-
I don’t know how to speak. It happens a lot. I try to takes, but not that many . . . there weren’t really too
pronounce the best I can, and when I try to pro- many. When you are talking, you don’t notice the
nounce better, my pronunciation gets worse, because mistakes, or the mistake is immediately corrected,
I get flustered. That is, I get flustered because I some- and now I have the opportunity to see myself on
times pronounce words badly. I try so hard to pro- video, I notice this.
568 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
DISCUSSION In considering these findings, it is important to
note that anxiety and perfectionism can do more
The reactions of the students to their own oral than make language learning unpleasant. These
performance indicate that anxious and non-anx- findings suggest one possible contribution to the
ious foreign language learners do differ in terms lower foreign language achievement levels found
of their self-reports of perfectionist tendencies. for anxious learners (Horwitz et al.,1986; MacIn-
Specifically, anxious learners reported higher tyre & Gardner, 1991). Frost, Turcotte, Heim-
standards for their English performance, a greater berg, Mattia, Holt, and Hope (1995) found that
tendency toward procrastination, greater worry participants who were highly concerned about
over the opinions of others, and a higher level of their mistakes reported more negative affect,
concern over their errors than the non-anxious lower self-confidence, and a greater feeling that
learners. These findings indicate that anxious lan- they should have performed better on the experi-
guage learners and perfectionists may have a mental task (which elicited frequent mistakes)
number of characteristics in common and that than less-perfectionist students. When compared
these characteristics have the potential for making with students who were not as concerned about
language learning unpleasant as well as less suc- making mistakes, the perfectionist students re-
cessful for them than for other students. The set- ported greater distress regarding their mistakes
ting of standards is a necessary step in accomplish- and rated their mistakes as more important. They
ing learning goals; however, the reactions of the also lamented their mistakes to a greater degree
anxious students to their oral performances dem- and reported greater concern over the negative
onstrated that they were never satisfied with what reactions of others and a greater desire to keep
they accomplished. The non-anxious students, their mistakes a secret. Such an array of negative
even though they also set personal standards, al- affective reactions likely contributed to the lower
lowed themselves to celebrate small victories. achievement levels of anxious language learners
In one way, however, the anxious and non-anx- reported in several studies.
ious learners were similar. Both sets of students
were able to recognize their errors during the
CONCLUSIONS AND LIMITATIONS
oral interviews, but the anxious and non-anxious
students had vastly different emotional responses The results of this study indicate that language
to similar errors. (The majority of errors made by anxiety and perfectionism can have similar mani-
all participants during the oral interviews were festations in anxious language learners, a finding
verb tense and preposition errors.) The anxious that suggests that procedures that have been used
learners were disturbed by their mistakes, to help individuals overcome perfectionism may
whereas the non-anxious students took them in also be useful in helping anxious foreign or sec-
stride. It may be that anxious and non-anxious ond language learners. It also appears that anxi-
students are equally aware of imperfect perfor- ety and the actual language proficiency levels
mance but differ in their reactions to imperfec- demonstrated by the students during the inter-
tion. It is interesting to note that the anxious views were not highly related in these learners. All
participants often attributed their errors to their of the participants were at the same course level,
anxiety, an excuse never offered by the non-anx- all had been successful high school language
ious learners, who often seemed pleased with learners, and all felt that they had the potential
their own lack of anxiety. Consistent with the to become English teachers. Yet, 4 of the partici-
findings of MacIntyre, Noels, and Clément pants were highly anxious, and 4 reported little if
(1997), the anxious learners in this study tended any anxiety. It appears, rather, that anxious and
to overestimate the number and seriousness of non-anxious learners differ in terms of their reac-
their errors whereas the non-anxious students tions to their performance. Thus, awareness of
tended toward underestimation. In addition, the their limitations in the target language does not
anxious learners consistently linked their mis- appear to be a cause of anxiety in all individuals.
takes to the possibility of negative evaluations by Both groups of learners recognized the limita-
others. Clearly, perceptions of evaluation were an tions in their language production but had vastly
important area of difference between the two different responses to these limitations. Finally,
groups. The anxious participants viewed their we suggest that having students watch their re-
performance as being constantly evaluated by corded oral performance—sometimes referred
teachers and peers whereas the non-anxious not to as stimulated recall—is useful in the study of
only relied on self-evaluation but generally evalu- affective reactions to language learning.
ated themselves positively. Several limitations to this study must be noted.
Tammy Gregersen and Elaine K. Horwitz 569
Only a small group of learners in a specific lan- instead of facts. When an individual’s underlying
guage learning context was examined, which perfectionist beliefs are restated as suggestions,
does not make generalizability of the findings the individual is often better able to consider a cur-
possible. It is entirely possible that other lan- rent situation in conjunction with other evidence,
guage learners in the same or different learning such as past experiences and the opinions of oth-
situation would have different reactions. Yet, ers, in order to modify questionable beliefs
there were great similarities among the learners (Ramirez, 1999). Arthur and Hayward (1997) be-
in each group, and the comments reported here lieve that many students who maintain perfection-
are familiar to experienced language teachers. ist standards about the expectations of others have
We must also consider that perfectionism plays a not actually discussed those expectations with the
greater role in anxiety in students at this level people involved. Students may need help in over-
than in less advanced learners. In addition, per- coming their hesitation to discuss performance
fect language performance may be of greater expectations with the individuals whose opinions
concern to people who plan to be language they value (family, friends, etc.).
teachers than to more typical language learners Language teachers may themselves have per-
(Horwitz, 1996). The relationship of perfection- fectionist tendencies and inadvertently encour-
ism and language anxiety should, therefore, be age or develop these tendencies in their students.
examined in a variety of learning groups at vari- Indeed, Brophy (1999) found that nagging or
ous stages of language learning with various criticizing perfectionist students or giving them
learning goals. Finally and most important, it additional time to complete assignments only en-
must be noted that this study began with the couraged more perfectionism. He suggested in-
premise that anxious language learners would stead that teachers try the following:
show indications of perfectionism, and it was
therefore designed to detect instances of this [1] building a friendly, supportive learning environ-
trait. Anxious and non-anxious language learners ment;
[2] establishing the expectation that mistakes are a
likely differ in many important ways other than
normal part of the learning process;
the characteristics examined here. Future studies [3] presenting themselves as helpful instructors con-
should address the relationship of anxiety with cerned primarily with promoting student learning,
other personal traits as well as its relationship to rather than as authority figures concerned primarily
students’ ultimate levels of foreign language with evaluating student performance;
achievement. [4] articulating expectations that stress learning and
improvement over perfect performance of assign-
ments;
PEDAGOGICAL IMPLICATIONS [5] explaining how perfectionism is counterproduc-
tive;
Given that language anxiety may stem from [6] reassuring perfectionist students that they will get
perfectionist tendencies in some students, ap- the help they need to achieve success;
proaches used to help perfectionist learners may [7] following through with help, and communicating
also benefit anxious language learners. Ramirez teacher approval of students’ progress and accom-
(1999) argued that perfectionism is the result of plishments. (p. 2)
a set of unrealistic self-beliefs. Most important,
perfectionists believe that some personally-valued Above all, it is important to remember that per-
goal will be achieved when they are perfect. fectionist students need help. According to Bro-
phy (1996), teachers may tend to ignore perfec-
For example, “If I do it perfectly, then . . . [I] will tionist students because they do good work and
finally be accepted . . . [I] can finally stop worrying do not cause trouble.
. . . I will get what I have been working toward . . . I Finally, we offer two suggestions based on our
can finally relax.” The flip side of this schema, also findings in this study. Both the anxious and the
subscribed to by perfectionists, is that “If I make a
non-anxious participants recognized the value of
mistake,” there will be a catastrophic outcome (“I will
remaining calm while participating in the oral
be humiliated . . . I am a failure . . . I am stupid . . . I
am worthless”). (p. 33) interview. Thus, we suggest that all learners be
reminded of the value of controlling their emo-
In order to overcome these self-defeating tional state when speaking the target language.
thoughts, Ramirez suggested that people identify Horwitz (1990) recommended that anxious stu-
the misconceptions in such beliefs and work to de- dents visualize themselves relaxing when they
velop more realistic expectations. Perfectionists make mistakes in the target language. This prac-
must learn to treat their self-beliefs as hypotheses tice would seem to be particularly beneficial for
570 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
perfectionist students who react so strongly to Ely, C. M. (1986). An analysis of discomfort, risktaking,
mistakes. It also appears that the non-anxious stu- sociability, and motivation in the L2 classroom.
dents value continuing to talk even if they make Language Learning, 36, 1–25.
Frost., R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R.
mistakes. Anxious students could be taught to fo-
(1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cogni-
cus on continuing a conversation (or oral dis-
tive Therapy and Research, 14, 449–468.
course) as a goal in itself whenever they make Frost, R. O., Turcotte, T. A., Heimberg, R. G., Mattia, J. I.,
mistakes. Thus, as an antidote to their overcon- Holt, C. S., & Hope, D. A. (1995). Reactions to mis-
cern with errors, they should be told that continu- takes among participants high and low in perfec-
ation should be given precedence over errors. tionist concern over mistakes. Cognitive Therapy and
Ultimately, perfectionist students need to un- Research, 19, 195–205.
derstand that the classroom is not merely for dem- Gregersen, T. G. (1999/2000). Improving teaching
onstrating knowledge and skill, but also for gain- methodologies for communicatively apprehensive
ing it, and that errors are a normal and acceptable foreign language students. Lenguas Modernas,
26–27, 119–133.
part of everyone’s language learning experience.
Horwitz, E. K. (1986). Preliminary evidence for the reli-
ability and validity of a Foreign Language Anxiety
Scale. TESOL Quarterly, 20, 559–562.
NOTES Horwitz, E. K. (1990). Attending to the affective domain
in the foreign language classroom. In S. Magnan
1 Pittman (1987b), for example, proposes a cy- (Ed.), 1990 Reports of the Northeast Conference of For-
bernetic model of Obsessive Compulsive Disor- eign Language Teachers (pp. 15–31). Lincolnwood,
IL: National Textbook Company.
der utilizing a control system analogy. It is be-
Horwitz, E. K. (1996). Even teachers get the blues: Rec-
lieved that individuals constantly compare a ognizing and alleviating language teachers’ feel-
signal event (e.g., behavior) with a preset crite- ings of foreign language anxiety. Foreign Language
rion (e.g., standard of performance). The differ- Annals, 29, 365–372.
ence between the perceptual signal and the crite- Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M. B., & Cope, J. (1986). For-
rion is referred to as the “error signal.” A nonzero eign language classroom anxiety. Modern Language
error signal activates a systemic response to bring Journal, 70, 125–132.
the signal and the criterion into a matched MacIntyre, P. D., & Gardner, R. C. (1989). Anxiety and
state—in other words, to make the behavior second-language learning: Toward a theoretical
match the desired standard. Behavior that does clarification. Language Learning, 39, 251–275.
MacIntyre, P. D., & Gardner, R. C. (1991). Methods and
not match the preset criterion is adjusted and
results in the study of anxiety in language learn-
repeated. Perfectionist individuals, according to ing: A review of the literature. Language Learning,
this model, have an excess of control and thus 41, 85–117.
tolerate little or no mismatch between the signal MacIntyre, P. D., Noels, K. A., & Clément, R. (1997).
and the criterion. Thus, their error signals are Biases in self-ratings of second language profi-
nearly always in a nonzero state, and the perfec- ciency: The role of language anxiety. Language
tionist must constantly attempt to improve per- Learning, 47, 265–287.
formance in order to match his or her overly rigid Mallinger, A. E. (1984). The obsessive’s myth of control.
standard. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 12,
147–165.
McFall, M. E., & Wollersheim, J. P. (1979). Obsessive-
compulsive neurosis: A cognitive-behavioral for-
REFERENCES
mulation and approach to treatment. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 3, 333–348.
Arthur, N., & Hayward, L. (1997). The relationships Pacht, A. R. (1984). Reflections on perfectionism. Ameri-
between perfectionism, standards for academic can Psychologist, 39, 386–390.
achievement, and emotional distress in post-sec- Pittman, R. K. (1987a). Pierre Janet on obsessive-com-
ondary students. Journal of College Student Develop- pulsive disorder (1903): Review and commentary.
ment, 38, 622–630. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 226–232.
Brophy, J. (1996). Teaching problem students. New York: Pittman, R. K. (1987b). A cybernetic model of obsessive-
Guilford Press. compulsive psychopathology. Comprehensive Psy-
Brophy, J. (1999). Working with perfectionist students. (Re- chiatry, 28, 334–343.
port No. 4). Urbana, IL: ERIC Clearinghouse on Ramirez, M. (1999). The perfect trap. Psychology Today,
Elementary and Early Childhood Education. 32, 30–34.
(ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED Salzman, L. (1968). The obsessive personality. New York:
400124) Jason Aronson.

You might also like